Abstract
BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVE
Exposure to food advertisements may cue overeating among children, especially among those genetically predisposed to respond to food cues. We aimed to assess how television food advertisements affect eating in the absence of hunger among children in a randomized trial. We hypothesized that the Fat Mass and Obesity Associated Gene (FTO) rs9939609 single nucleotide polymorphism would modify the effect of food advertisements.
SUBJECTS/METHODS
In this randomized experiment, 200 children aged 9–10 years old were served a standardized lunch and then shown a 34-minute television show embedded with either food or toy advertisements. Children were provided with snack food to consume ad libitum while watching the show and we measured caloric intake. Children were genotyped for rs9939609 and analyses were conducted in the overall sample and stratified by genotype. A formal test for interaction of the food ad effect on consumption by rs9939609 was conducted.
RESULTS
172 unrelated participants were included in this analysis. Children consumed on average 453 (SD=185) kCals during lunch and 482 (SD=274) kCals during the experimental exposure. Children who viewed food advertisements consumed an average of 48 kCals (95% CI: 10, 85; P=0.01) more of a recently advertised food than those who viewed toy advertisements. There was a statistically significant interaction between genotype and food advertisement condition (P for interaction = 0.02), where the difference in consumption of a recently advertised food related to food advertisement exposure increased linearly with each additional FTO risk allele, even after controlling for BMI percentile.
CONCLUSIONS
Food advertisement exposure was associated with greater caloric consumption of a recently advertised food, and this effect was modified by an FTO genotype. Future research is needed to understand the neurological mechanism underlying these associations.
Introduction
Over one third of U.S. children are overweight or obese1 putting them at higher risk for adverse health outcomes.2–4 Food intake must balance with energy expenditure and growth demands in order for children to maintain a healthy body weight. Complex neural regulatory feedback systems monitor available energy stores in the body in order to prompt feeding behaviors to match energy needs.5 While these homeostatic mechanisms play a critical role in maintaining energy balance, non-homeostatic (hedonic) pathways can also drive consumption of highly palatable foods.6 Such foods are known to activate the dopaminergic mesolimbic pathway that is responsible for detecting rewarding stimuli and motivating behaviors to repeat exposure to those stimuli.7 This pathway is also involved in classical conditioning,8 i.e. a learning process where a reward-related stimulus can lead to anticipation and motivation for that reward.9 Our current obesogenic environment is replete with food-related stimuli, or food cues, that may activate reward pathways and motivate overconsumption.
Food advertising is a highly pervasive source of exposure to food cues. The food industry spends $1.79 billion marketing primarily energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods to U.S. children under 11 years old,10 resulting in an average viewing of 15 TV ads per day or 5,500 over each year.11 Given the high prevalence of exposure, it is important to understand whether TV food ads prompt excessive caloric consumption in children.
Randomized studies assessing whether TV food advertising affects consumption in children have had mixed results. Some have shown significantly higher consumption when children view food ads compared to non-food ads.12–16 For example, Harris et al. showed 118 children, ages 7–11 years, two minutes of food or non-food ads embedded in a 14-minute cartoon and provided crackers to consume while watching the show.16 Children who viewed the food ads consumed an average of 28.5 g (133 kCals) more than those shown the non-food ads, even though the crackers were not advertised during the session. Others have not observed a main effect of food ad exposure on consumption;17–21 however, several of these studies reported effects of food ad exposure in subsets of participants (e.g. boys17 and children with maternal encouragement to be thin18), or when looking at specific foods (e.g. celebrity-endorsed foods19).
Genetic factors likely interact with environmental drivers of eating behavior and could affect how individuals respond to environmental cues to eat. A common variant in the Fat Mass and Obesity Associated (FTO) gene was the first genetic factor to be associated with common obesity in large genome-wide association studies.22,23 While the biological mechanism is yet unknown pediatric studies suggest that FTO may decrease satiety responsiveness and lead to excess consumption.24–26 Interestingly, one study (n=24) examined FTO rs9939609 in relation to brain response to food images in adult men.27 The authors reported that, for the participants in a fasted state, FTO homozygous high-risk participants had a significantly greater response to food vs. nonfood images in brain reward regions compared to homozygous low-risk participants. This past research motivated our hypothesis that children with the FTO rs9939609 high-risk allele would have heightened susceptibility to excess consumption after viewing food ads.
In this study, we tested the effect of food ad exposure on cued eating among children enrolled in a randomized trial and further explored whether a common variant in FTO modified that effect.
Subjects and Methods
Participants
We recruited 200 children through community fliers and a contact list from Children’s Hospital at Dartmouth between July 2013 and February 2015 (Figure 1). Inclusion criteria included age 9 or 10 years, English fluency, absence of food allergies/restrictions, and absence of health conditions/medication use that may impact appetite or attention span. One caregiver accompanied each child to the visit. Participants were told that the study focused on how children process visual media. Caregivers and children provided written consent and assent, respectively, and Dartmouth’s Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects approved all study protocols.
Figure 1.
Participant Flow Diagram
To limit the analysis to unrelated children, one sibling from each of 21 sibling pairs was excluded at random using a computerized random number generator. In addition, four children were excluded because they did not report satiety after lunch, and three were excluded because of protocol violations (i.e., health condition potentially affecting appetite, caregiver interaction with child during experiment). The final analysis sample thus consisted of 172 unrelated children. There were no significant differences between included and excluded children in terms of baseline covariates or consumption (data not shown).
Preload Lunch
Children and one parent were schedule for a study appointment at 11:30 am or 12:30 pm. Children were instructed to eat a normal breakfast, but not to eat for the two hours prior to the appointment. During the appointment, children were provided lunch with their choice of main dish (macaroni and cheese, pizza bites, or chicken nuggets with ketchup), along with string cheese, carrots and dressing, apple slices, bread, butter, milk, and water. All meals were 1153–1183 kCals to help ensure that children would eat to satiety. Meals were balanced on macronutrients and contained 552–704 kCals of carbohydrates, 315–405 kCals of fat, and 152–188 kCals of protein. Foods were pre-and post-weighed, and nutrient labels and the USDA National Nutrient Database for Standard Reference28 were used to calculate caloric consumption.
After lunch, each child was asked to assess his/her satiety with a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “I am very hungry” to “I am very full”. Children who reported, “I am very hungry” or “I am a little hungry” were excluded from the analysis.
Food Advertisement Exposure
After lunch, all children viewed a 34-minute TV show (Figure it Out!, Nickelodeon) that included 7.7 minutes of either food or toy ads, along with 3.1 minutes of neutral ads. The products advertised are listed in Table S2.
Eating in the Absence of Hunger
Our outcome measure was caloric consumption after self-reported satiety, i.e., eating in the absence of hunger (EAH),29,30 during the experimental exposure. We provided four snack foods during the experimental exposure: gummy candy (546 kCals), cookies (692 kCals), chocolate (1000 kCals), and cheese puffs (536 kCals). Only the gummy candy was advertised during the food ad condition. Nutritional information for the snack foods is presented in Table S1. Food was pre- and post-weighed, and product nutrition information was used to calculate caloric consumption.
FTO rs9939609 Genotyping
Buccal cell swabs were collected before lunch and stored at room temperature with desiccant capsules (Isohelix, Kent, U.K.). DNA was isolated using DDK-50 isolation kits (Isohelix, Kent, U.K.). Genotyping for rs9939609 was conducted with real-time PCR and Taqman chemistry using the 7500 Fast Real-time instrument (primers and instrument from Thermo Fisher Scientific (Waltham, USA)). All samples were successfully genotyped and there was 100% genotyping consistency among the 10% blinded replicates.
Covariates
We measured children’s weight (to the nearest 0.1 kg) and height (to the nearest 0.5 cm), using a digital scale and stadiometer (Model 597KL, Seca, Hamberg, Germany). We calculated body mass index (BMI) percentile using U.S. Center for Disease Control (CDC) 2000 age- and sex-specific distributions.31 Healthy weight was defined as <85th percentile, overweight was defined as ≥85th – <95th percentile and obese was defined as ≥95th percentile.
Estimated daily energy requirement (EER) was calculated according to Institute of Medicine guidelines using child’s sex, measured height and weight, and caregiver-reported daily average physical activity.32 Caregivers answered, “How much time does your child spend doing physical activity such as running around, climbing, biking, dancing, swimming, playing sports, etc.?” separately for school or weekend days. A single weighted average was created and categorized as “sedentary” for <30, “low active” for 30–<60, “active” for 60–<120, and “very active” for ≥120 min/day.
Caregivers reported their child’s race and ethnicity, their highest level of education, their spouse’s highest level of education, and their household income category. Caregivers also reported their child’s typical number of hours spent watching TV or movies on a weekend day and school day, and responses were used to create a single weighted average. “Parental eating restriction” was calculated as an average of caregiver responses to the restriction subscale questions of the Child Feeding Questionnaire,33 (e.g., “I decide what my child eats” answered on a scale of: 1=disagree, 2=slightly disagree, 3=neutral, 4=slightly agree, and 5=agree).
Statistical Analysis
We compared participant characteristics by study condition using unpaired, 2-tailed t-tests for continuous measures and X2 or Fisher’s Exact tests for categorical measures, as appropriate. Next, we examined unadjusted associations between participant characteristics and caloric consumption during the lunch and EAH phases using linear regression; consumption during the EAH phase was computed for total foods and separately for foods that were and were not advertised during the experiment. We then estimated the effect of the experimental condition on EAH for total foods, advertised food, and non-advertised foods separately using multivariable linear regression adjusted for EER and all covariates related to EAH at the P < 0.10 threshold. We did not include sex and BMI percentile as covariates because they were used to calculate EER; however, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to determine whether adding those variables to the final models influenced findings. To address whether the FTO rs9939609 genotype modified the effect between food ad exposure and EAH, we included a multiplicative interaction term between the exposure and genotype in the adjusted models and used a Wald test to determine the significance of the interaction term.
To assess the robustness of our findings, we repeated the final models after excluding participants who were above the 90th percentile for EAH consumption (>840 kCals). We also performed analyses stratified by sex and weight status, because some previous studies found interactions between food ad exposure and these variables on consumption. We conducted all analyses using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, USA).
Results
The analysis included 172 children who were equally distributed across study conditions (Table 1). Approximately half of the children were male, and they were mostly white (86%) and non-Hispanic (97%). Twenty-three percent of children were overweight or obese, which is slightly lower than the New Hampshire rate of 26%. The mothers of the children were generally highly educated with 78% obtaining at least a college degree. Children watched an average (SD) of 1.4 (1.0) hours of TV per day. Baseline characteristics were balanced across experimental conditions with the exception of EER, which was 145 kCals higher in the toy vs. food arm of the study (P = 0.03).
Table 1.
Selected characteristics of study participants (n=1721).
Variable | Advertisement Condition
|
||
---|---|---|---|
Toy (n=86) | Food (n=86) | P Value2 | |
Age (years), mean (SD) | 9.9 (0.6) | 9.9 (0.6) | 0.62 |
Male, N (%) | 44 (51) | 40 (47) | 0.54 |
Race, N (%) | |||
White | 70 (81) | 78 (91) | 0.08 |
Non-white | 16 (19) | 8 (9) | |
Ethnicity, N (%) | |||
Hispanic | 4 (5) | 2 (2) | 0.68 |
Non-Hispanic | 82 (95) | 84 (98) | |
FTO rs9939609, N (%) | 0.63 | ||
TT | 34 (40) | 28 (33) | |
AT | 39 (45) | 44 (51) | |
TT | 13 (15) | 14 (16) | |
Maternal Education, N (%) | |||
Some High School or High School Diploma | 4 (5) | 1 (1) | 0.53 |
Some Post-High School or Associates Degree | 16 (19) | 14 (16) | |
College Graduate | 22 (26) | 27 (31) | |
Professional or Graduate School | 42 (49) | 43 (50) | |
Household Income, N (%) | |||
< $25,000 | 2 (2) | 3 (3) | 0.61 |
$25,000 – $64,999 | 19 (22) | 19 (22) | |
$65,000 – $144,999 | 40 (47) | 41 (48) | |
$145,000 – $224,999 | 13 (15) | 17 (20) | |
> $225,000 | 12 (14) | 6 (7) | |
Weight Status3, N (%) | |||
Healthy Weight: BMI <85th percentile | 63 (73) | 69 (80) | 0.41 |
Overweight: BMI 85th–94.9th %tile | 8 (9) | 8 (9) | |
Obese: BMI ≥95th percentile | 15 (17) | 9 (10) | |
Moderate/Vigorous Physical Activity (hrs/day), mean (SD) | 2.4 (1.2) | 2.1 (1.2) | 0.15 |
Estimated Daily Energy Requirement (kCals)4, mean (SD) | 2461 (475) | 2316 (412) | 0.03 |
TV and movie viewing (hours/day), mean (SD) | 1.4 (1.1) | 1.3 (0.9) | 0.59 |
Parental Eating Restriction (range: 1–5)5, mean (SD) | 3.0 (0.9) | 3.0 (1.0) | 0.79 |
Participants missing race=1, maternal education=2, TV/movie viewing = 1
Calculated from an upaired 2-tailed t test with an equal variance assumption to analyze the difference in means or a X2 test to analyze the difference in proportions when expected cell counts were above 5 and Fisher’s Exact test when expected cell counts were below 5.
Based on U.S. Centers for Disease Control (CDC) 2000 growth charts31
Calculated using measured height and weight, and reported sex, age, and physical activity level using guidelines from the Institute of Medicine Guidelines32
From the parental restriction subscale of the Child Feeding Questionnaire33
The frequency of FTO rs9939609 genotype frequencies (36% TT, 48% AT, and 16% AA) were similar to those of other studies22 and were consistent with Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (X2 test P value = 0.93). There was a strong relationship between the FTO genotype and adiposity; the rate of overweight/obesity was 18% among low-risk (TT) children compared to 44% among the highest-risk (AA) children (P < 0.01). In our sample, 21% of heterozygotes were overweight/obese, a rate similar to that for homozygous low-risk participants (P = 0.68). The FTO genotype was not associated with any other child, caregiver or household characteristic at the P <0.05 significance level (Table S3).
Participants across both experimental conditions consumed an average (SD) of 453 (185) kCals during lunch. Being male, having a higher BMI, and having a higher EER were associated with higher caloric consumption at lunch (Table 2).
Table 2.
Unadjusted associations between participant characteristics and consumption during lunch preload and in the absence of hunger (n=1721)
Variable |
β̂(95%CI) for consumption (Δ kCals/unit predictor)2
|
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Lunch Preload | Eating in the Absence of Hunger Phase
|
|||
All Foods | Food advertised in session | Foods not advertised in session | ||
Food advertisement exposure | −8 (−64,48) | 19 (−63, 102) | 44 (7, 81) | −24 (−98, 49) |
FTO rs9939609 (per risk allele) | 33 (−7, 73) | 42 (−18, 102) | 17 (−11, 44) | 25 (−28, 79) |
Male | 123 (71, 176) | 116 (35, 197) | 15 (−23, 52) | 102 (29, 174) |
Age in years | 8 (−40, 56) | −35 (−106, 36) | −22 (−54, 10) | −13 (−76, 50) |
Maternal Education3 | 3 (−30, 36) | 12 (−37, 61) | 14 (−9, 36) | −2 (−45, 42) |
Household Income3 | 5 (−24, 34) | −21 (−64, 22) | −5 (−25, 15) | −16 (−55, 22) |
BMI: 10 percentile increase4 | 21 (12, 31) | 35 (21, 49) | 8 (1, 15) | 27 (15, 40) |
Moderate/ Vigorous Physical Activity (Hrs/Day) | 6 (−17, 29) | −28 (−62, 7) | −7 (−22, 9) | −21 (−52, 9) |
Expected Daily Energy Req. (per 100 kCals)5 | 17 (11, 22) | 21 (12, 30) | 2 (1, 6) | 18 (10, 25) |
TV/movie viewing (hours/day) | 9 (−20, 37) | 21 (−20, 62) | 0 (−20, 19) | 21 (−15, 58) |
Parental Eating Restriction (1 pt. increase)6 | 4 (−25, 33) | 51 (9, 94) | 8 (−11, 28) | 43 (5, 81) |
Participants missing race = 1, maternal education = 2, TV/movie viewing = 1
From a series of linear regressions with the food ad exposure or participant characteristics as the independent variable and consumption (all foods, advertised food, or non-advertised foods) as the dependent variable
Treated as ordinal with categories shown in Table 1
Based on U.S. Centers for Disease Control (CDC) 2000 growth charts31
Calculated using Institute of Medicine guidelines32
From the parental restriction subscale of the Child Feeding Questionnaire (range: 1–5) 33
Participants consumed an additional 482 (SD: 274) kCals during the EAH phase. Baseline characteristics associated with greater total EAH consumption were being male, increased BMI percentile, EER and parental eating restriction (Table 2). There was a statistically significant main effect of the food vs. toy ad exposure on the consumption of the food advertised during the session; children exposed to food ads consumed, on average, 44 additional calories of gummy candy than children exposed to toy ads (P = 0.02). There was no main effect of experimental condition on total consumption or on foods not advertised during the experiment.
In analyses adjusted for EER and parental eating restriction (Table 3), the association between food ad exposure and consumption of the advertised food remained similar (β = 48 kCals, P = 0.01). There were no main effects for total consumption (P = 0.21) or consumption of foods not advertised (P = 0.98). We conducted a Bonferonni correction of the significance level for the 3 hypothesis tests of consumption conducted (total food, advertised food, non-advertised food) and the association between food ad exposure and consumption of advertised food was still significant at the P < 0.017 level.
Table 3.
Adjusted associations with consumption in the absence of hunger (n=172)
Adjusted β̂ (95%CI) for consumption (Δ kCals/unit predictor)1 | |||
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Variable | Total consumption | Food advertised during session | Foods not advertised during session |
Food Advertisement Condition | 43 (−34, 120) | 48 (10, 85) | −5 (−74, 65) |
Estimated Daily Energy Requirement (per 100 kCals)2 | 22 (13, 30) | 3 (−1, 8) | 18 (10, 26) |
Parental Eating Restriction (1 pt increase)3 | 50 (10, 90) | 8 (−12, 27) | 42 (6, 78) |
From a series of multiple linear regressions that included the food advertisement condition, estimated daily energy requirement, and parental eating restriction as independent variables and consumption (all foods, advertised food, or nonadvertised foods) as the dependent variable.
Calculated using measured height, weight, and reported sex, age, and physical activity level using guidelines from the Institute of Medicine Guidelines32
From the parental restriction subscale of the Child Feeding Questionnaire (range: 1–5)33
In a sensitivity analysis that also included sex and BMI percentile in the final models, results were not substantially changed. Findings also remained unchanged when the analysis was restricted to individuals below the 90th percentile of EAH consumption. In addition, findings were unchanged in analyses restricted to white participants.
There was a significant interaction between the FTO rs9939609 genotype and food ad exposure with advertised food consumption (P for interaction = 0.02) (Figure 2). The magnitude of the association between food ad exposure and consumption increased linearly with each additional risk allele; the estimate (95% CI) for the TT, AT, and AA genotypes were −3 (−64, 59), 59 (4, 115), and 125 (16, 233) kCals, respectively. Stratum-specific estimates also increased linearly when considering total foods rather than only the advertised food, though the estimates were not significant, nor was the interaction. There were no significant interactions between participant sex or weight status with food ad exposure on consumption (data not shown).
Figure 2.
FTO rs9939609 stratum specific estimates (± standard error bars) of the association between food ad exposure and advertised food consumption after adjustment for estimated daily energy requirement, which was calculated using Institute of Medicine guidelines.32
Discussion
In this randomized study, we observed a significant interaction between the FTO rs9939609 genotype and food ad exposure on the consumption of a recently advertised food. Our results suggest that the FTO obesity-risk allele may confer children with a predisposition to heightened consumption in response to food cues. Although we did not find evidence of a generalized effect of food ad exposure on overall cued eating, we did find that exposure to food ads influenced the consumption of a recently advertised food. During just 34 minutes of TV viewing, children who viewed a show with embedded food ads, including one ad for gummy candy, ate an average of 48 more kCals of gummy candy than children who viewed toy ads. Moreover, that consumption occurred immediately after children reported eating lunch to satiety, reflecting excessive intake. The implications of such findings are concerning given the frequent exposure that children have to TV food ads and that the majority of television food advertising is for energy-dense, nutrient poor foods.34
Food cues such as those present in food advertising are thought to drive non-homeostatic pathways of food consumption via their incentive-motivational properties acquired via classical conditioning.9,35 The nucleus accumbens, part of the dopaminergic reward pathway of the brain, is the likely functional interface between motivational food cues and consumptive behaviors,36 and functional neuroimaging studies have observed activation of the nucleus accumbens in response to food advertisements.37,38 In addition, studies also suggest that the neural reward response to food cues is greater for participants with FTO rs9939609 obesity risk alleles, independent of adiposity.27,38 FTO rs9939609 may regulate dopamine (D2)-dependent reward learning,39 so the increased responsiveness to food cues may be a result of heightened prior conditioning. We hypothesize that genetic differences in the neural reward response to food cues underlies our observed behavioral findings; however, further research is needed to better understand genetic differences in the effect of food cues on overconsumption.
Ten studies performed by five unique research groups have explored measured food consumption in children during or after viewing TV food ads (systematically reviewed by Boyland in 201640).12–21 Of these studies, five reported a main effect of food ad exposure.12–16 A series of school-based experiments in the U.K. found that children consumed more after viewing a TV program proceeded by 8–10 food ads compared to toy ads.13–15 While compelling, the non-naturalistic presentation of ads in a single block before the show may have suggested the study goals to participants and thereby influenced behavior. However, two other studies presented the ads during more naturalistic commercial breaks, as we did in our study, and showed increased consumption related to food ad exposure.12,16 The remaining five studies did not find a main effect of food advertising exposure on consumption.17–21
The fact that the advertising effect was only observed for an advertised food differs from the majority of previous studies on cued eating that have demonstrated that the effect of advertising extends to non-advertised foods, a “beyond-brand” effect.40 Unlike our study, however, most of those previous studies only provided participants with foods that were not advertised during the experimental session and did not also provide them the choice of an advertised food. Only one other study provided participants with the choice between an advertised and non-advertised food; in that study, exposure to an ad for a celebrity-endorsed potato chip increased consumption of that specific chip, but did not impact consumption of chips with a generic label.19 Like our study, that study also did not observe a significant difference in consumption of the non-advertised food across study condition. We do not deem our study findings a contradiction of a beyond-brand effect, but consider them evidence of a preference for consuming advertised food when provided, a behavior that was not captured in most previous studies. The specificity of our food ad effect could also relate to the particular characteristics of the gummy candy, such as macronutrient composition, that differ from those of the other provided foods (cookies, chocolate, cheese puffs). In addition, while only one ad for the gummy candy was shown, six of the 20 ads shown were for candy and those ads may have also influenced the consumption of the gummy candy. Future research is necessary to better understand the observed specificity of the advertising effect on consumption.
The implications of our findings for weight gain are unclear given that we did not find a significant difference in total caloric consumption related to ad exposure, though we did observe a positive trend for the association. Given the high inter-individual variability in total consumption, our study may have been underpowered to test the effect of food ad exposure on this outcome. Unlike four of the five studies that found a significant main effect, our study did not measure consumption in the same children under both conditions. While a within-participant design may have been more powerful, we chose not to use such a design because of our concerns that participant awareness of study goals could influence their behavior. We posited that children would be more likely to remain naïve to the study goals when they were randomized to a single experiment condition. A larger study will be necessary to more definitively assess food ad exposure’s effect on total consumption.
Our finding that parental eating restriction of child eating was positively related to total consumption recapitulate results from other groups.41–43 It is possible that children develop an increased preference for restricted foods.44 Alternatively, parents may impose more restrictive feeding practices in response to a child’s tendency to overeat. Longitudinal studies are necessary to clarify the directionality of this relationship.
This study has the strength of controlling for initial satiety level, thereby enabling us to measure EAH. Indeed, a surprising finding was that children consumed approximately the same number of calories during the EAH and lunch phases, suggesting the inability of children to regulate caloric intake when presented with highly palatable foods. Through this EAH paradigm, we were able to demonstrate that recent food ad exposure prompted children to consume a recently advertised food even when they were full.
Our study was limited in that it measured one instance of cued eating in a laboratory setting, and it is unknown whether children compensate for increased short-term intake by modifying long-term intake, thus mitigating any effects on excess weight gain. Also, the generalizability of our laboratory-based findings to the home environment is unknown. However, studies suggest that children frequently eat snacks while watching TV.45 Furthermore, studies also suggests that TV ads prompt children to request the purchase of advertised foods,46,47 which may relate to an increased availability of those foods at home. Thus, it is plausible that exposure to TV food ads among children at home may indeed relate to cued eating.
While TV is still a primary mechanism for advertising to children,11 food companies are increasingly using other marketing tactics.10 For example, advergames, internet games that promote brand recognition, have been shown to increase short-term caloric consumption in children.48,49 In addition, the use of celebrity endorsements19 and character tie-ins50 is concerning and warrants further research.
Our study was also limited by its examination of a single genetic obesity risk factor, rs9939609. This polymorphism has shown one of the strongest associations with child obesity, however, so understanding potential mechanisms of its action are of both scientific and public health importance. This is also one of the first studies to examine genetic predisposition to reactivity to food cues and the first examination of this research area in children. Our study cannot rule out the possibility that unmeasured confounders, like other genetic loci related to excess consumption, were unbalanced between study arms and could have contributed to the association we report. Future, larger studies are necessary to study multiple genetic obesity risk factors related to food cue reactivity.
Conclusion
In this randomized experimental trial, exposure to TV food ads was associated with increased caloric consumption of a recently advertised food in children who had already eaten a meal to satiety, and that association was modified by the FTO rs9939609 obesity-risk allele. Future research is needed to understand the neurological mechanism underlying these associations and whether other genetic risk factors also influence reactivity to food cues. Given the high exposure that children have to ads marketing unhealthy foods, the observed cued eating may have a substantial impact on children’s dietary choices.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions: Dr. Gilbert-Diamond had full access to the data and takes full responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: Gilbert-Diamond, Lansigan, Kelley, Heatherton, Sargent
Acquisition of data: Gilbert-Diamond, Lansigan, Rapuano
Analysis and interpretation of data: Gilbert-Diamond, Emond, Sargent
Drafting of the manuscript: Gilbert-Diamond, Emond
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors
Statistical Analysis: Gilbert-Diamond, Emond
Obtaining Funding: Gilbert-Diamond, Heatherton, Sargent
Administrative, technical, or material support: Lansigan
Study Supervision: Gilbert-Diamond
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant R21HD076097 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NIH) (Dr. Gilbert-Diamond); by grant R01DA022582 from the National Institute of Drug Addiction (NIH) (Heatherton); by grant R01CA077026 from the National Cancer Institute (NIH) (Sargent); by grant R01AA021347 from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIH) (Sargent); by grant NIHGMS P20GM104416 from the NIH (Dr. Karagas); by grant ES022832 from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIH) (Dr. Karagas); and by grant RD83544201 from the Environmental Protection Agency (Dr. Karagas).
Role of the Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: Participating children and their caregivers enabled this research study to happen. Horacio Romero Castillo, Archana Ramanujam and other study staff members contributed significantly to running this study. Margaret R. Karagas, Department of Epidemiology, Geisel School of Medicine at Dartmouth, obtained additional funding for this study.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Declaration
The authors declare they have no actual or potential competing financial interests related to the described work.
Supplementary information is available at The International Journal of Obesity’s website.
References
- 1.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity in the United States. JAMA. 2014;312(2):189–190. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.6228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dietz WH. Overweight in childhood and adolescence. N Engl J Med. 2004;350(9):855–857. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp048008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Turer CB, Lin H, Flores G. Health status, emotional/behavioral problems, health care use, and expenditures in overweight/obese US children/adolescents. Acad Pediatr. 2013;13(3):251–258. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2013.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Guo SS, Chumlea WC. Tracking of body mass index in children in relation to overweight in adulthood. Am J of Clin Nutr. 1999;70(1):145S–8S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/70.1.145s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Guyenet SJ, Schwartz MW. Regulation of Food Intake, Energy Balance, and Body Fat Mass: Implications for the Pathogenesis and Treatment of Obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(3):745–755. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lutter M, Nestler EJ. Homeostatic and Hedonic Signals Interact in the Regulation of Food Intake. Journal of Nutrition. 2009;139(3):629–632. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.097618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Modulation of feeding-induced activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by appetitive stimuli and its relation to motivational state. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11(12):4389–4397. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00843.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bassareo V, Cucca F, Musio P, Lecca D, Frau R, Di Chiara G. Nucleus accumbens shell and core dopamine responsiveness to sucrose in rats: role of response contingency and discriminative/conditioned cues. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(6):802–809. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pavlov IP. Experimental psychology and psycho-pathology in animals. Lectures on conditioned reflexes: Twenty-five years of objective study of the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals. 1928;(001):47–60. doi: 10.1037/11081-001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Federal Trade Commission. A Review of Food Marketing to Children and Adolescents: Follow-Up Report. Washington: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 11.The Rudd Center for Food Policy. Trends in Television Food Advertising. New Haven, CT: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dovey TM, Taylor L, Stow R, Boyland EJ, Halford JCG. Responsiveness to healthy television (TV) food advertisements/commercials is only evident in children under the age of seven with low food neophobia. Appetite. 2011;56(2):440–446. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Halford JCG, Gillespie J, Brown V, Pontin EE, Dovey TM. Effect of television advertisements for foods on food consumption in children. Appetite. 2004;42(2):221–225. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2003.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Halford JCG, Boyland EJ, Hughes G, Oliveira LP, Dovey TM. Beyond-brand effect of television (TV) food advertisements/commercials on caloric intake and food choice of 5–7-year-old children. Appetite. 2007;49(1):263–267. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Halford JCG, Boyland EJ, Hughes GM, Stacey L, McKean S, Dovey TM. Beyond-brand effect of television food advertisements on food choice in children: the effects of weight status. PHN. 2008;11(09):897–904. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007001231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Harris JL, Bargh JA, Brownell KD. Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior. Health Psychol. 2009;28(4):404–413. doi: 10.1037/a0014399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Anschutz DJ, Engels RCME, Van Strien T. Side effects of television food commercials on concurrent nonadvertised sweet snack food intakes in young children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89(5):1328–1333. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Anschutz DJ, Engels RCME, Van Strien T. Maternal encouragement to be thin moderates the effect of commercials on children’s snack food intake. Appetite. 2010;55(1):117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Boyland EJ, Harrold JA, Dovey TM, et al. Food choice and overconsumption: effect of a premium sports celebrity endorser. J Pediatr. 2013;163(2):339–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.01.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Anderson GH, Khodabandeh S, Patel B, Luhovyy BL, Bellissimo N, Mollard RC. Mealtime exposure to food advertisements while watching television increases food intake in overweight and obese girls but has a paradoxical effect in boys. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2015;40(2):162–167. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2014-0249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gorn GJ, Goldberg ME. Children’s responses to repetitive television commercials. Journal of Consumer Research. 1980;6(4):421–424. doi: 10.2307/2488743. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Frayling TM, Timpson NJ, Weedon MN, et al. A common variant in the FTO gene is associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. SCIENCE- …. 2007;316(5826):889–894. doi: 10.1126/science.1141634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Loos RJF, Yeo GSH. The bigger picture of FTO-the first GWAS-identified obesity gene. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2014;10(1):51–61. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2013.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tanofsky-Kraff M, Han JC, Anandalingam K, et al. The FTO gene rs9939609 obesity-risk allele and loss of control over eating. Am J of Clin Nutr. 2009;90(6):1483–1488. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wardle J, Carnell S, Haworth CMA, Farooqi IS, O’Rahilly S, Plomin R. Obesity associated genetic variation in FTO is associated with diminished satiety. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2008;93(9):3640–3643. doi: 10.1210/jc.2008-0472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wardle J, Sanderson S. The FTO gene and measured food intake in children. International Journal of Obesity. 2009;33(1):42–45. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Karra E, O’Daly OG, Choudhury AI, et al. A link between FTO, ghrelin, and impaired brain food-cue responsivity. J Clin Invest. 2013;123(8):3539–3551. doi: 10.1172/JCI44403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Agriculture USDO. USDA National Nutrient Database for Standard Reference. 2014 Aug; http://www.ars.usda.gov/Services/docs.htm?docid=8964.
- 29.Fisher JO, Birch L. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am J of Clin Nutr. 2002;76(1):226–231. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/76.1.226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lansigan RK, Emond JA, Gilbert-Diamond D. Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: A systematic review of existing studies. Appetite. 2015;85(C):36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. CDC Growth Charts: United States. Washington, DC: 2000. http://www.cdc.gov/growthcharts/ [Google Scholar]
- 32.Institute of Medicine (U.S.). Panel on Macronutrients, Institute of Medicine (U.S.) Dietary Reference Intakes for Energy, Carbohydrate, Fiber, Fat, Fatty Acids, Cholesterol, Protein, and Amino Acids. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 2005. Standing Committee on the Scientific Evaluation of Dietary Reference Intakes. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Birch L, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36(3):201–210. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Powell LM, Schermbeck RM, Chaloupka FJ. Nutritional content of food and beverage products in television advertisements seen on children’s programming. Child Obes. 2013;9(6):524–531. doi: 10.1089/chi.2013.0072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Berthoud H-R. Homeostatic and non-homeostatic pathways involved in the control of food intake and energy balance. Obesity. 2006;14(Suppl 5 S8):197S–200S. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zahm DS. An integrative neuroanatomical perspective on some subcortical substrates of adaptive responding with emphasis on the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24(1):85–105. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00065-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rapuano KM, Huckins JF, Sargent JD, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Individual Differences in Reward and Somatosensory-Motor Brain Regions Correlate with Adiposity in Adolescents. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(6):2602–2611. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rapuano KM, Kelley WM, Sargent JD, Heatherton TF, Gilbert-Diamond D. Genetic risk for obesity predicts nucleus accumbens size and responsivity to real-world food cues. 2016 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605548113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sevgi M, Rigoux L, Kühn AB, et al. An Obesity-Predisposing Variant of the FTO Gene Regulates D2R-Dependent Reward Learning. J Neurosci. 2015;35(36):12584–12592. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1589-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Boyland EJ, Nolan S, Kelly B, et al. Advertising as a cue to consume: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of acute exposure to unhealthy food and nonalcoholic beverage advertising on intake in children and adults. Am J of Clin Nutr. 2016;103(2):519–533. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.120022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Birch L, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J of Clin Nutr. 2003;78(2):215–220. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/78.2.215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch L, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res. 2004;12(11):1711–1722. doi: 10.1038/oby.2004.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ventura AK, Birch L. Does parenting affect children’s eating and weight status? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5(1):15. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fisher JO, Birch L. Restricting access to palatable foods affects children’s behavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J of Clin Nutr. 1999;69(6):1264–1272. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/69.6.1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Matheson DM, Killen JD, Wang Y, Varady A, Robinson TN. Children’s food consumption during television viewing. Am J of Clin Nutr. 2004;79(6):1088–1094. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/79.6.1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.AKTAŞ ARNAS Y. The effects of television food advertisement on children’s food purchasing requests. Pediatrics International. 2006;48(2):138–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2006.02180.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Galst JP, White MA. The Unhealthy Persuader: The Reinforcing Value of Television and Children’s Purchase-Influencing Attempts at the Supermarket. Child Dev. 1976;47(4):1089. doi: 10.2307/1128446. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Folkvord F, Anschutz DJ, Buijzen M, Valkenburg PM. The effect of playing advergames that promote energy-dense snacks or fruit on actual food intake among children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97(2):239–245. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.047126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Folkvord F, Anschutz DJ, Nederkoorn C, Westerik H, Buijzen M. Impulsivity, “advergames,” and food intake. Pediatrics. 2014;133(6):1007–1012. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Harris JL, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Marketing foods to children and adolescents: licensed characters and other promotions on packaged foods in the supermarket. PHN. 2010;13(03):409–417. doi: 10.1017/S1368980009991339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.