Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2016 Dec;5(4):384–394. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.013

How Common are WM Deficits in Children with Difficulties in Reading and Mathematics?

Susan E Gathercole 1,, Francesca Woolgar 1; the CALM Team1, Rogier A Kievit 1, Duncan Astle 1, Tom Manly 1, Joni Holmes 1
PMCID: PMC5198752  PMID: 28066703

Highlights

  • Children's learning abilities were associated with WM.

  • The link with verbal WM was stronger for reading than mathematics.

  • The majority of children with learning difficulties had WM deficits.

Keywords: Working memory, Reading, Language, Mathematics, Learning

Abstract

The extent to which deficits in working memory (WM) are characteristic of children with reading and mathematics difficulties was investigated in a large sample aged 5–15 years reported to have problems in attention, learning and memory. WM performance was highly correlated with reading and mathematics scores. Although deficits in individual tests of short-term memory (STM) and WM occurred in less than half of the children with detected learning difficulties, three-quarters of the children with low reading and mathematics scores obtained one or more WM scores in the deficit range. These findings are consistent with proposals that WM or the broader cognitive dimensions it taps impede school-based learning, and point to the importance of managing WM loads in the classroom.


Working memory (WM) is the cognitive system that supports the temporary maintenance and manipulation of information necessary for many demanding cognitive activities. Alternative models differ among many dimensions including the extent to which the system is domain-specific or domain-general (e.g., Baddeley and Hitch, 1974, Hambrick et al., 2004), the roles played by attention and inhibitory processes (Cowan, 1999, Engle and Kane, 2004, Kane et al., 2007, Wilhelm et al., 2013), and linkage with longer-term memory systems (e.g., Baddeley, 2000, Unsworth and Engle, 2007). There is however a common assumption that WM is subject to stable capacity limitations that vary widely between individuals across the lifespan. Indeed, the study of individual differences within WM has proved to be one of the most powerful tools for exploring and understanding its structure e.g. (Alloway et al., 2006, Bayliss et al., 2003, Daneman and Carpenter, 1980, Kane et al., 2004, Miyake et al., 2000).

Variations in children's WM capacities are highly related to progress across many areas of the curriculum including reading, mathematics and science (Gathercole et al., 2004, Jarvis and Gathercole, 2003). Low levels of WM performance are also widely reported in groups of children with difficulties in these areas of academic learning (e.g., Archibald and Gathercole, 2006a, Siegel and Ryan, 1989, Swanson, 1994). These findings have led to speculation that poor WM skills may contribute directly to problems in reading and mathematics. In the present study we investigate the consistency with which WM deficits are present in individual children in a large sample of students receiving support from education and health services for problems related to educational progress. The data are critical both for characterizing the symptom profiles in learning-disabled populations and understanding how low WM capacities may disrupt learning.

In children with reading difficulties and those with a more specific diagnosis of dyslexia, problems are most marked on verbal tasks that tap simple storage capacity (short-term memory or STM tasks) and more complex (WM) tasks with significant verbal processing demands in addition to storage (Kudo et al., 2015, Swanson et al., 2009). A similar profile of verbal memory deficits is evident in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI, e.g., Archibald and Gathercole, 2006b, Archibald and Gathercole, 2007, Hesketh and Conti-Ramsden, 2013, Montgomery and Evans, 2009, Montgomery, 2000, Newbury et al., 2005, Ramus et al., 2013, Schuchardt et al., 2013). This is a disorder characterized by poor language development in the absence of other sensory or intellectual deficits. For individuals with mathematical difficulties, broader deficits are often reported that extend across verbal and visuo-spatial aspects of both STM and WM (Swanson and Sachse-Lee, 2001, Van den Bos et al., 2013). Some recent studies of children with selective maths problems suggest that their WM difficulties are primarily restricted to the visuo-spatial domain (Swanson et al., 2009, Szucs et al., 2013). The mixed profile reported in many other studies may therefore reflect the high levels of comorbidity between problems in reading and mathematics.

This evidence has led to speculation that poor WM skills may contribute directly to the difficulties experienced by some children in academic learning. WM failures may lead to the loss of task-critical information across multiple learning episodes, thereby impairing the rate of educational progress (Gathercole, Lamont, & Alloway, 2006). Weak storage in verbal STM also impair the learning of novel phonological representations of new words (Baddeley et al., 1998, Gathercole, 2006a, Gathercole, 2006b) and compromise complex aspects of language comprehension such as inference and anaphoric reference that rely on the storage of multiple segments for off-line syntactic analysis (Cain et al., 2004, Pimperton and Nation, 2012). In mathematical abilities and development, WM has been proposed to provide support for simple strategies such as verbal counting (Geary et al., 2004, Krajewski and Schneider, 2009), the direct retrieval of mathematical facts (Barrouillet & Lépine, 2005), selecting and switching between appropriate solution strategies (Bull and Scerif, 2001, DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004), the co-ordination of multiple steps of complex mathematical problems (Imbo et al., 2007a, Imbo et al., 2007b), the maintenance of interim calculations during mental arithmetic (Adams & Hitch, 1997) and other aspects of verbal problem-solving (Swanson & Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004).

Causal explanations such as these are by no means universally accepted. In the field of research on reading and language development, it has been argued that close associations between deficits in WM and in learning are simply common consequences of impairments in broader representational dimensions. Poor verbal STM and WM in children with language-related difficulties has been suggested to be a consequence of core impairments in either phonological processing or phonological representations that also impede mastery of the orthographic system (Hulme and Snowling, 2009, McDougall et al., 1994, Metsala, 1999, Ramus et al., 2013).

The present study provided the opportunity to investigate the consistency with which deficits in WM are present in children experiencing academic learning difficulties. Data were collected from a sample of over 200 children referred to a research clinic by professionals in the fields of education and health on the basis of problems in attention, learning and/or memory. Tests of verbal and visuo-spatial WM, vocabulary, nonverbal reasoning, reading, and mathematics were administered to each child. Although most of the children scored below age-typical levels on assessments of reading and mathematics at the clinic visit, a substantial minority did not. This degree of variability in the learning scores made it possible to examine the frequency of low WM test scores in children with learning scores in the age-typical range as well as in the struggling learners.

The co-occurrence of deficits in WM and learning was investigated in two ways. First, associations between WM and learning scores were examined across the sample as a whole. In line with previous research, significant associations were predicted for both reading and mathematics, with closer links expected between reading abilities and verbal than visuo-spatial aspects of WM. As a consequence of the high levels of co-morbidity between reading and mathematical difficulties (Vukovic, Lesaux, & Siegel, 2010) both verbal and visuo-spatial WM scores were expected to correlate with mathematics scores.

Second, we examined whether low scores on tests of WM could identify children with poor reading and mathematics performance and distinguish them from children with age-typical levels of attainment. To the best of our knowledge, use of individual WM scores to classify children according to learning abilities has been restricted to date to children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Two measures of verbal STM, sentence repetition and nonword repetition, are reliable discriminators of children who do and do not have a clinical diagnosis of SLI or a cognitive profile consistent with the diagnosis (Archibald and Joanisse, 2009, Dollaghan and Campbell, 1998, Hesketh and Conti-Ramsden, 2013). Bishop et al. have suggested that nonword repetition deficits are a phenotypic marker of SLI with a strong hereditary component (Bishop and Snowling, 2004, Bishop et al., 1996). The association may extend beyond verbal STM to verbal WM: in a sample of 14 children with SLI, every child had very low verbal WM scores (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006a).

The consistency of WM deficits in struggling learners is a critical test of theories ascribing a direct causal role to WM in learning to read and becoming mathematically competent. WM deficits that are highly consistent in children with reading and mathematics impairments but not in those without these problems would be entirely consistent with theories attributing a causal role to WM limitations e.g., (Gathercole et al., 2006, Swanson, 1993, Swanson et al., 2009). Conversely, findings that WM scores in the deficit range are not highly characteristic of children with learning difficulties would challenge claims that WM problems are either a necessary or sufficient cause of these problems. Finally, comparable numbers of children with WM deficits with and without detectable learning problems would rule out any simple explanation that WM capacity limitations directly and inevitably impair learning.

Method

Participants

Children aged 5–15 years were recruited via recommendations to parents/carers from health or educational professionals supporting the learning needs of the children. Families interested in the study returned a form to the Cambridge Centre for Attention, Learning and Memory (CALM) indicating their willingness to participate. Centre staff contacted the referrer to discuss the nature of the difficulties for which the child was receiving support and information regarding formal diagnoses of any medical, psychological or psychiatric conditions. If the verbal report met the inclusionary criteria of probable problems of attention, learning, and/or memory, the child and accompanying carer (typically, parent or other family member) were invited to a 2.5-hour assessment session at the CALM clinic. Children were tested individually on a wide set of standardized cognitive tests, and at the time of the visit carers were asked to complete questionnaires relating to the child's behaviour and their developmental and medical history. A report of the outcomes of the principal assessments was provided to the referrer to inform their continuing support for the child. The study was approved by the Local NHS Research Ethics Committee reference 13/EE/0157.

The participants for this study were the first 230 children (154 boys, 76 girls, mean age 9y 4m, range 5y 5m to 15y 11m) to attend the CALM clinic between October 2014 and November 2015. The majority of children lived within a 25-mile radius of Cambridge, England. Recruitment to the clinic was achieved through local partners in education (specialist needs coordinators, specialist teachers and educational psychologists) and children's health services (speech and language therapy services, Child and Adolescent Mental Health services, and community paediatrics).

Measures

Working Memory

Four subtests of the Automated Working Memory Assessment (Alloway, 2007) were administered. Standard scores (population mean 100, SD = 15) were calculated for each subtest.

Digit Span

This test of verbal STM involves immediate serial spoken recall of sequences of spoken digits. Test–retest reliability is .89.

Backward Digit Span

This verbal WM test involved the serial recall the digits in reverse sequence. Test–retest reliability is .86.

Dot Matrix

This test of visuo-spatial STM involves serial recall of the locations of successive dots that appeared in an otherwise empty grid a single dot appeared in sequence in an otherwise empty blank matrix. Test–retest reliability is .85.

Mr X

In this test of visuo-spatial WM, sequences of pairs of cartoon characters holding a ball in each hand were presented. The task was to judge whether the two characters were holding the ball in the same hand or not. The character on the right was presented in one of six possible positions in each display, where the ball is held at one of six possible compass locations. At the end of the sequence the task was to recall the locations corresponding to the position of the ball held by the character on the right in each successive display. Test–retest reliability is .84.

Learning

Reading

The Single Word Reading subtest of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test II (Wechsler, 2005) was administered. Children read aloud single words that increased in length and complexity as quickly and as accurately as possible. This test was completed by all but one child. Test–retest reliability ranges from .85 to.98 for this age range.

Mathematics

The first 66 children tested in the sample completed the Maths Fluency subtest of the Woodcock Johnson II (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). This involved completion of as many written calculations as possible in 3 min. Test–retest mean is r = .96. Subsequent participants completed the Number Operations subtest of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test II (Wechsler, 1999, Wechsler, 2005), an untimed test that involved completion of written mathematical problems that become progressively more complex and varied drawing on knowledge of fractions, decimals and algebra. Test–retest is greater than .90 for this age range.

General Abilities

Vocabulary

The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 2009) is a measure of receptive vocabulary. It involves the child matching the word spoken by the tester to one of four pictures. Two children did not complete this test. Test–retest reliability is .91 for this age range.

Nonverbal Reasoning

The Matrix Reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999) is a test of nonverbal reasoning corresponding to fluid intelligence. Raw scores were converted into T-scores (population mean = 50, SD = 10). All children completed this assessment. The average test–retest reliability for this age range is .85.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Standard scores and T-scores were used for the purpose of analysis due to the wide age range of the sample. Mahalanobis distance values were computed with the aim of identifying outlier for the following variable set: the 4 WM tests, the reading test, a single mathematics measure derived from the particular test (WIAT or WJ) completed by each child, the 2 measures of general abilities, and for scores on the inattentivity and hyperactivity/impulsivity subscales of the short version of the Conners Parent Rating Scale 3rd Edition (Conners, 2008), data from which are not reported here. Twenty children with Mahalanobis scores exceeding 16.919 (χ2 value for p = .05) were excluded, yielding a sample size for analysis of 210. Finally, the multiple imputation option of SPSS v22, which uses the MCMC fully conditional implementation algorithm, was applied to impute missing data. Data were imputed for 2 vocabulary scores, 1 reading score and 1 Mr X score. Missing data could not be imputed for the two different mathematics tests for which there were three missing data values, due to the lower numbers of children completing each test (61 for the Woodcock–Johnson test and 146 for the WIAT).

Table 1 summarizes the information provided for participants at the time of recruitment. In total, 75% of the children were referred via professionals in education services, and the remaining 25% through health service professionals. At least one diagnosed developmental disorders prior to the clinic referral was recorded for 27% of the children, most commonly for ADHD (n = 15) and dyslexia (n = 14). Referring agents were asked to identify the primary reason for recommending that the child should attend the CALM clinic. As shown in Table 1, poor academic performance including problems in either literacy or mathematics or both was the reason given for 66% of the children.

Table 1.

Referral Sources and Diagnoses

Category M F Total
Referrer
 SENCo 92 48 140
 Specialist teacher 8 4 12
 Educational psychologist 3 1 4
 Speech and language therapist 12 9 21
 Clinical psychologist 9 6 15
 Paediatrician 13 3 16



Diagnosis
 None 95 51 146
 ADD 2 2 4
 ADHD 10 2 12
 DAMP 1 1 2
 Dyslexia 6 4 10
 Dyslexia, dysgraphia 1 0 1
 Dyspraxia 2 3 5
 Dyspraxia and dyslexia 2 0 2
 FASD 1 2 3
 Generalized developmental delay 1 1 2
 Global delay and dyspraxia 1 0 1
 Social anxiety disorder and depression 0 1 1
 Autism/aspergers 6 0 6
 Autism/aspergers and dyslexia 1 0 1
 ADHD and tourettes 1 0 1
 Autism/aspergers and DAMP 1 0 1
 ASD & ADHD 1 1 2



Primary reason for referral 37 17 54
 Attention 26 11 37
 Memory 16 7 23
 Literacy 28 7 35
 Maths 6 2 8
 Language 14 7 21
 Poor general academic progress 42 34 76

Descriptive statistics for the sample on the principal measures from the study are provided in Table 2. Reading and mathematics standard scores were, on average, low (84 for reading, 82 for WIAT Number Operations and 77 for WJ Maths Fluency). For both the reading and WIAT mathematics tests the range of scores was very wide, extending to the above-average range. Nonverbal reasoning abilities were in the low average range (T-score = 43), as were all measures of WM except the Mr X test of visuo-spatial WM which was within the age-typical range (97). Vocabulary scores were close to the population mean (97). Skewness and kurtosis values for all measures fell within acceptable ranges for statistical analyses assuming normality of distributions. Density plots smoothed with a Gaussian smoothing kernel created using ggplot (Wickham, 2009) in R (2015) with a bandwidth adjustment parameter of 3 are shown for the learning and general ability measures in Figure 1, and for the WM measures in Figure 2.

Table 2.

Sample Descriptive Statistics

Measure N Min. Max. Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Age in months 210 65 182 112.857 25.658 0.508 −0.283
Matrix reasoning 210 20 67 43.481 9.341 0.039 −0.482
Vocabulary 210 66 130 97.010 14.034 0.089 −0.548
Reading 210 44 130 83.998 16.467 0.080 −0.426
WIAT maths 146 42 118 81.890 13.774 0.135 0.081
WJ maths 61 56 102 77.213 10.758 0.313 −0.313
Digit span 210 61 139 92.324 14.434 0.081 −0.293
Backward digit span 210 58 135 91.452 11.557 0.445 1.345
Dot matrix 210 62 135 93.919 14.162 0.162 −0.366
Mr X 210 62 137 96.502 13.667 0.333 0.143

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Density plots of the learning and general ability measures.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Density plots of the WM measures, by domain.

Individual Differences Analyses

To reduce the four WM measures to more stable dimensions of individual differences, an exploratory factor analysis was performed on the four WM tests. A Varimax extraction identified the following two factors. The first factor, accounting for 49.7% of the variance, had the following component loadings: digit span (.892), backward digit span (.743), dot matrix (.135), and Mr X (.207). The second factor accounted for further 20.2% of variance and had the following loadings: digit span (−.571), backward digit span (−.28), dot matrix (.488), and Mr X (.408). The first factor is accordingly labelled verbal WM and the second factor visuo-spatial WM.

The matrix of correlation coefficients for the principal measures and the two factor scores is shown in Table 3. All WM measures were significantly correlated with reading and mathematics scores with the exception of the dot matrix test of visuo-spatial STM and WJ maths fluency. Vocabulary and nonverbal reasoning scores were also significantly correlated with the WM test scores. Reading was more strongly correlated with the verbal than the visuo-spatial WM factor, p = .001. No significant differences in the strengths of the simple correlations across the two WM factors were found for mathematics scores (p > .05, both cases). The upper triangle of Table 3 shows the partial correlation coefficients with the matrix reasoning measure taken into account in order to control for general fluid cognitive abilities. The correlations between the two WM factors and reading remained significant, with a significantly higher correlation for verbal than visuo-spatial WM, p = .005. The WIAT Numerical operations test correlated significantly with both factor scores, but the WJ fluency measure correlated significantly only with verbal WM. No significant differences in the strengths of the correlations with the two WM factors were found for either mathematics test (p > .05, both cases).

Table 3.

Correlation Coefficients for Principal Measures and WM Factor Scores: Simple Correlations in Lower Triangle, Partial Correlations Controlling for Matrix Reasoning in Upper Triangle

Digit span Backward digit span Dot matrix Mr X Verbal WM Visuo-spatial WM Vocabulary Reading WIAT maths WJ maths
Digit span 1 0.391** 0.200** 0.219** 0.885** 0.205** 0.314** 0.359** 0.164* 0.293*
Backward digit span .433** 1 0.234** 0.295** 0.773** 0.414** 0.292** 0.338** 0.267** 0.355**
Dot matrix .254** .320** 1 0.293** 0.226** 0.805** 0.127 0.189** 0.272** 0.100
Mr X .260** .354** .349** 1 0.313** 0.789** 0.169* 0.048 0.259** 0.205
Verbal WM .892** .743** 0.135 .207** 1 0.339** 0.363** 0.412** 0.247** 0.383**
Visuo-spatial WM 0.076 .338** .817** .776** - 1 0.186** 0.155* 0.342** 0.200
Vocabulary .374** .416** .270** .268** .415** .254** 1 0.396** 0.195* 0.425**
Reading .402** .414** .274** 0.125 .449** .163* .490** 1 0.340** 0.396**
WIAT maths .247** .394** .385** .342** .275** .404** .408** .444** 1 -
WJ maths .356** .464** 0.245 .297* .443** .261* .576** .489** - 1
Matrix reasoning .215** .350** .321** .247** .256** .315** .519** .329** .509** .507**
*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

We examined whether the particularly close association between verbal WM and reading could be mediated by vocabulary knowledge, which is also known to have strong links with verbal aspects of STM (Baddeley et al., 1998, Gathercole, 2006a, Gathercole, 2006b). Further partial correlations were performed controlling for differences in vocabulary scores. The correlation between reading and verbal WM remained highly significant, r(207) = .321, p < .001.

Diagnostic Analyses

The extent to which scores on individual WM tests distinguished those children with either low reading or low mathematics scores from children with age-typical scores was investigated. Each child was classified according to whether each learning and WM score were in the deficit range. Two cut-off values were used to define deficits: 80 (1.33 SDs below the population mean, corresponding to the lowest 9.2% in a normally distributed population) and 85 (1 SD below the population mean, corresponding to the lowest 15.9%). The patterns of overlap in the frequency distributions for each pairwise combination of a learning measure and a WM test scores were examined.

The resulting frequency distributions and summary statistics are shown in Table 4. The two principal measures were the probabilities that a child with and without learning performance in a deficit range had a WM score in the deficit range. The presence of a WM deficit in a child with age-typical learning in the present sample must be interpreted cautiously, as all of the children had been recruited on the basis of reports of learning-related difficulties even if there were not detectable in the learning assessments at the time of the clinic visit. Sub-threshold WM problems could therefore be present in these children.

Table 4.

Frequency Data and Statistics by Learning and WM Cut-Off Values

Working memory
Digit recall
Backward digit
Dot matrix
Mr X
Cutoff value: 80
85
80
85
80
85
80
85
Group =< > =< > =< > =< > =< > =< > =< > =< >
Reading
 80− 27 56 40 43 17 66 40 43 24 59 33 50 15 68 24 59
 81+ 21 106 30 97 12 115 26 101 14 113 27 100 13 114 27 100
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.33 0.48 0.20 0.48 0.29 0.40 0.18 0.29
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.17 0.24 0.09 0.20 0.11 0.21 0.10 0.21
 85− 33 77 49 61 23 87 50 60 28 82 41 69 19 91 32 78
 86+ 15 85 21 79 6 94 16 84 10 90 19 81 9 91 19 81
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.30 0.45 0.21 0.45 0.25 0.37 0.17 0.29
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.15 0.21 0.06 0.16 0.10 0.19 0.09 0.19



Maths WIAT
 80− 20 45 30 35 14 51 29 36 19 46 29 36 17 48 27 38
 81+ 11 70 19 62 7 74 16 65 7 74 13 68 6 75 12 69
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.31 0.46 0.22 0.45 0.29 0.45 0.26 0.42
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.14 0.23 0.09 0.20 0.09 0.16 0.07 0.15
 85− 26 68 39 55 17 77 37 57 23 71 34 60 20 74 33 61
 86+ 5 47 10 42 4 48 8 44 3 49 8 44 3 49 6 46
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.28 0.41 0.18 0.39 0.24 0.36 0.21 0.35
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.10 0.19 0.08 0.15 0.06 0.15 0.06 0.12



Maths WJ
 80− 12 27 14 25 7 32 18 21 10 29 13 26 5 34 10 29
 81+ 4 18 5 17 1 21 2 20 2 20 5 17 1 21 2 20
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.31 0.36 0.18 0.46 0.26 0.33 0.13 0.26
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.18 0.23 0.05 0.09 0.09 0.23 0.05 0.09
 85− 15 32 18 29 8 39 20 27 11 36 16 31 6 41 11 36
 86+ 1 13 1 13 0 14 0 14 1 13 2 12 0 14 1 13
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.32 0.38 0.17 0.43 0.23 0.34 0.13 0.23
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.07 0.07 0.00 0.00 0.07 0.14 0.00 0.07



Mean values across all measures
 p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.31 0.42 0.19 0.44 0.26 0.38 0.18 0.31
 p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.13 0.20 0.06 0.13 0.09 0.18 0.06 0.14

The frequency distributions and statistics for each combination of learning and WM cut-off scores are shown in Table 4. The proportions of children with learning scores in the deficit range with deficits on an individual test of WM was higher than would be expected by chance alone (.24 compared with .09 for the cut-off point of 80, and .39 compared with .16 the 85 cut-off point). The prevalence was nonetheless relatively low, present in only a minority of struggling learners. The strength of associations between the four tests of WM and the three learning measures did not vary systematically. The incidence of WM deficits in the children with reading scores in the age-typical range was lower (between .13 and .18), close to chance levels.

Next we investigated whether deficits aggregated across the multiple WM tests would improve discrimination of learning scores. There are two reasons why this might be the case. First, although test reliabilities for all measures included in this study are reasonably high (in excess of .80 in each case), the error component of any individual score will inevitably limit the differentiation between the children with and without detectable academic learning difficulties. The error can be reduced by combining data from multiple assessments. Second, the greatest challenge to learning abilities may arise when more than one aspect of WM is impaired in an individual, and this cannot be identified by examining test statistics in isolation. The number of WM scores in the deficit range was calculated for each child. For cut-off values of 80, the frequencies of each number of deficits were as follows: 0 (125), 1 (40), 2 (26), 3 (15), 4 (4). For 85, the frequencies were: 0 (73), 1 (68), 2 (35), 3 (27), 4 (7). Given the relatively low frequency of occurrence of 2, 3 and 4 deficits, each child was classified according to whether he or she had either (i) no WM deficit scores or (ii) 1 or more deficits at each criterion level. The resulting frequency distributions are shown in Table 5.

Table 5.

Frequency Data and Statistics for Learning Cut-Off Values by Total WM Deficits

Reading
Maths WIAT
Maths WJ
WM cut-off 80
85
80
85
80
85
Total WM deficits 0 1+ 0 1+ 0 1+ 0 1+ 0 1+ 0 1+
Learning cut-off
80− 34 49 15 68 27 38 10 55 19 20 12 27
81+ 91 36 58 69 59 22 36 45 18 4 13 9
p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.59 0.82 0.58 0.85 0.51 0.69
p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.28 0.54 0.27 0.56 0.18 0.41



80− 49 61 23 87 45 49 21 73 24 23 14 33
86+ 76 24 50 50 41 11 25 27 13 1 11 3
p (reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.55 0.79 0.52 0.78 0.49 0.70
p (no reading deficit|WM deficit) 0.24 0.50 0.21 0.52 0.07 0.21

A much stronger association between WM and learning was evident than for the data from single tests reported above. Across the whole sample, 40% of the children had WM deficit scores at the 80 criterion (chance level 32%), and 64% (chance level 50%). Averaged across the three learning measures, 54% of the children with poor learning scores had one or more WM test scores at or below 80, and 77% for a criterion of 85. The corresponding values for children with learning scores above the deficit range were much lower, at 21% and 46%. Similar levels of deficits were observed across the reading test and both mathematics measures.

Discussion

This study investigated the extent to which WM problems are present in individuals within a large and heterogeneous sample of children struggling academically in mainstream schooling. The children were referred to the research clinic on the basis of attention, learning, and memory problems by practitioners within education and community health services. At the clinic visit, the majority of the children scored poorly on measures of literacy and mathematics. However, their mean vocabulary performance was age-appropriate and nonverbal reasoning abilities were in the low average range.

Learning abilities within the sample were closely associated with two distinct but correlated aspects of WM: a verbal dimension incorporating both STM and more complex WM test scores, and a visuo-spatial second dimension also extending across both STM and WM tasks (Jarvis & Gathercole, 2003). Both WM dimensions were correlated with achievements in reading and mathematics abilities. For reading, the association was stronger for verbal than visuo-spatial WM. This is entirely consistent with reports of poor verbal STM and WM skills in children with reading difficulties and dyslexia (e.g., Kudo et al., 2015). The special association between reading and verbal WM persisted even when fluid nonverbal abilities and vocabulary knowledge were taken into account. This suggests that the association does not simply reflect the broader dimensional constructs of either language or domain-general higher-level cognitive abilities.

There is much still to be understood regarding the links between verbal WM and reading. The potential role of more specific phonological processing skills in mediating these links is inconsistent across individual studies (e.g., McDougall et al., 1994, Swanson and Jerman, 2007) and has been strongly debated (Hulme and Snowling, 2009, Kudo et al., 2015). Whether the deficits in verbal STM and verbal WM have common or distinct origins is also unclear (Swanson et al., 2009). These uncertainties are a challenge for confident interpretation of the link between verbal aspects of WM and reading. It leaves open the contrasting possibilities that verbal WM may either contribute to the phonologically-based processing and learning of orthography required for literacy acquisition, or more simply arise from the common contribution of phonological processing skills (Hulme and Snowling, 2009, Ramus et al., 2013).

Mathematical abilities were strongly linked with both verbal and visuo-spatial aspects of WM. This is broadly consistent with reports that when the commonly comorbid reading impairments in children with mathematical difficulties characterized by strong verbal WM deficits are excluded, a specific impairment in visuo-spatial WM remains (Swanson et al., 2009, Szucs et al., 2013). Many elements of mathematical learning such as processing and transforming spatial relations such as symmetry, 3D geometry, and use of graphs require the learner to use spatially-based representations. It may therefore be unsurprising that the ability to do so is closely associated with an individual's visuo-spatial WM capacity (Holmes and Adams, 2006, Jarvis and Gathercole, 2003, St Clair-Thompson and Gathercole, 2006, Toll et al., 2016)

The current study also investigated the degree to which problems in learning for the majority of individuals within this sample could plausibly be attributed to WM. Performance on single WM test scores was only weakly related to learning achievements. Only about a third of the children with difficulties in reading or mathematics scored at the deficit level for any single test, and even fewer individuals with age-typical learning scores. The test scores also failed to show the same sensitivity to individual differences in the domain-specific dimensions of WM such as the stronger links between reading and verbal than visuo-spatial WM that was evident in correlational analyses across the sample. Individual WM test scores are therefore not reliable indicators of significant current problems in reading and mathematics.

More compelling evidence that WM problems accompany learning difficulties for the majority of children was provided when deficits across multiple WM tests were considered. Approximately three-quarters of the children performing poorly on the reading and mathematics tests scored at least one standard deviation below the population mean on one or more WM test. Of the remaining children with learning performance in the age-typical range, 54% obtained deficit scores on 1 or more test, close to the chance level of 50%. Scoring poorly on one of the four WM tests employed in this study therefore increases the risk of a child within the current sample by 50% of having poor reading or mathematics performance. This high level of co-occurrence in WM and learning deficits even within a sample at high risk of learning difficulties reinforces other evidence for the value of WM as a screening tool prior to or at school entry to identify prospectively children at risk of poor academic attainment (Gathercole et al., 2003, Gersten et al., 2005).

The present findings are entirely consistent with proposals that WM is critical for key classroom activities including remembering instructions (Gathercole and Alloway, 2008, Gathercole et al., 2006), the use of mathematical strategies and mental arithmetic (e.g., Adams and Hitch, 1997, Geary et al., 2004, Imbo et al., 2007a, Imbo et al., 2007b, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). On this basis low WM capacities would indeed be expected to impair learning. It should, however, be acknowledged that WM impairments rarely occur in isolation during childhood and need to be considered in their broader developmental context. Verbal measures of WM are closely associated with other risk factors for learning including information processing speed e.g., (Moll, Göbel, Gooch, Landerl, & Snowling, 2016), weak phonological processing (e.g., Hulme and Snowling, 2009, Ramus et al., 2013, Ramus and Szenkovits, 2008) and fluid cognitive abilities including problem-solving (e.g., Engle and Kane, 2004, Kudo et al., 2015, Swanson et al., 2009). Rather than operating as a stand-alone system, WM may therefore be better conceived as closely integrated with the broader dimensions of cognitive abilities that include language and executive systems. By this account, WM deficits are symptoms of constellations of developmental problems that adversely affect learning but are also correlated with other cognitive abilities imposing other constraints. This perspective is in step with the emerging consensus within the field of cognitive developmental science that neurodevelopmental disorders of attention and learning are most appropriately characterized by impairments in multiple broad dimensions that can exist either singly or in combination in individual children (Coghill and Sonuga-Barke, 2012, Hulme and Snowling, 2009, Ramus et al., 2013, Willcutt et al., 2013).

For these reasons, it is unlikely that the kinds of learning difficulties encountered by many children in the current sample can be wholly explained in terms of their WM problems or, indeed, that addressing these problems in isolation will be sufficient to overcome their educational problems. Understanding the nature of the WM limitations of individual children may nonetheless be extremely valuable informing their educational support. Poor WM undoubtedly compromises cognitive performance in mental activities central to the classroom. Experimental evidence from adults and neuropsychological patients that cannot be readily explained in terms of other developmentally correlated skills has established that reducing WM capacities impairs three key abilities: understanding and following instructions (Caramazza et al., 1981, Jaroslawska et al., 2016, Vallar and Baddeley, 1987, Yang et al., 2013), learning the phonological structures of new words (Baddeley et al., 1988, Papagno and Vallar, 1992, Papagno et al., 1991) and engaging in mental arithmetic (Adams & Hitch, 1997). The implications are clear. Classrooms and teaching methods designed to help compensate or avoid WM-related learning failures, such as providing access to and training with effective mnemonic devices and employing methods for information delivery that minimize WM loads, should be one key element of programmes of educational support for struggling learners (Elliott et al., 2010, Gathercole and Alloway, 2008).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

The CALM research clinic at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit in Cambridge (CBSU) is supported by funding from the Medical Research Council of Great Britain to Duncan Astle, Susan Gathercole and Tom Manly. The clinic is led by Dr Joni Holmes and managed by Francesca Woolgar, with data collection by Joe Bathelt, Erica Bottacin, Sarah Bishop, Sally Butterfield, Gemma Crickmore, Laura Forde, Sara Gharooni, Erin Hawkins, Agnieszka Jaroslawska, Amy Johnson and Sinead O’Brien. Thanks go the many professionals working in children's services in the South-East and East of England for their support, and to the children and their families for giving up their time to visit the clinic.

Footnotes

Authors Note

Please note that this paper was handled by the current editorial team of JARMAC.

References

  1. Adams J.W., Hitch G.J. Working memory and children's mental addition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1997;67(1):21–38. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alloway T.P. Pearson Publishing; 2007. Automated working memory assessment. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alloway T.P., Gathercole S.E., Pickering S.J. Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development. 2006;77(6):1698–1716. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Archibald L., Gathercole S.E. The complexities of complex span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language. 2007;57:174–194. [Google Scholar]
  5. Archibald L.M.D., Joanisse M.F. On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:899–914. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0099). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Archibald L.M., Gathercole S.E. Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders/Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists. 2006;41(6):675–693. doi: 10.1080/13682820500442602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Archibald L.M., Gathercole S.E. Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders/Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists. 2006;41(6):1–13. doi: 10.1080/13682820500442602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2000;4(11):417–423. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baddeley A.D., Hitch G. Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 1974;8:47–89. [Google Scholar]
  10. Baddeley A., Gathercole S., Papagno C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review. 1998;105(1):158–173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Baddeley A.D., Papagno C., Vallar G. When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language. 1988;27:586–596. [Google Scholar]
  12. Barrouillet P., Lépine R. Working memory and children's use of retrieval to solve addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2005;91(3):183–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bayliss D.M., Jarrold C., Gunn D.M., Baddeley A.D. The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2003;132(1):71–92. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bishop D.V., North T., Donlan C. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1996;37(4):391–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bishop D.V., Snowling M.J. Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130(6):858–886. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bull R., Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001;19(3):273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cain K., Oakhill P., Bryant P. Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96(1):31–42. [Google Scholar]
  18. Caramazza A., Basili A.G., Koller J.J., Berndt R.S. An investigation of repetition and language processing in a case of conduction aphasia. Brain and Language. 1981;14:235–271. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(81)90078-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Coghill D., Sonuga-Barke E.J. Annual research review: Categories versus dimensions in the classification and conceptualisation of child and adolescent mental disorders – Implications of recent empirical study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2012;53(5):469–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conners C.K. 3rd ed. Pearson Publishing; 2008. Conners Parent Rating Scale. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cowan N. An embedded-processes model of working memory. Models of Working Memory. 1999;20:506. [Google Scholar]
  22. Daneman M., Carpenter P.A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;19:450–466. [Google Scholar]
  23. DeStefano D., LeFevre J.-A. The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2004;16(3):353–386. [Google Scholar]
  24. Dollaghan C., Campbell T. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1136–1146. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dunn L., Dunn D. GL Assessment; 2009. British Picture Vocabulary Scale. [Google Scholar]
  26. Elliott J.G., Gathercole S.E., Alloway T.P., Holmes J., Kirkwood H. An evaluation of a classroom-based intervention to help overcome working memory difficulties and improve long-term academic achievement. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology. 2010;9(3):227–250. [Google Scholar]
  27. Engle R.W., Kane M.J. Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In: Ross B., editor. Vol. 44. Elsevier; New York: 2004. pp. 145–199. (The psychology of learning and motivation). [Google Scholar]
  28. Gathercole S.E., Pickering S.J., Knight C., Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from National Curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2004;40:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gathercole S.E. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2006;27:513–543. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gathercole S.E. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2006;27:599–613. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gathercole S.E., Alloway T.P. Sage; 2008. Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gathercole S.E., Brown L., Pickering S.J. Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educational and Child Psychology. 2003;20(3):109–122. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gathercole S.E., Lamont E., Alloway T.P. Working memory in the classroom. Working Memory and Education. 2006:219–240. [Google Scholar]
  34. Geary D.C., Hoard M.K., Byrd-Craven J., DeSoto M.C. Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;88:121–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gersten R., Jordan N.C., Flojo J.R. Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38(4):293–304. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380040301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hambrick D.Z., Engle R.W., Kane M.J. The role of working memory in higher level cognition: Domain-specific vs. domain-general perspectives. In: Sternberg R.J., Pretz J., editors. Intelligence and cognition. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hesketh A., Conti-Ramsden G. Memory and language in middle childhood in individuals with a history of specific language impairment. PLOS ONE. 2013;8(2):e56314. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Holmes J., Adams J.W. Working memory and children's mathematical skills: Implications for mathematical development and mathematics curricula. Educational Psychology. 2006;26(3):339–366. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hulme C., Snowling M.J. John Wiley & Sons; Padstow: 2009. Developmental disorders of language learning and cognition. [Google Scholar]
  40. Imbo I., Vandierendonck A., De Rammelaere S. The role of working memory in the carry operation of mental arithmetic: Number and value of the carry. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2007;60(5):708–731. doi: 10.1080/17470210600762447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Imbo I., Vandierendonck A., Vergauwe E. The role of working memory in carrying and borrowing. Psychological Research. 2007;71(4):467–483. doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0044-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jaroslawska A.J., Gathercole S.E., Logie M.R., Holmes J. Following instructions in a virtual school: Does working memory play a role? Memory & Cognition. 2016;44(4):580–589. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0579-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jarvis H.L., Gathercole S.E. Verbal and non-verbal working memory and achievements on national curriculum tests at 11 and 14 years of age. Educational and Child Psychology. 2003;20(3):123–140. [Google Scholar]
  44. Kane M.J., Hambrick D.Z., Tuholski S.W., Wilhelm O., Payne T.W., Engle R.W. The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133(2):189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kane M.J., Conway A.R.A., Hambrick D.Z., Engle R.W. Variation in working-memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In: Conway A.R.A., Jarrold C., Kane M.J., Miyake A., Towse J.N., editors. Variation in working memory. Oxford University Press; New York: 2007. pp. 21–48. [Google Scholar]
  46. Krajewski K., Schneider W. Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual–spatial working memory, and preschool quantity – Number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103(4):516–531. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kudo M.F., Lussier C.M., Swanson H.L. Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2015;40:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McDougall S., Hulme C., Ellis A., Monk A. Learning to read: The role of short-term memory and phonological skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1994;58(1):112–133. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1994.1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Metsala J.L. Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;91(1):3. [Google Scholar]
  50. Miyake A., Friedman N.P., Emerson M.J., Witzki A.H., Howerter A., Wager T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Moll K., Göbel S.M., Gooch D., Landerl K., Snowling M.J. Cognitive risk factors for specific learning disorder processing speed, temporal processing, and working memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2016;49(3):272–281. doi: 10.1177/0022219414547221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Montgomery J.W. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43(2):293–308. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.293. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10757685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Montgomery J.W., Evans J.L. Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52(2):269–288. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0116). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Newbury D.F., Bishop D.V.M., Monaco A.P. Genetic influences on language impairment and phonological short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(11):528–534. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Papagno C., Valentine T., Baddeley A.D. Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language. 1991;30:331–347. [Google Scholar]
  56. Papagno C., Vallar G. Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effects of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1992;44A:47–67. [Google Scholar]
  57. Pimperton H., Nation K. Poor comprehenders in the classroom: Teacher ratings of behavior in children with poor reading comprehension and its relationship with individual differences in working memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2012 doi: 10.1177/0022219412454172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ramus F., Marshall C.R., Rosen S., van der Lely H.K. Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain. 2013;136(Pt 2):630–645. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ramus F., Szenkovits G. What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2008;61(1):129–141. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Schuchardt K., Bockmann A.-K., Bornemann G., Maehler C. Working memory functioning in children with learning disorders and specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders. 2013;33(4):298–312. [Google Scholar]
  61. Siegel L.S., Ryan E.B. The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development. 1989:973–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03528.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. St Clair-Thompson H.L., Gathercole S.E. Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2006;59(4):745–759. doi: 10.1080/17470210500162854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Swanson H.L. Working memory in learning disability subgroups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1993;56(1):87–114. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1993.1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Swanson H.L. Short-term memory and working memory: Do both contribute to our understanding of academic achievement in children and adults with learning disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1994;27(1):34–50. doi: 10.1177/002221949402700107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Swanson H.L., Beebe-Frankenberger M. The relationship between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96(3):471. [Google Scholar]
  66. Swanson H.L., Jerman O. The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2007;96(4):249–283. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Swanson H.L., Sachse-Lee C. Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001;79(3):294–321. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Swanson H.L., Xinhua Z., Jerman O. Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2009;42(3):260–287. doi: 10.1177/0022219409331958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Szucs D., Devine A., Soltesz F., Nobes A., Gabriel F. Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex. 2013;49(10):2674–2688. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Toll S.W.M., Kroesbergen E.H., Van Luit J.H. Visual working memory and number sense: Testing the double deficit hypothesis in mathematics. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2016 doi: 10.1111/bjep.12116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Unsworth N., Engle R.W. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review. 2007;114(1):104–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vallar G., Baddeley A.D. Phonological short-term store, phonological process and sentence comprehension: A neuropsychological case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 1987;1:417–438. [Google Scholar]
  73. Van den Bos F., van der Ven S.H.G., Kroesbergen E.H., van Luit J.E.H. Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review. 2013;10:29–44. [Google Scholar]
  74. Vukovic R.K., Lesaux N.K., Siegel L.S. The mathematics skills of children with reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences. 2010;20(6):639–643. [Google Scholar]
  75. Wechsler D. Pearson Publishing; 2005. Weschler Indivisual Achievement Test II. [Google Scholar]
  76. Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. [Google Scholar]
  77. Wickham H. Springer Science & Business Media; 2009. ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wilhelm O., Hildebrandt A., Oberauer K. What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Willcutt E.G., Petrill S.A., Wu S., Boada R., Defries J.C., Olson R.K. Comorbidity between reading disability and math disability: Concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2013;46(6):500–516. doi: 10.1177/0022219413477476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Woodcock R.W., McGrew K.S., Mather N. Riverside Publishing Company; 2001. Woodcock Johnson III Test of Achievement. [Google Scholar]
  81. Yang T., Gathercole S.E., Allen R.J. Benefit of enactment over oral repetition of verbal instruction does not require additional working memory during encoding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2013 doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0471-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES