Abstract
Objective:
To investigate the relation between orthostatic hypotension (OH) and posture-mediated cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease (PD) using a cross-sectional and within-group design.
Methods:
Individuals without dementia with idiopathic PD included 18 with OH (PDOH) and 19 without OH; 18 control participants were also included. Neuropsychological tests were conducted in supine and upright-tilted positions. Blood pressure was assessed in each posture.
Results:
The PD groups performed similarly while supine, demonstrating executive dysfunction in sustained attention and response inhibition, and reduced semantic fluency and verbal memory (encoding and retention). Upright posture exacerbated and broadened these deficits in the PDOH group to include phonemic fluency, psychomotor speed, and auditory working memory. When group-specific supine scores were used as baseline anchors, both PD groups showed cognitive changes following tilt, with the PDOH group exhibiting a wider range of deficits in executive function and memory as well as significant changes in visuospatial function.
Conclusions:
Cognitive deficits in PD have been widely reported with assessments performed in the supine position, as seen in both our PD groups. Here we demonstrated that those with PDOH had transient, posture-mediated changes in excess of those found in PD without OH. These observed changes suggest an acute, reversible effect. Understanding the effects of OH due to autonomic failure on cognition is desirable, particularly as neuroimaging and clinical assessments collect data only in the supine or seated positions. Identification of a distinct neuropsychological profile in PD with OH has quality of life implications, and OH presents itself as a possible target for intervention in cognitive disturbance.
Orthostatic hypotension (OH) is among the most commonly reported nonmotor manifestations in persons with Parkinson disease (PD), with prevalence as high as 53%.1 Symptoms include lightheadedness, fatigue, neck pain, presyncope, and syncope.2,3 The causes can include both CNS and peripheral nervous system degeneration.4 Dopamine replacement therapies have been implicated as well.5
OH is associated with cognitive impairment. Even when controlling for systolic blood pressure (SBP), both elderly and younger individuals with OH show relative deficits in verbal memory and sustained attention, both of which are predictors of subsequent cognitive decline that is greater than would be expected in the context of normal aging.6
Idiopathic PD is itself associated with cognitive deficits. Historically, these were thought to be limited to psychomotor/information processing speeds and caused by disease-specific subcortical pathologies.7 However, deficits across executive functions are present, even in cases without significant motor slowing, and are attributed to an alteration of connectivity between prefrontal and striatal regions, where dopaminergic and cholinergic projections are abundant.8–12 An updated model of cognitive decline in PD is now appropriately characterized as a disconnection syndrome.13
We hypothesized that cognition is transiently impaired during OH in PD and that persons with PD and OH would have cognitive deficits that are more severe than those with PD without OH. We used standard autonomic assessment tools to determine whether OH would be associated with immediate effects on cognition as well as impairment independent of posture.
METHODS
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.
The study was approved by the Boston University and Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center Institutional Review Boards. All participants provided written informed consent.
Study sample.
Fifty-five individuals without dementia participated in the study: 18 patients with PD and neurogenic OH (PDOH), 19 normotensive patients with PD without OH (PD), and 18 control participants (controls). OH was defined as a sustained reduction in SBP of at least 20 mm Hg or a reduction in diastolic blood pressure (DBP) of at least 10 mm Hg during the first 3 minutes of standing or head-up tilt on a 60° tilt table.14 All groups were matched on age, education, and male:female ratio. Control participants were not excluded if maintained on antihypertensive medications, so long as they were normotensive at the time of testing and had no evidence of OH. The Mini-Mental State Examination was used as a general cognitive screening with a cutoff of 27 for controls and 25 for PD. The cutoff for PD was lower to account for disease-specific (i.e., motor) errors that are not associated with dementia.15 Participants also received the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II),16 the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS),17 and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI).18 For those with PD, side of symptom onset, Hoehn & Yahr motor stage, and treatment history were obtained. All met clinical criteria for mild to moderate idiopathic PD (Hoehn & Yahr stages I-II). Dopaminergic therapies were converted to levodopa equivalent dosages.19
Autonomic assessment.
Assessment of autonomic functioning was completed in accordance with standardized clinical protocols. Participants were instructed to remain on all medications as prescribed and to eat a light breakfast the morning of testing. Testing was scheduled for a 12:00 pm start time to minimize diurnal effects on hemodynamics. Upon arrival, participants were allowed a 20-minute rest in the supine position to attain psychological and physiologic equilibration. RR interval, beat-to-beat blood pressure (Finometer; FMS, Amsterdam, the Netherlands), and oscillometric blood pressure (Dinamap; Critikon Company, Tampa, FL) were measured for 5 minutes in the supine position, followed by 15 minutes in the 60-degree tilted position.
Prespecified exclusion criteria included any subjective symptoms associated with orthostasis (lightheadedness, dizziness) that were rated as severe (>8 on a 10-point Likert scale), significant tachycardia (>150 BPM), or SBP in the range associated with presyncope/syncope. Of the 55 participants who met eligibility, none was excluded using these criteria. During the testing session, arterial pressure was measured for 3 minutes at 1-minute intervals while supine. A battery of neuropsychological tests was then administered. Participants were then tilted to 60° at a rate of 6°/s. Arterial pressures were again measured at 1-minute intervals for 3 minutes prior to the start of the second session of testing. Following test completion, participants were returned to the supine position. Once systolic pressure returned to within 10 mm Hg of baseline, this sequence (supine-tilt) was repeated for sessions 3 and 4. Throughout the assessment, participants reported subjective symptoms using the same 10-point Likert scale introduced during the initial screening. To control for the effects of subjective symptoms on cognitive performance, a score of 8 or higher was used to determine data exclusion; no participants met this criterion.
Cognitive assessment.
The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) was administered prior to testing as a measure of premorbid verbal intelligence.20 Each of the 4 testing sessions was completed in approximately 15 minutes. For each session, the order of test administration was counterbalanced, and validated alternate forms or split-halves were used in each subsequent session to maintain internal consistency. All tests with visual stimuli or motor components were adapted for projection on a large screen at approximately 53° visual angle with responses given orally.21 Specific tests included the following.
Attention/executive functioning.
A Stroop test was used as a measure of sustained attention and response inhibition (color vs color-word conditions)22; the Digit Span Test (forward [DSF], backward [DSB]) to assess basic auditory attention (DSF, DSB) and working memory (DSB); the Arithmetic test to assess working memory and logical reasoning23; Verbal Fluency tests (phonemic and semantic) to assess lexical retrieval processes24; and the Symbol Search Test to assess visual scanning and psychomotor speed.23
Memory.
The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) Verbal Learning Test was used to assess verbal memory. Variables analyzed included scores on the first recall trial, the total encoded words across trials (total score), learning slope, and delayed recall (assessed 10 minutes after the final learning trial).25
Visuospatial functioning.
The Hemifield Lines Test was used to measure right/left biases in 2 conditions. For this measure, a line presented in one hemifield changes incrementally in size until the participant perceives it to be the same size as a line of constant length in the opposite hemifield.26 The Line Bisection test was used to determine egocentric reference point, and the Visual Dependence test was used to assess line orientation and angle judgment.26
Statistical analysis.
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS v17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Analyses of variance with Tukey post hoc analysis were used to examine group differences on clinical measures except for median Hoehn & Yahr score (χ2 analysis). Group differences in baseline supine conditions were analyzed using one-way analysis of variance with Tukey post hoc analysis. Paired-samples t tests were used to detect differences from supine to tilted performance in within-group cognitive performance, and change in z score performance from supine to tilted positions was used as an index of relative impairment. The α for cognitive measures was 0.01 to correct for multiple comparisons.
RESULTS
Study sample.
None of the participants met criteria for dementia. There were no differences across groups with regard to age, male:female distribution, or premorbid verbal IQ. PD groups were similar with regard to left/right side of symptom onset, disease stage/duration, and levodopa equivalent dosages (table 1). Patients with PDOH were more likely to be on antihypotensive medications than control and PD groups. Controls were more likely to be on antihypertensive medications than patients with PDOH but not patients with PD. Both PD and PDOH groups scored higher on the BDI-II than did controls (p < 0.05, both groups) and patients with PDOH scored higher than patients with PD (p < 0.05). Patients with PDOH scored higher on the GDS than did controls (p < 0.05), while differences between PD groups were not significant. On the BAI, both PD groups scored higher than controls (p < 0.05) but were similar to each other.
Table 1.
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Hemodynamic measures.
There were no significant group differences in baseline SBP, DBP, or heart rate. Supine hypertension (SPB >135 mm Hg or DBP >100) was found in 6 of 19 patients with PDOH, 5 of 18 patients with PD, and 4 of 18 controls (table 2).
Table 2.
Hemodynamic information
Cognitive measures.
Supine.
Compared to controls, both PD groups showed impaired cognition (table 3). Patients with PD performed more poorly than controls on semantic fluency and on both Stroop conditions. They also displayed reduced memory encoding and delayed recall. There were no differences on any visuospatial measures. Patients with PDOH were impaired relative to controls on several executive tasks, including DSB, Symbol Search, both fluency tests, and the Stroop test. They were also impaired on all memory submeasures, but not on any visuospatial measures. There were no differences between PD groups on any cognitive measure.
Table 3.
Across-group comparison of performance on cognitive measures while supine
Upright tilt.
Compared to controls, patients with PD were impaired on several executive measures and had poorer memory encoding (table 4). Patients with PDOH, however, performed more poorly than controls on nearly all executive measures, including several that did not elicit differences between controls and patients with PD. Patients with PDOH also had worse memory encoding than patients with PD. There were no significant differences between groups on any measure of visuospatial functioning.
Table 4.
Across-group comparison of performance on cognitive measures under upright tilt
Changes in performance.
Within-group, following tilt.
Controls had no within-group changes when supine and upright performances were compared. Patients with PD demonstrated deficits when upright relative to supine on Symbol Search and 2 CERAD subtests (Learning and Delayed Recall, p < 0.001). Patients with PDOH showed posture-mediated impairment on nearly all measures of cognition, including Arithmetic (p < 0.001), Symbol Search (p < 0.001), both fluency conditions (p < 0.01), memory encoding and retention (p < 0.01), and Line Bisection (p < 0.01).
Across-group, following tilt.
The change in group-specific z score following tilt was used as an index of relative performance (i.e., to control for baseline differences that may have skewed the effect of postural change) (figure). There were no significant differences in the effect of postural change on cognition between the controls and PD groups. By contrast, patients with PDOH showed a greater posture-mediated impairment than controls on several tests, including Arithmetic, Symbol Search, Phonemic Fluency, DSB, the Stroop test (color-word condition), memory encoding (p < 0.01), and Visual Dependence (p = 0.01). Compared to patients with PD, patients with PDOH showed a significantly greater effect of postural change on Symbol Search (p < 0.01), CERAD total score (p < 0.01), and Visual Dependence (p < 0.01).
Figure. Cognitive performance reflected as group-specific (normal control [NC], Parkinson disease [PD], Parkinson disease with orthostatic hypotension [PDOH]) change from baseline following tilt.
Prior to analysis of change, all raw scores on cognitive measures were converted to group-specific z scores, where the mean and SD in the supine position of each specific group (NC, PD, PDOH) were used to determine relative within-group performance while under upright tilt. Values reported are for within-group z score change for each measure. Error bars represent standard error. *NC vs PDOH, p < 0.01.
DISCUSSION
We assessed cross-sectional and within-group, posture-specific neuropsychological performance to determine the effects of OH on cognition in patients with PD. When cognition was assessed in the upright position, we found more widespread deficits in executive function and memory as well as visuospatial impairments in PDOH as part of an overall exacerbated deficit profile, with a subsequent return to baseline performance in the supine position. This transient change was not observed in patients with PD or control participants and would thus imply a direct effect of OH.
Cognitive deficits in PD are, at least in part, the result of central neurodegeneration. This presents a challenge when determining the relative contribution of other comorbidities. Reports of cognitive deficits in pure autonomic failure, where degeneration, at least in the early stages of the disease, is limited to the peripheral autonomic nervous system, suggest that hemodynamic changes may indeed play a contributing role. Cognitive deficits centered in the domains of memory and executive functioning have also been observed in autonomic autoimmune ganglionopathy, a rare disorder in which nicotinic acetylcholine receptor antibodies disrupt transmission across autonomic ganglia, leading to autonomic failure. Following plasma exchange and titer reduction, OH resolves and cognition improves.27,28
As expected, both PD groups displayed frontostriatal and visuospatial cognitive deficits relative to controls. This pattern is observed in as many as 55% of all individuals with PD and is most likely the result of a functional disconnection. Specifically, the basal ganglia and their dense dopaminergic and cholinergic projections to multiple cortical regions as well as the thalamus are selectively affected in a progressive and disease-specific process.29 It is this central degeneration that causes global alterations of neurotransmission and the emergence of numerous motor and nonmotor signs that are associated with the disease.
We found no meaningful differences across PD groups while supine. This contrasts with at least one previous report.30 Unlike this previous study, in which the participants with PD included those with and without supine hypertension, our PD groups included only normotensive participants. Thus, we reduced the likelihood of comorbid white matter angiopathy, a known and independent risk factor for cognitive impairment.31–33 PD groups were also matched across measures of disease duration, motor symptom severity, and levodopa equivalent dosages. There is considerable support, therefore, that these transient decrements in cognition following postural change are independently related to a failure of cerebral autoregulation during orthostatic stress.
The overwhelming majority of clinical neuropsychological tests, if not all, are administered in the seated position. Based on these results, it seems reasonable to modify assessment methodology in PD to include testing in a variety of postures. Notably, none of our participants reported severe symptoms of OH and many were asymptomatic; nevertheless, cognitive deficits emerged. Clinicians should consider incorporating both autonomic and functional cognitive assessments in all persons with PD regardless of subjective concerns brought forth by the patient. Furthermore, as delayed OH is common in patients with PD and other α-synucleinopathies, clinicians should also be mindful of this manifestation of autonomic failure in those individuals who show a normal hemodynamic response within the first 3 minutes of standing.34,35
A multiphasic cognitive profile, if detected as part of a comprehensive assessment across postural changes, would be instructive for providers and would reveal otherwise unrecognized targets for intervention. For instance, impairments in verbal fluency, already noted in PD, could make it increasingly difficult to communicate effectively, particularly in the setting of already reduced nonverbal expression caused by declining motor functions. This might not be appreciated in a private office setting, but may manifest when conducting affairs in public spaces where sitting is not an option and the effects of social anxiety are perhaps more palpable. Impairments in visual and spatial processing could lead to problems in the marketplace, where searching for wanted items among a varied array of goods would be a particular challenge. Difficulties with judging line orientation and egocentric reference point could affect proprioception and other aspects of postural stability, thereby increasing the risk of falls that would otherwise be entirely attributed to motor features of PD. Again, clinical providers might miss an important target for intervention when not considering the contribution of such cognitive impairment. Working memory problems, along with lapses in self-monitoring and vigilance, might lead to difficulties in tracking conversations, counting change, or when processing pedestrian safety signals and other orienting markers, further complicating social interactions and possibly leading to withdrawal from meaningful activities.
There are also implications for functional neuroimaging studies, including those measuring regional metabolic activity and markers of network connectivity, as imaging data are collected while supine and thus without consideration of posture-induced hemodynamic changes. If the cause of cognitive change in OH is related to alterations in regional cerebral blood flow or task-specific metabolic activity as we suggest, these prior studies may have failed to demonstrate fully the neurophysiologic underpinnings of cognitive decline in diseases where OH is present.
It is, of course, difficult to fully rule out all potential confounds when assessing cognition, particularly when utilizing repeat assessments and in disease states where multiple comorbidities are common. With regard to possible iatrogenic effects of dopamine agonists, which have been shown to produce or worsen OH and cause cognitive deficits, we controlled for levodopa equivalent dosages and found no associated differences in OH severity or cognitive performance.36,37 We also addressed potential confounds that would have otherwise affected interpretation of our neuropsychological test findings. As noted, we ensured that no participants had severe (hence distracting) symptoms during tilt, and while reliance on the subjective report has some inherent flaws, we are confident that the cognitive changes as observed in this study were not driven by state-based factors related to somatic stress. With regard to the potential threats to validity following repeated neuropsychological assessment, several methodologic safeguards were put in place. We counterbalanced the order of test administration, used alternate forms when available, and randomized split-halves when no alternate forms existed. Perhaps more importantly, had any order or practice effects emerged, they would have served to mitigate our hypothesized differences across groups, as all participants were first tested in the supine position.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors thank the patients for their participation; Dr. Daniel Tarsy, Dr. Ludy Shih, Dr. Katherine Czarnecki, and Dr. David Simon for support with clinical referrals; Steven Dewitte of the “Moving Forward” support group and its members for help with organizing participant travel and collaboration; and Rachel Lavoi for help with data collection and entry.
GLOSSARY
- BAI
Beck Anxiety Index
- BDI-II
Beck Depression Inventory II
- CERAD
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease
- DBP
diastolic blood pressure
- DSB
Digit Span Backward
- DSF
Digit Span Forward
- GDS
Geriatric Depression Scale
- OH
orthostatic hypotension
- PD
Parkinson disease
- PDOH
Parkinson disease with orthostatic hypotension
- SBP
systolic blood pressure
- WTAR
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
Footnotes
Editorial, page 11
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Dr. Centi: study design and conceptualization, data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation, primary preparation of manuscript. Dr. Freeman: study design and conceptualization, interpretation of the data, and manuscript preparation. Dr. Neargarder: primary statistical analysis and interpretation of the data, manuscript preparation. Dr. Gibbons: study design and conceptualization, interpretation of the data. Alex Canova: data acquisition, analysis and interpretation, manuscript preparation. Dr. Cronin-Golomb: study design and conceptualization, interpretation of the data, manuscript preparation.
STUDY FUNDING
Supported by a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F31NS074801) (J.C.) and by grants R01NS067128 (A.C.-G.) and U54NS065736 (R.F.) from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
DISCLOSURE
The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.
REFERENCES
- 1.Senard JM, Rai S, Lapeyre-Mestre M, et al. Prevalence of orthostatic hypotension in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997;63:584–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Xin W, Mi S, Lin Z, Wang H, Wei W. Orthostatic hypotension and the risk of incidental cardiovascular diseases: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Prev Med 2016;85:90–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Xin W, Lin Z, Mi S. Orthostatic hypotension and mortality risk: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Heart 2014;100:406–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Goldstein DS, Holmes CS, Dendi R, Bruce SR, Li ST. Orthostatic hypotension from sympathetic denervation in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 2002;58:1247–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Goldstein DS. Dysautonomia in Parkinson's disease: neurocardiological abnormalities. Lancet Neurol 2003;2:669–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Czajkowska J, Ozhog S, Smith E, Perlmuter LC. Cognition and hopelessness in association with subsyndromal orthostatic hypotension. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2010;65:873–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Revonsuo A, Portin R, Koivikko L, Rinne JO, Rinne UK. Slowing of information processing in Parkinson's disease. Brain Cogn 1993;21:87–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sawamoto N, Honda M, Hanakawa T, Fukuyama H, Shibasaki H. Cognitive slowing in Parkinson's disease: a behavioral evaluation independent of motor slowing. J Neurosci 2002;22:5198–5203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sawada Y, Nishio Y, Suzuki K, et al. Attentional set-shifting deficit in Parkinson's disease is associated with prefrontal dysfunction: an FDG-PET study. PLoS One 2012;7:e38498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Poletti M, Bonuccelli U. Orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex functioning in Parkinson's disease: neuropsychological evidence. Brain Cogn 2012;79:23–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Koerts J, Van Beilen M, Tucha O, Leenders KL, Brouwer WH. Executive functioning in daily life in Parkinson's disease: initiative, planning and multi-task performance. PLoS One 2011;6:e29254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jokinen P, Karrasch M, Bruck A, Johansson J, Bergman J, Rinne JO. Cognitive slowing in Parkinson's disease is related to frontostriatal dopaminergic dysfunction. J Neurol Sci 2013;329:23–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cronin-Golomb A. Parkinson's disease as a disconnection syndrome. Neuropsychol Rev 2010;20:191–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Freeman R, Wieling W, Axelrod FB, et al. Consensus statement on the definition of orthostatic hypotension, neurally mediated syncope and the postural tachycardia syndrome. Clin Auton Res 2011;21:69–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Jefferson AL, Cosentino SA, Ball SK, et al. Errors produced on the Mini-Mental State Examination and neuropsychological test performance in Alzheimer's disease, ischemic vascular dementia, and Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;14:311–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Robinson BE, Kelley L. Concurrent validity of the Beck Depression Inventory as a measure of depression. Psychol Rep 1996;79:929–930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale. Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24:709–711. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56:893–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gjerstad MD, Boeve B, Wentzel-Larsen T, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Occurrence and clinical correlates of REM sleep behaviour disorder in patients with Parkinson's disease over time. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:387–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Green RE, Melo B, Christensen B, Ngo LA, Monette G, Bradbury C. Measuring premorbid IQ in traumatic brain injury: an examination of the validity of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2008;30:163–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Azulay JP, Mesure S, Amblard B, Pouget J. Increased visual dependence in Parkinson's disease. Percept Mot Skills 2002;95:1106–1114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Trenerry MR. Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tulsky DS. Clinical Interpretation of the WAIS III and WMS III. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Homack S, Lee D, Riccio CA. Test review: Delis-Kaplan executive function system. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2005;27:599–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Welsh K, Butters N, Hughes J, Mohs R, Heyman A. Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer's disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch Neurol 1991;48:278–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Davidsdottir S, Wagenaar R, Young D, Cronin-Golomb A. Impact of optic flow perception and egocentric coordinates on veering in Parkinson's disease. Brain 2008;131:2882–2893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gibbons CH, Freeman R. Antibody titers predict clinical features of autoimmune autonomic ganglionopathy. Auton Neurosci 2009;146:8–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gibbons CH, Centi J, Vernino S, Freeman R. Autoimmune autonomic ganglionopathy with reversible cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2012;69:461–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zgaljardic DJ, Borod JC, Foldi NS, Mattis P. A review of the cognitive and behavioral sequelae of Parkinson's disease: relationship to frontostriatal circuitry. Cogn Behav Neurol 2003;16:193–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kim JS, Oh YS, Lee KS, Kim YI, Yang DW, Goldstein DS. Association of cognitive dysfunction with neurocirculatory abnormalities in early Parkinson disease. Neurology 2012;79:1323–1331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lee SJ, Kim JS, Yoo JY, et al. Influence of white matter hyperintensities on the cognition of patients with Parkinson disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010;24:227–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Maillard P, Carmichael O, Fletcher E, Reed B, Mungas D, DeCarli C. Coevolution of white matter hyperintensities and cognition in the elderly. Neurology 2012;79:442–448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hajjar I, Quach L, Yang F, et al. Hypertension, white matter hyperintensities, and concurrent impairments in mobility, cognition, and mood: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Circulation 2011;123:858–865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gibbons CH, Freeman R. Clinical implications of delayed orthostatic hypotension: a 10-year follow-up study. Neurology 2015;85:1362–1367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gibbons CH, Freeman R. Delayed orthostatic hypotension: a frequent cause of orthostatic intolerance. Neurology 2006;67:28–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kujawa K, Leurgans S, Raman R, Blasucci L, Goetz CG. Acute orthostatic hypotension when starting dopamine agonists in Parkinson's disease. Arch Neurol 2000;57:1461–1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Reid JL, Calne DB, George CF, Vakil SD. The action of L(-)-dopa on baroreflexes in parkinsonism. Clin Sci 1972;43:851–859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.