Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Jan 3.
Published in final edited form as: Prog Brain Res. 2015 Nov 14;224:285–304. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.019

Cognitive interventions for addiction medicine: Understanding the underlying neurobiological mechanisms

Anna Zilverstand 1, Muhammad A Parvaz 1, Scott J Moeller 1, Rita Z Goldstein 1,1
PMCID: PMC5206794  NIHMSID: NIHMS836904  PMID: 26822363

Abstract

Neuroimaging provides a tool for investigating the neurobiological mechanisms of cognitive interventions in addiction. The aim of this review was to describe the brain circuits that are recruited during cognitive interventions, examining differences between various treatment modalities while highlighting core mechanisms, in drug addicted individuals. Based on a systematic Medline search we reviewed neuroimaging studies on cognitive behavioral therapy, cognitive inhibition of craving, motivational interventions, emotion regulation, mindfulness, and neurofeedback training in addiction. Across intervention modalities, common results included the normalization of aberrant activity in the brain’s reward circuitry, and the recruitment and strengthening of the brain’s inhibitory control network. Results suggest that different cognitive interventions act, at least partly, through recruitment of a common inhibitory control network as a core mechanism. This implies potential transfer effects between training modalities. Overall, results confirm that chronically hypoactive prefrontal regions implicated in cognitive control in addiction can be normalized through cognitive means.

Keywords: Substance use disorders, CBT, Treatment, Psychotherapy, Training, fMRI, EEG, Cognitive control, Emotion regulation, Neurofeedback

1 INTRODUCTION

Addiction is a chronically relapsing disorder, characterized by continued drug-seeking despite reduced pleasure derived from the drug. Its recurring nature poses a substantial economic burden to society, and significant personal distress to the individual and their family (Volkow et al., 2011). Current standard treatment options include cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and motivational interventions. Cognitive-behavioral treatment approaches comprise an array of cognitive techniques and behavioral skills trainings aimed at increasing coping skills, which are employed depending on the individual’s needs (Carroll, 1998). Motivational interventions aim at enhancing intrinsic motivation to change (Smedslund et al., 2011). Behavioral treatment studies show that both treatment modalities are effective in alleviating craving, preventing relapse and reducing substance use across a range of substance use disorders (Dutra et al., 2008). However, the exact neural mechanisms underlying such treatments are not fully known; their better understanding may enhance the further development of therapeutic interventions.

Imaging techniques, such as positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and electroencephalography (EEG), offer a window into the functioning brain, providing a unique opportunity to examine the neurobiological effects of these interventions in addiction. Imaging studies can be used to describe the brain systems involved in select interventions, clarify which mechanisms are dysfunctional, offering the opportunity to explore differences and commonalities between different interventions. Imaging-based neurobiological indicators entail information that goes beyond self-report or behavior alone and have been shown to be good predictors of relapse following treatment (Brewer et al., 2008; Janes et al., 2010; Paulus et al., 2005; Moeller et al., 2015, this volume). They may thus provide unique information, hinting at potential new and efficacious treatment options.

While previous reviews and a meta-analysis of the neurobiological effects of treatment in addiction have broadly discussed the mechanisms underlying current treatment approaches (Konova et al., 2013; Potenza et al., 2011), this review discusses the neurobiological mechanisms of select cognitive and motivational interventions in more depth. To review the current neuroimaging literature on cognitive and motivational interventions in addiction in adults, we performed a systematic Medline search and a manual search of the citation lists of the relevant publications. Keywords used for the Medline search were: treatment, therapy, psychotherapy, training, intervention, cognitive behavioral therapy, contingency management, cessation, coping, cognitive control, regulation, self-regulation, reappraisal, mindfulness, neurofeedback, and biofeedback. These treatment keywords were used in a combined search with substance, drug, cocaine, alcohol, marijuana, nicotine, abuse, dependence, use, and either EEG, fMRI, or PET. This search identified 24 neuroimaging studies of cognitive interventions, investigating the effect of CBT (n = 2), cognitive inhibition of craving (n = 6), motivational interventions (n = 7), emotion regulation (n = 2), mindfulness training (n = 2), and neurofeedback training (n = 6). Neuroimaging studies examining training of working memory (n = 1) and attention bias training (n = 1) were excluded due to the low number of studies. Of the identified neuroimaging studies seventeen (71%) were conducted with smokers, four (17%) with cocaine users, two (8%) with alcoholics, and one (4%) with a mixed population.

2 COGNITIVE INTERVENTIONS

Generally, two different research approaches were implemented in the selected studies. Either a pre–post study design was employed to investigate posttreatment change in relation to a pretreatment baseline measurement, or participants were scanned while they were actively engaged in a therapeutic intervention.

2.1 COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY

Standard CBT comprises an array of approaches directed toward modifying dysfunctional thinking and behavior. The two critical components are analysis of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, as well as skills training for achieving active behavior and thought modification (Carroll, 1998). Skills training, considered essential to treatment, includes interventions to enhance motivation and coping with craving, and dealing with high-risk situations. Additional skills training depends on the patient’s needs and includes self-monitoring, emotion regulation, relaxation, and building a social support network (Carroll, 1998). Standard CBT has been shown to be effective in reducing drug use in individuals addicted to nicotine, alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, and opiates and in polysubstance use disorders (Dutra et al., 2008).

Although CBT has been used in addiction treatment for over two decades, not many studies have employed neuroimaging to demonstrate the neurobiological changes associated with treatment. We identified two studies that used neuroimaging to investigate its effects. Both studies, performed in smokers and a mixed population of drug users prior to and after 2 months of CBT, reported a reduction in drug use and an improvement on relevant behavioral measures (Costello et al., 2010; DeVito et al., 2012; Table 1). Both employed a pre–post design to compare brain activation before and after treatment in a within-subjects design, while controlling for treatment-unrelated changes over time by including a control group. The first study investigated PET resting glucose metabolism, comparing the CBT group (nicotine smokers) to another group of smokers receiving placebo treatment (Costello et al., 2010). The PET resting data analysis revealed reduced glucose metabolism in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) in the CBT group compared to the placebo group after treatment. This region encodes subjective valuation of potential rewards (Schacht et al., 2012), and is associated with attentional bias toward drug cues (Kang et al., 2012) and attentional control (Leech and Sharp, 2014). The second study used fMRI to investigate brain function during the Stroop color–word task, a neuropsychological task designed to probe cognitive control (DeVito et al., 2012). In comparison to healthy controls, drug users displayed a greater decrease in brain activation levels in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and globus pallidus after treatment relative to the pretreatment baseline. Both regions are part of the brain’s dopaminergic reward system involved in reinforcement learning, which has been linked to the core symptoms of addiction (Everitt and Robbins, 2005). This study also reported treatment-related changes in the subthalamic nucleus (STN) (as well as the thalamus and hypothalamus) (DeVito et al., 2012), which has been proposed to play a role in impulse control (Bari and Robbins, 2013). Overall, results suggest that treatment with CBT over the course of 2 months led to a reduced recruitment of regions involved in reward processing, attention, and impulse control. The normalized aberrant activation in the brain’s reward circuitry could indicate reduced reward sensitivity and reactivity after treatment.

Table 1.

Reviewed Studies

First Author Year Treatment Modality Intervention Subjects Addiction Behavioral Effects Imaging Main Contrast Brain Effects

Costello 2010 CBT 8 week EG = 20
PG = 17
Nicotine ↓ Smoking, craving FDG
PET
Group * treatment interaction Resting state: ↓ PCC
DeVito 2012 CBT 8 week EG = 12
HC = 12
Mixed ↓ Drug use, Stroop interference 3T fMRI Group * treatment interaction Stroop task: ↓ VTA, globus pallidus, STN, thalamus, hypothalamus
Brody 2007 Cognitive inhibition Self-select EG = 42 Nicotine NS on craving 1.5T fMRI Resist>crave ↑ dACC, medial SFG, rACC, precuneus, PCC; ↓ cuneus, MOG, MTG, postcentral
Hartwell 2011 Cognitive inhibition Self-select EG = 32 Nicotine ↓ Craving 3T fMRI Resist>crave ↑ IFG, STG, precentral; ↓ mid cingulate, precuneus, cuneus
Volkow 2010 Cognitive inhibition Four strategies EG = 24 Cocaine ↓ Craving FDG
PET
Inhibit>watch ↓ NAcc, OFC; ↓ NAcc negatively correlated with ↑ IFG
Kober 2010 Cognitive inhibition Consequences EG = 21 Nicotine ↓ Craving 3T fMRI Later>now ↑ Medial SFG, IFG, postcentral, STG, MTG, ITG; ↓ striatum (correlated with craving), VTA, OFC, rACC, insula, parahippocampal, dACC, dlPFC, PCC, MTG, IPL
Zhao 2012 Cognitive inhibition Soothing EG = 16 Nicotine ↓ Craving 3T fMRI Reappraise>watch ↑ dACC, cuneus, parietal, paracentral, cerebellum; ↓ vlPFC
Tabibnia 2014 Cognitive inhibition Reappraise EG = 25 Nicotine ↓ Craving 3T fMRI Reappraise>watch ↑ dACC, precentral, thalamus, insula
Langleben 2009 Motivational intervention PSA EG = 18 Nicotine ↑ Recognition 3T fMRI Ad>neutral video ↑ mPFC, STG, parahippocampal, precuneus, lingual, caudate, cerebellum; ↓ insula, dlPFC, SPL, occipital
Wang 2013 Motivational intervention PSA Strong = 33
Weak = 30
Nicotine ↓ Smoking 3T fMRI Strong ad>weak ad ↑ mPFC (predicted reduced smoking), precuneus correlated with intention to quit smoking
Chua 2009a Motivational intervention Tailored messages EG = 24 Nicotine NA 3T fMRI High tailored>low tailored>generic ↑ mPFC, medial SFG, IFG, lingual, precentral, MTG, STG, precuneus, PCC
Chua 2009b Motivational intervention Tailored messages EG = 41 Nicotine NA 3T fMRI Tailored (3 sorts)> neutral ↑ mPFC, medial SFG (personalized), precuneus/PCC (personalized), vmPFC (motivational), dlPFC (instructional)
Chua 2011 Motivational intervention Tailored messages EG = 91 Nicotine ↑ Recognition 3T fMRI Tailored> untailored>neutral ↑ mPFC, medial SFG, IFG, MTG, STG, precuneus, PCC, IPL, cerebellum
Feldstein Ewing 2011 Motivational intervention Individual message EG = 10 Alcohol NA 3T fMRI Change message>counter change message ↓ SFG, postcentral, IPL
Wilson 2013 Motivational intervention Self-focused Self = 28
Other = 29
Nicotine NS 3T fMRI Self-focused > other-focused ↑ mPFC, IFG, rACC, dACC, insula, precuneus, IPL, cerebellum
Tabibnia 2014 Emotion regulation Reappraisal EG = 25 Nicotine ↓ Negative emotion 3T fMRI Reappraise>watch ↑ IFG, dACC, dlPFC, pre-SMA
Albein-Urious 2014 Emotion regulation Reappraisal EG = 17
HC = 18
Cocaine ↓ Negative emotion 3T fMRI Group * reappraisal interaction ↓ IFG, dlPFC, OFC, insula, thalamus, PCC, cuneus, occipital, cerebellum
Tang 2013 Mindfulness Meditation EG = 15
PG = 12
Nicotine ↓ Smoking, craving 3T fMRI Post>pre Resting state: ↑ IFG, dACC, mPFC, vlPFC
Westbrook 2013 Mindfulness Mindful attention EG = 47 Nicotine ↓ Craving, distress 3T fMRI Mindful>watch ↓ sgACC (↓ correlation sgACC with striatum), vmPFC
Hanlon 2013 Neurofeedback ↓ sgACC ↑ dmPFC simultaneous EG = 15 Nicotine NS 3T fMRI Regulate>crave ↓ sgACC (↓ sgACC correlated with decrease in craving)
Li 2013 Neurofeedback ↓ sgACC alternating with ↑ dmPFC/MFG EG = 10 Nicotine ↓ Craving (↓ sgACC) 3T fMRI Regulate>crave ↓ sgACC (↓ sgACC correlated with decrease in craving)
Canterberry 2013 Neurofeedback ↓ sgACC EG = 9 Nicotine ↓ Craving 3T fMRI Regulate>crave ↓ sgACC
Peniston 1989 Neurofeedback Autogenic training+↑ alpha/theta EG = 10
PG = 10
Alcohol ↓ Relapse, depression EEG Group * treatment interaction ↑ % alpha, % theta, amplitude alpha (parietal)
Stotts 2006 Neurofeedback Motivational+↑ P300 EG = 17
PG = 14
Cocaine ↑ Adherence to treatment EEG Group * treatment interaction ↑ P300
Horrell 2010 Neurofeedback Motivational+↑ SMR EG = 10 Cocaine ↓ Drug use, stress, depression EEG Neurofeedback > baseline ↑ SMR; ↓ gamma (during cue exposure)

CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy; EG, experimental group; PG, placebo patient group; HC, healthy control group; VTA, ventral tegmental area; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; STN, subthalamic nucleus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; vlPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; dmPFC, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex; sgACC, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; SPL, superior parietal lobe; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; P300, positive 300; SMR, sensory motor rhythm.

However, since CBT is a multimodal treatment, results cannot be used to ascertain the precise components that may have contributed to the observed changes. Neuroimaging during the use of the select interventions is therefore necessary to achieve a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms.

2.2 COGNITIVE INHIBITION

Cognitive inhibition of craving is one of the cognitive control techniques practiced within standard CBT to enhance the patient’s ability to cope with craving and prevent relapse (Carroll, 1998). The deployment of effective cognitive control strategies for reducing craving is also associated with longer periods of abstinence in smokers (Bliss et al., 1989; Swan et al., 1996). Cognitive techniques most commonly used intuitively by abstinent smokers include self-talk, contemplating the adverse effects of drug use and the positive effects of abstinence, and distraction (Bliss et al., 1989). Similar strategies, along with talking about and reliving craving, are included in standard treatment protocols (Carroll, 1998).

We identified six neuroimaging studies (five fMRI studies in smokers and one PET study in cocaine users) that implemented a cognitive inhibition task within the scanner, to image participants while they were actively engaged in the use of a therapeutic technique. All six studies employed a within-subjects design comparing conditions where craving is induced by visual cues to those where craving is volitionally controlled during cue exposure. Two of the fMRI studies asked smokers to “resist” their urge to smoke using a strategy of their own choice (Brody et al., 2007; Hartwell et al., 2011). In both these studies distraction and ignoring smoking cues were reported as being the most commonly used strategy, but only participants in the second study reported that they also contemplated the adverse effects of their addiction. Importantly, only participants of this second study achieved a significant reduction in craving. The remaining four studies used explicit instructions for implementing cognitive control. These instructions included the following: “ignore your thoughts,” “do not recall how it felt,” “try to relax,” or “shift attention away” during PET imaging in cocaine users (Volkow et al., 2010); “think about long-term consequences associated with smoking” (Kober et al., 2010); “conjure a soothing image from nature” (Zhao et al., 2012); and “reinterpret” (Tabibnia et al., 2014) during fMRI imaging in smokers. Participants in all four studies with explicit instructions achieved a significant reduction of craving during the regulation condition (Table 1).

The most consistent finding across all studies was the recruitment of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), both core regions of the inhibitory control network (Luijten et al., 2014), during successful cognitive inhibition of craving. All studies reported upregulation of at least one region in this network, which has been reported to be activated during tasks requiring inhibition of a behavioral response (Luijten et al., 2014). Increased activation was also found in areas involved in executive functioning, including the medial superior frontal gyrus (SFG) (Nee et al., 2013) and precentral gyrus, and areas supporting language processing, such as the superior temporal gyrus (STG). Downregulation of regions in the brain’s reward circuitry, including VTA, nucleus accumbens (NAcc), striatum, and parahippocampal gyrus (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Koob and Volkow, 2010), was the second most consistent result across all studies. This downregulatory effect extended into regions that encode value signals in emotion regulation (vlPFC) (Kohn et al., 2014), represent expected value (OFC) (Chase et al., 2015), and self-relevance during emotional processing (rACC) (Moeller and Goldstein, 2014). Importantly, metabolism/activation levels in a core region of the reward network, the NAcc/striatum, was negatively correlated with metabolism in the inhibitory control network (IFG), and positively correlated with craving (Kober et al., 2010; Volkow et al., 2010). As the NAcc has been most consistently linked to subjective craving (Schacht et al., 2012), this suggests that recruitment of the inhibitory control network is closely associated with reduced reward processing. Third, regions involved in motivated attention (precuneus, PCC), and self-referential emotional processing (rACC), were not consistently up- or downregulated. Finally, while the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) has been implicated as a core region in inhibitory control of negative affect (Kober et al., 2010), none of the six studies showed involvement of the dlPFC during cognitive inhibition of craving in a whole-brain analysis (Table 1). In summary, results suggest that the same inhibitory control network as that activated during tasks requiring inhibition of a behavioral response is recruited during cognitive inhibition of craving, thereby exerting regulatory influence on regions in the brain’s reward circuitry.

Overall results from these cognitive inhibition studies are comparable to the studies utilizing the CBT approach. Both lines of research suggest that cognitive techniques can be employed to inhibit the reward system of the brain. Importantly, the cognitive inhibition results demonstrate that substance abusers can cognitively recruit their inhibitory control network to enhance control over craving. This suggests that inhibitory control regions, which are chronically hypoactive in addiction (Luijten et al., 2014), can be reactivated through cognitive effort at least in a supervised, explicitly instructed, and monitored environment.

2.3 MOTIVATIONAL INTERVENTIONS

Another technique used in standard treatment is motivational enhancement. Motivational interventions are designed to increase the commitment to make behavioral and psychological changes. Motivational interviewing is one form of intervention that is effective in reducing drug use in addiction (Smedslund et al., 2011). It enhances the motivation for change through reflective listening, analyzing the discrepancy between goals and current behavior, and supporting self-efficacy. A different form of motivational intervention, designed as a stand-alone intervention, uses public service announcements (PSAs) that are broadcasted to modify public behavior. Evidence for the efficacy of these advertisements is mixed (Wang et al., 2013). However, when change messages are tailored to provide individualized support, they do have a measurable effect on drug use behavior in smokers (Lancaster and Stead, 2002).

We found seven fMRI studies that implemented a motivational intervention during imaging, six in smokers and one in alcoholics. All studies contrasted a stronger intervention condition with a weaker intervention or no intervention. Five studies employed a within-subjects design. The first contrasted smoking PSA ads with neutral videos (Langleben et al., 2009); three other studies compared individually tailored verbal smoking cessation messages to low tailored, untailored, and neutral messages, respectively (Chua et al., 2009a,b, 2011); and the fifth study investigated verbal change messages developed during a motivational interview (“I need to stop drinking—it is ruining my life”) in comparison to counter change messages (“drinking is not a problem”) in alcoholics (Feldstein Ewing et al., 2011). While the direct effects on drug use behavior could not be evaluated with these within-subjects designs, a recognition test administered after the intervention showed improved retention of stronger messages, confirming that stronger interventions had an enhanced effect (Chua et al., 2011; Langleben et al., 2009). To directly evaluate the effects of motivational intervention on smoking behavior, the remaining two studies used a between-subjects design. In one study participants watched strong or weak smoking PSA ads, as categorized by independent raters (Wang et al., 2013). This study reported differences in smoking behavior after the intervention, with the stronger PSA ad group demonstrating a reduction in smoking after treatment (Table 1). However, the study which compared the effects of “self-focused” versus “other-focused” motivational messages did not find a significant impact on cigarette use behavior (Wilson et al., 2013). This study was the only study that, in both the self-relevant and the control conditions, presented motivational messages which were exclusively focused on the positive effects of quitting rather than the negative consequences of drug use.

With the exception of one study (Feldstein Ewing et al., 2011), all of these studies showed the recruitment of a self-referential processing network (Table 1). This network includes regions such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and precuneus, and areas involved in motivated attention (e.g., PCC). Activation in the mPFC has been linked to self-referential processing and self-awareness in healthy subjects (D’Argembeau et al., 2007), with abnormalities in this region contributing to self-awareness deficits in addiction (Moeller and Goldstein, 2014). Similarly, the precuneus has been linked to first-person perspective taking and experience of agency (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), and has been implicated in addiction (Engelmann et al., 2012; Schacht et al., 2012). The involvement of the PCC suggests high motivated attention during internally directed cognition (Kang et al., 2012; Leech and Sharp, 2014; Schacht et al., 2012). Activation of this self-referential processing network during strong motivational intervention conditions indicates that the presented information was processed with regard to its self-relevance during the intervention. Importantly, activation levels in the precuneus were correlated with the intention to quit smoking in one study (Wang et al., 2013), supporting the notion that the information processed was indeed relevant to therapeutic change. Furthermore, the majority of studies demonstrated recruitment of the inhibitory control network (e.g., IFG and dACC) during strong interventions, suggesting that self-referential information was updated in a way that strengthened inhibitory control. Additionally, imaging results showed the involvement of regions involved in executive functioning (e.g., SFG) (Nee et al., 2013) and the semantic network, including MTG, STG, and IPL, involved in verbal and semantic processing (Binder et al., 2009; Rauschecker, 2011). Evidence for activation level changes in other regions, such as the insula and dlPFC, was limited and inconsistent. Finally, none of the studies reported reduced activation in regions associated with the reward circuit, indicating that there was no immediate impact of motivational interventions on reward sensitivity. Overall, only one study diverged from this very consistent activation pattern, showing reduced activation of the semantic network during the change intervention (Feldstein Ewing et al., 2011). This single study in alcoholics, which used completely individualized messages, comprised of the smallest sample size among all other studies of motivational intervention. The diverging results, which may be driven by differences in procedure and population, should therefore be interpreted with caution.

The imaging results thus generally confirm the efficacy of the motivational intervention. Stronger interventions enhanced self-referential processing, and were associated with a strengthening of inhibitory control networks. This further suggests that cognitive motivational strategies, which in contrast to cognitive inhibition strategies do not directly train self-control of craving, can nevertheless be effective in recruiting inhibitory control.

2.4 AFFECT REGULATION

Addiction is associated with elevated negative affect and deficits in emotion regulation (Cheetham et al., 2010). Importantly, effective affect regulation plays a role in maintaining abstinence, as high negative affect has been shown to predict drug use and relapse (Albein-Urios et al., 2014; Cheetham et al., 2010). Skill training for improving self-regulation of affect is therefore an optional component of standard CBT treatment. The aim of such training is to find strategies for reducing the intensity of the affective response. Cognitive reappraisal, which reduces the intensity of the emotional response through reinterpretation of the situation (Gross, 1998), is one possible cognitive strategy.

We identified two fMRI studies that investigated cognitive reappraisal of negative emotions in addiction. Both studies employed a within-subjects design, contrasting a “regulation” condition with a “watch” condition during presentation of unpleasant high arousing pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Albein-Urios et al., 2014; Tabibnia et al., 2014). Participants were instructed to either look passively at the IAPS pictures during the “watch” condition, or to regulate emotional stress by reinterpreting or distancing themselves from the pictures’ content during the “regulation” condition. The first study was performed in smokers (Tabibnia et al., 2014); and the second in cocaine users, additionally comparing them with healthy controls (Albein-Urios et al., 2014). In both studies participants successfully downregulated subjective emotion during the regulation condition (Table 1), with the cocaine users performing the regulation task equally well as healthy controls, but showing slightly higher overall negative affect according to self-report.

During reappraisal, smokers activated the inhibitory control network (e.g., IFG and dACC), as well as the dlPFC involved in goal setting during motivated behavior (Ballard et al., 2011), and supplementary motor area (SMA) activated during motor planning (Luijten et al., 2014; Table 1). Both inhibitory control regions and the dlPFC have been previously implicated in the regulation of negative affect (Diekhof et al., 2011). Cocaine users, however, did not show any increased activation during affect regulation, but instead showed reduced recruitment of the inhibitory control network (e.g., IFG), areas involved in goal setting, working memory, arousal and attention (e.g., dlPFC, insula, and PCC), thalamus, and the OFC when compared to healthy controls (Table 1). Thus, the cocaine users did not activate the network known to be involved in the regulation of negative affect in healthy subjects (Diekhof et al., 2011), supporting previous behavioral studies on affective dysregulation in addiction (Cheetham et al., 2010). This impaired recruitment of the emotion control network in cocaine users may be related to reported higher overall levels of affect, as similar impairment of recruitment of inhibitory control regions and dlPFC has been shown in patients with pathologically increased negative affect levels (Manber-Ball et al., 2013; New et al., 2009).

The imaging results therefore demonstrate that brain circuits involved in regulation of negative affect partly overlap with brain systems involved in cognitive control of craving, suggesting that recruitment of the inhibitory control network is relevant both during affect regulation and cognitive control of craving. This further implies that disruption in affect regulation may be related to impaired inhibitory control in addiction.

2.5 MINDFULNESS TRAINING

Mindfulness training is a systematic training of attention and self-control (Tang et al., 2013). Participants are asked to attend to any thought, feeling, or sensation that occurs by simply acknowledging it, without attempting to regulate emotions (Westbrook et al., 2013). This technique is designed to help participants maintain a state of inner ease through taking a neutral stance, therefore reducing craving by reducing reactivity to drug-related cues (Westbrook et al., 2013). While mindfulness techniques have not been integrated into standard treatment protocols of addiction, a recent randomized controlled trial shows their effectiveness in reducing smoking behavior (Brewer et al., 2011).

We found two fMRI studies which implemented a mindfulness intervention in smokers. One study compared a group of smokers participating in a 2-week mindfulness training with a second group taking part in a 2-week relaxation training (Tang et al., 2013). As compared to smokers in the relaxation group, smokers in the mindfulness group achieved a reduction in self-reported craving and smoking as measured with carbon monoxide monitor after the 2-week intervention. Changes in brain activation were evaluated with a pre–post training fMRI resting state scan. Brain imaging results largely demonstrated involvement of the same networks as in the other cognitive interventions. After the 2-week mindfulness training participants showed increased recruitment of the inhibitory control network (e.g., IFG and dACC), the self-referential processing network (medial PFC), and a region involved in encoding value signals in emotion regulation (e.g., vlPFC) (Table 1). The second study implemented a within-subjects design to directly investigate the mechanisms of mindfulness techniques during cue-induced craving (Westbrook et al., 2013). Participants reported lower level of craving and distress during mindful attention to smoking cues, which was accompanied by reduced activation level in the subgenual ACC (sgACC) and adjacent vmPFC (Table 1). This reduction in sgACC activation levels likely reflects the reported reduction in craving, as sgACC has been generally implicated in increased craving in smokers (Engelmann et al., 2012; Kober et al., 2010). There was no evidence for an involvement of prefrontal control regions during mindful attention (Westbrook et al., 2013), suggesting that mindfulness may act through a different mechanism than other cognitive interventions. Importantly, brain imaging also demonstrated that mindful attention to drug cues led to a reduced correlation between activation levels in the sgACC and striatum, a core region implicated in reward processing. This indicates that reduced reactivity to drug cues may be the main therapeutic mechanism, as opposed to enhancement of cognitive control in other interventions.

The results therefore suggest that while mindfulness interventions may overall strengthen the same networks implicated in other cognitive interventions, the mechanisms involved may be different.

2.6 NEUROFEEDBACK TRAINING

Neurofeedback training allows participants to self-regulate their own brain response. Participants are presented with either fMRI- or EEG-based feedback derived from select relevant brain processes. Thus, subjects receive instant feedback regarding potentially pathological brain processes, hypothesized to aid in shaping one’s own brain activation in a desired direction, and thereby improving symptoms. Neurofeedback training can be used either as a stand-alone intervention to modify dysfunctional brain activation patterns without explicit instruction, or as a tool to enhance learning of cognitive and behavioral strategies. A significant advantage of neurofeedback training is its ability to be customized for each participant as the feedback is dependent on the participant’s own neural activity. While there is limited evidence for efficacy of EEG neurofeedback training in addiction (Sokhadze et al., 2008), the clinical effectiveness of fMRI neurofeedback awaits to be explored empirically.

We identified six neurofeedback training studies (three fMRI and three EEG) which used neuroimaging to evaluate treatment success. The fMRI neurofeedback training studies were performed in smokers, and comprised two or three training sessions employing a within-subjects paradigm to compare a self-regulation feedback condition with a craving condition (Canterberry et al., 2013; Hanlon et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013). Specifically, participants were asked to use a self-selected strategy to downregulate sgACC activation levels (Canterberry et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013), as a measure of craving-related brain processes, and/or upregulate dorsal mPFC/ middle frontal gyrus (MFG) activation levels to increase prefrontal control over craving (Hanlon et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013). Results showed that participants were able to downregulate the sgACC signal, leading to an associated reduction in craving (Canterberry et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013). Similarly, when smokers were instructed to downregulate sgACC activation while simultaneously upregulating dorsal mPFC/MFG signal, the sgACC reduction was correlated with a respective reduction in craving (Hanlon et al., 2013; Table 1). This demonstrates that regions implicated in increased craving in smokers (Engelmann et al., 2012; Kober et al., 2010) can be controlled by voluntary self-regulation guided by neural feedback only. Results further showed that participants were not able to increase the dorsal mPFC/MFG signal, when presented separately or displayed simultaneously with the sgACC signal. They also did not experience a change in subjective craving during upregulation of dorsal mPFC/MFG (Hanlon et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013). In a future extension and in larger sample sizes it will be important to inspect brain networks that are recruited to support neurofeedback-enhanced self-regulation and investigate the overlap with neurobiological mechanisms of explicitly instructed cognitive inhibition of craving.

The first EEG neurofeedback training study in addiction compared a neurofeed-back training group with treatment as usual in alcoholics (Peniston and Kulkosky, 1989). Neurofeedback training was comprised of eight sessions relaxation training, “autogenic training,” in combination with temperature biofeedback and subsequently 15 sessions of EEG neurofeedback training. The EEG feedback signal was derived from alpha and theta EEG frequencies, which are associated with a tranquil/calm/serene state of mind (Peniston and Kulkosky, 1989). At the end of training, neurofeedback participants were able to control the feedback signal, showed an increase in alpha and theta frequencies in comparison to baseline measurement and control participants, and reported a reduction in depression symptoms (Peniston and Kulkosky, 1989; Table 1). Furthermore, they relapsed less often during 13 months of follow-up.

The other two EEG neurofeedback training studies combined a motivational intervention with neurofeedback training in cocaine users. The goal of these training studies was to enhance attention during cognitive involvement rather than induce a relaxed state as in the previous study with alcoholics. One study employed a between-subjects design to investigate the effect of adding two sessions of neurofeedback to treatment as usual (Stotts et al., 2006). Feedback was provided from the P300 signal, as an index of cognitive involvement, while participants were attending to visual stimuli. The neurofeedback group showed increased P300 signal feedback after training, and reported higher engagement in adjacent treatment in comparison to the control group following treatment as usual. The other EEG neurofeedback training study employed a pre- and posttraining design to investigate change after 12 sessions of neurofeedback based on a sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) protocol (Horrell et al., 2010). Participants were instructed to increase amplitude of SMR frequency, as high SMR amplitude has been linked to improved attention (Horrell et al., 2010). At the end of training, cocaine users demonstrated increased SMR signal, reported reduction in stress and depression symptoms, and tested collaborated reports of decreased drug use (Table 1).

In summary, while the neurobiological mechanisms of neurofeedback training cannot be critically evaluated due to the diverging approaches, limited sample sizes and low number of studies, the reviewed studies do suggest that learning to self-regulate brain signals has the potential to reduce drug-related behaviors.

3 SUMMARY

Overall, results suggest that imaging is a suitable tool to investigate the mechanisms underlying cognitive and motivational interventions in addiction. Imaging studies have shown that CBT dampens reward sensitivity to drug-related cues, whereas cognitive inhibition and motivational interventions recruit and strengthen the inhibitory control network (Fig. 1). Cognitive inhibition reactivates the chronically hypoactive inhibitory control network, leading to an associated reduction in the reactivity of the brain’s reward circuitry. Motivational interventions enhance self-referential processing, and increase activation in inhibitory control regions. Affect regulation activates an overlapping inhibitory network. The mechanism of neurofeedback training remains to be explored. Interestingly, mindfulness training was the only intervention showing a reduction of activation in craving-related brain systems, without direct involvement of prefrontal control regions.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Brain circuits in cognitive interventions in addiction. Common results across intervention modalities were the normalization of aberrant activity in the brain’s reward circuitry, and the recruitment and strengthening of the brain’s inhibitory control network (regions involved in reward processing are depicted in red (light gray in the print version): e.g., striatum; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; VTA, ventral tegmental area; inhibitory control network is shown in blue (dark gray in the print version): e.g., IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate). During motivational interventions regions involved in self-referential processing and motivated attention were activated (in green (light gray in the print version): mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus).

This pattern of results generally confirms that chronically hypoactive regions implicated in prefrontal control in drug addiction (Goldstein and Volkow, 2011) can be normalized through cognitive and motivational/emotional interventions. Results also demonstrate that different cognitive interventions act, at least partly, through a common mechanism, supporting a previous meta-analysis that posited the recruitment of the inhibitory control network as a shared therapeutic mechanism between cognitive and pharmacological interventions (Konova et al., 2013). Importantly, this conclusion implies that learned coping mechanisms may partially generalize across different methods for skills training. It would therefore be valuable to investigate directly if there are transferable effects between different modalities of training, and to systematically compare different mechanisms within one study sample. Overall, the low number of implemented training protocols warrants further research in all reviewed training modalities.

4 CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Two-thirds of the reviewed treatment studies were performed in smokers. Given this clear gap between research in nicotine addiction as compared to other substance use disorders, this review cannot generalize conclusions to other substance use disorders. Aside from extending results to additional substance abusing populations and optimizing study paradigms, future studies should draw on new analysis methods to investigate the interaction between regions, and understand dynamic reconfigurations within networks. Studies investigating how changes in the recruitment of brain networks over time predict treatment success are necessary. Understanding the reconfiguration of brain networks during cognitive interventions may allow us to build a theoretical model of the relevant neurobiological mechanisms of treatment in addiction. Such a theoretical model may guide the development of effective treatment strategies in substance use disorders in the future.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (grant number 1R21DA034954 to R.Z.G., 1K01DA037452 to S.J.M., and 1F32DA033088 to M.A.P.).

Footnotes

Disclosure/Conflict of Interest: None declared.

References

  1. Albein-Urios N, Verdejo-Román J, Asensio S, Soriano-Mas C, Martínez-González JM, Verdejo-García A. Re-appraisal of negative emotions in cocaine dependence: dysfunctional corticolimbic activation and connectivity. Addict Biol. 2014;19:415–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00497.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00497.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ballard IC, Murty VP, Carter RM, MacInnes JJ, Huettel SA, Adcock RA. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex drives mesolimbic dopaminergic regions to initiate motivated behavior. J Neurosci. 2011;31(28):10340–10346. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0895-11.2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUR-OSCI.0895-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bliss RE, Garvey AJ, Heinold JW, Hitchcock JL. The influence of situation and coping on relapse crisis outcomes after smoking cessation. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989;57(3):443–449. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.3.443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.57.3.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Potenza MN. Pre-treatment brain activation during stroop task is associated with outcomes in cocaine-dependent patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(11):998–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brewer JA, Mallik S, Babuscio TA, Nich C, Johnson HE, Deleone CM, Rounsaville BJ. Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;119(1–2):72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Olmstead RE, Jou J, Tiongson E, Allen V, Cohen MS. Neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(6):642–651. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Canterberry M, Hanlon CA, Hartwell KJ, Li X, Owens M, LeMatty T, George MS. Sustained reduction of nicotine craving with real-time neurofeedback: exploring the role of severity of dependence. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15(12):2120–2124. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntt122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carroll KM. A Cognitive Behavioral Approach: Treating Cocaine Addiction. National Institute on Drug Abuse; Rockville, MD: 1998. Retrieved from, http://www.athealth.com/practitioner/ceduc/cbt_cocaine.html. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129:564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chase HW, Kumar P, Eickhoff SB, Dombrovski AY. Reinforcement learning models and their neural correlates: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2015;15:435–459. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0338-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0338-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cheetham A, Allen NB, Yücel M, Lubman DI. The role of affective dysregulation in drug addiction. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(6):621–634. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chua HF, Liberzon I, Welsh RC, Strecher VJ. Neural correlates of message tailoring and self-relatedness in smoking cessation programming. Biol Psychiatry. 2009a;65(2):165–168. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.030. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.030.Neural. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chua HF, Polk T, Welsh R, Liberzon I, Strecher V. Neural responses to elements of a web-based smoking cessation program. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2009b;144:174–178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chua HF, Ho SS, Jasinska AJ, Polk TA, Welsh RC, Liberzon I, Strecher VJ. Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:426–427. doi: 10.1038/nn.2761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Costello MR, Mandelkern MA, Shoptaw S, Shulenberger S, Baker SK, Abrams AL, Brody AL. Effects of treatment for tobacco dependence on resting cerebral glucose metabolism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(3):605–612. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. D’Argembeau A, Ruby P, Collette F, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Luxen A, Salmon E. Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:935–944. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. DeVito EE, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Kober H, Potenza MN. A preliminary study of the neural effects of behavioral therapy for substance use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;122(3):228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.10.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalc-dep.2011.10.002.A. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, Gruber O. Fear is only as deep as the mind allows. A coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. Neuroimage. 2011;58(1):275–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dutra L, Stathopoulou G, Basden SL, Leyro TM, Powers MB, Otto MW. A meta-analytic review of psychosocial interventions for substance use disorders. Am J Psychiatr. 2008;165:179–187. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06111851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Engelmann JM, Versace F, Robinson JD, Minnix JA, Lam CY, Cui Y, Cinciripini PM. Neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 2012;60(1):252–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Feldstein Ewing SW, Filbey FM, Sabbineni A, Chandler LD, Hutchison KE. How psychosocial alcohol interventions work: a preliminary look at what fMRI can tell us. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35(4):643–651. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01382.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01382.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(11):652–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2(3):271–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hanlon CA, Hartwell KJ, Canterberry M, Li X, Owens M, LeMatty T, George MS. Reduction of cue-induced craving through realtime neurofeedback in nicotine users: the role of region of interest selection and multiple visits. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2013;213(1):79–81. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hartwell KJ, Johnson KA, Li X, Myrick H, Lematty T, George MS, Brady KT. Neural correlates of craving and resisting craving for tobacco in nicotine dependent smokers. Addict Biol. 2011;16(4):654–666. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00340.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00340.x.Neural. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Horrell T, El-Baz A, Baruth J, Tasman A, Sokhadze G, Stewart C, Sokhadze E. Neurofeedback effects on evoked and induced EEG gamma band reactivity to drug-related cues in cocaine addiction. J Neurother. 2010;14(3):195–216. doi: 10.1080/10874208.2010.501498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10874208.2010.501498.Neurofeedback. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Janes AC, Pizzagalli DA, Richardt S, deB Frederick B, Chuzi S, Pachas G, Kaufman MJ. Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(8):722–729. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.034. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kang OS, Chang DS, Jahng GH, Kim SY, Kim H, Kim JW, Chae Y. Individual differences in smoking-related cue reactivity in smokers: an eye-tracking and fMRI study. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2012;38(2):285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.04.013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(33):14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007779107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation—an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Neuroimage. 2014;87:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Konova AB, Moeller SJ, Goldstein RZ. Common and distinct neural targets of treatment: changing brain function in substance addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:2806–2817. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lancaster T, Stead LF. Self-help interventions for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2002;3:CD001118. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Langleben DDD, Loughead JW, Ruparel K, Hakun JG, Busch-Winokur S, Holloway MB, Lerman C. Reduced prefrontal and temporal processing and recall of high “sensation value” ads. Neuroimage. 2009;46(1):219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.062. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. 2014;137(2013):12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Li X, Hartwell KJ, Borckardt J, Prisciandaro JJ, Saladin ME, Morgan PS, George MS. Volitional reduction of anterior cingulate cortex activity produces decreased cue craving in smoking cessation: a preliminary real-time fMRI study. Addict Biol. 2013;18(4):739–748. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00449.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00449.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Luijten M, Machielsen MWJ, Veltman DJ, Hester R, de Haan L, Franken IHA. Systematic review of ERP and fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control and error processing in people with substance dependence and behavioural addictions. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014;39(3):149–169. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130052. http://dx.doi.org/10.1503/jpn.130052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Manber-Ball T, Ramsawh HJ, Campbell-Sills L, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Prefrontal dysfunction during emotion regulation in generalized anxiety and panic disorders. Psychol Med. 2013;43(7):1475–1486. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Moeller SJ, Goldstein RZ. Impaired self-awareness in human addiction: deficient attribution of personal relevance. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(12):635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Moeller SJ, Bederson L, Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ. Neuroscience of inhibition for addiction medicine: from prediction of initiation to prediction of relapse. Brain Res. 2015;223:xxx. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nee DE, Brown JW, Askren MK, Berman MG, Demiralp E, Krawitz A, Jonides J. A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:264–282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. New AS, Fan J, Murrough JW, Liu X, Liebman RE, Guise KG, Charney DS. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of deliberate emotion regulation in resilience and posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(7):656–664. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schuckit MA. Neural activation patterns of methamphetamine-dependent subjects during decision making predict relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:761–768. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Peniston EG, Kulkosky PJ. Alpha-theta brainwave training and beta-endorphin levels in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1989;13(2):271–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1989.tb00325.x. 0145-6008/89/1302-027 1$2.00/0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Potenza MN, Sofuoglu M, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ. Neuroscience of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for addictions. Neuron. 2011;69(4):695–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009.Neuroscience. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rauschecker JP. An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration. Hear Res. 2011;271(1–2):16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.09.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schacht JP, Anton RF, Myrick H. Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: a quantitative meta-analysis and systematic review. Addict Biol. 2012;18:121–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Smedslund G, Berg RC, Hammerstrom KT, Steiro A, Leiknes KA, Dahl HM, Karlsen K. Motivational interviewing for substance abuse. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011;5:CD008063. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008063.pub2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD008063.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sokhadze TM, Cannon RL, Trudeau DL. EEG biofeedback as a treatment for substance use disorders: review, rating of efficacy, and recommendations for further research. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2008;33:1–28. doi: 10.1007/s10484-007-9047-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10484-007-9047-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stotts AL, Potts GF, Ingersoll G, George MR, Martin LE. Preliminary feasibility and efficacy of a brief motivational intervention with psychophysiological feedback for cocaine abuse. Subst Abus. 2006;27(4):9–20. doi: 10.1300/j465v27n04_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Swan GE, Ward MM, Jack LM. Abstinence effects as predictors of 28-day relapse in smokers. Addict Behav. 1996;21(4):481–490. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(95)00070-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(95)00070-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tabibnia G, Creswell JD, Kraynak TE, Westbrook C, Julson E, Tindle HA. Common prefrontal regions activate during self-control of craving, emotion, and motor impulses in smokers. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;5:611–619. doi: 10.1177/2167702614522037. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702614522037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tang YY, Tang R, Posner MI. Brief meditation training induces smoking reduction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(34):13971–13975. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311887110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1311887110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Telang F, Logan J, Jayne M, Swanson JM. Cognitive control of drug craving inhibits brain reward regions in cocaine abusers. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2536–2543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.088. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Volkow ND, Baler RD, Goldstein RZ. Addiction: pulling at the neural threads of social behaviors. Neuron. 2011;69(4):599–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wang AL, Ruparel K, Loughead JW, Strasser AA, Blady SJ, Lynch KG, Langleben DD. Content matters: neuroimaging investigation of brain and behavioral impact of televised anti-tobacco public service announcements. J Neurosci. 2013;33(17):7420–7427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3840-12.2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biotechadv.2011.08.021.Secreted. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Westbrook C, Creswell JD, Tabibnia G, Julson E, Kober H, Tindle HA. Mindful attention reduces neural and self-reported cue-induced craving in smokers. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8:73–84. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr076. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wilson SJ, Sayette MA, Fiez JA. Neural correlates of self-focused and other-focused strategies for coping with cigarette cue exposure. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(2):466–476. doi: 10.1037/a0027055. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Zhao LY, Tian J, Wang W, Qin W, Shi J, Li Q, Lu L. The role of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in the regulation of craving by reappraisal in smokers. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):e43598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043598. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES