Abstract
Prior knowledge speeds up system consolidation and accelerates integration of newly acquired memories into existing neocortical knowledge networks. By using targeted memory reactivations, we demonstrate that prior knowledge is also essential for successful reactivation and consolidation of memories during sleep, both on the behavioral and oscillatory level (i.e., theta and fast spindle activity). Thus, prior knowledge is a prerequisite for new memories to enter processes of system consolidation during sleep.
System memory consolidation refers to a long-lasting process whereby a newly encoded labile hippocampal memory trace becomes stabilized and permanently stored in the neocortex1,2,3. This process can be speeded up when neocortical networks of associated knowledge exist into which the newly acquired memory trace can be incorporated4,5,6,7. It has been assumed that the overlapping reactivation of new memory traces in the hippocampus and associated neocortical networks during sleep enables the integration of new information into existing neocortical networks8,9. Thus, memory reactivations should most efficiently promote the consolidation of new memories when neocortical networks of prior knowledge are present. However, this has not been tested so far. The present study aims to start filling this gap by investigating the effects of targeted memory reactivation on the consolidation of newly acquired memories that either can or cannot be related to prior knowledge.
Results
The effect of memory cueing on retention performance
Participants learnt associations between pseudo-words (e.g. “Wiemel”) and pictures showing familiar objects (e.g. a watering can; i.e. stimuli that are related to prior knowledge, “PriorKnow”) or novel objects (stimuli that are not related to prior knowledge, ”noPriorKnow”; see Fig. 1 for a description of the procedure and of the task). Half of the words from both conditions were presented again via loudspeakers during subsequent NonREM (Rapid Eye Movement) sleep. This method also referred to as “memory cueing” is known to induce the reactivation of associated memories10,11,12. In the morning after sleep, memory was tested for all picture-word associations. In the evening before sleep, participants showed higher learning performance for stimuli related to prior knowledge as compared to stimuli not related to prior knowledge (t(15) = 2.86; P = 0.012; Fig. 2a). Cueing during sleep improved later recall with this critically depending on the existence of prior knowledge. More specifically, memory recall was higher for cued as compared to uncued stimuli when prior knowledge was available (t(15) = 2.14, P = 0.05; Fig. 2b). However, no such improvement was present without prior knowledge (P > 0.41; interaction ‘cueing’ x ‘condition’: F(1,15) = 5.21; P = 0.037). Importantly, performance at encoding was not associated with the beneficial effect of memory cueing neither for stimuli related to prior knowledge (r = −0.26, P > 0.33) nor for stimuli not related to prior knowledge (r = −0.07, P > 0.79; Fig. 2c).
Oscillatory correlates of memory cueing during sleep
In a next step, we aimed to uncover the underlying neurophysiological correlates of the beneficial effect of cueing for stimuli related to prior knowledge but not for stimuli not related to prior knowledge. To analyze the induced oscillatory responses to cues during post-learning periods of sleep, we calculated the subsequent memory effect (SME,13), i.e. the difference in the induced oscillatory response to cues associated with later remembered and cues associated with later forgotten stimuli in both conditions. Based on previous findings demonstrating a role of theta and fast spindle activity in the successful reactivation of memories14,15, statistical analyses were restricted to these frequency bands. The SME differed between stimuli related to prior knowledge and stimuli not related to prior knowledge in the fast spindle frequency range (14–16 Hz; Fig. 3a) and in the theta frequency range (5–8 Hz; Fig. 3b). More specifically, the SME did significantly differ between both conditions in an electrode cluster over the right fronto-temporal cortex in the fast spindle frequency band, 1100–1300 ms after cue onset (P = 0.039, corrected for multiple comparisons; Fig. 3c) and in a cluster over the centro-parietal cortex in the theta frequency band, 500–800 ms after cue onset (P = 0.035, corrected for multiple comparisons; Fig. 3d). Later remembered as compared to later forgotten induced higher theta as well as fast spindle activity for stimuli related to prior knowledge (theta: t(15) = 2.48, P = 0.026; spindle: t(15) = 2.55, P = 0.022; Fig. 3e,f) but not for stimuli not related to prior knowledge (theta: t(15) = −1.82, P > 0.09; spindle: t(15) = −0.69, P = 0.50). Moreover, later remembered information related to prior knowledge induced a higher response than later remembered information not related to prior knowledge in both frequency bands (theta: t(15) = 2.22, P = 0.043; spindle: t(15) = 3.23, P = 0.006). Finally, fast spindle but not theta activity in response to later remembered information related to prior knowledge was correlated with the beneficial effect of cueing (spindle: r = 0.51, P = 0.042, theta: r = 0.32, P = 0.22; Fig. 3g,h).
Discussion
Here, we show for the first time that the existence of prior knowledge is an essential prerequisite for the beneficial effect of cueing on memory consolidation during sleep. More specifically, we found that cueing of newly acquired information benefits their later recall only if the cued memories can be related to prior knowledge. Our findings on increased theta and fast spindle activity in response to memory cues related to prior knowledge are in line with recent findings demonstrating that both oscillatory events are related to the successful reactivation of memories14,15. Importantly, they go beyond these earlier findings in showing that the increase in theta and spindle activity critically depends on the existence of prior knowledge. The temporal and topographical distribution of both oscillatory events indicates their unique role in the successful reactivation of memories. The earlier theta response to word cues over the parietal cortex might reflect the immediate reinstatement of the memory trace in the brain region of initial storage16,17. The later spindle response over frontotemporal regions which predicted the cueing-induced memory benefit might reflect the successful integration of new information into these neocortical networks and their stabilization17,18,19,20. Future studies need to experimentally manipulate theta and spindle activity in response to memory cues with different level of prior knowledge in order to empirically test the exact role of these oscillations in the reinstatement and the integration of these memories into the existing memory networks. In sum, our findings provide first empirical evidence for recent theoretical assumptions1,8,9 indicating that overlapping reactivations of newly acquired hippocampal memory traces together with associated neocortical networks of prior knowledge facilitate the integration of new information into these memory networks in the process of memory consolidation.
Participants remembered a higher number of words associated with a familiar object than words associated with a novel object. This is in line with a number of previous studies demonstrating that the existence of prior knowledge benefits the encoding of new memories21,22,23,24. Can these differences at encoding account for the effect of memory cueing merely for information that can be related to prior knowledge? First studies indeed point towards the notion that factors acting during encoding essentially modulate the effect of memory cueing. More specifically, performance level at encoding was found to be positively associated with the beneficial effect of memory cueing14,25. However, in our study the correlation between performance level at encoding and the cueing-induced benefit was not significant. Nevertheless to finally exclude any impact of differences at encoding, future studies should 1) experimentally manipulate or at least control factors acting at the time of encoding that come along with prior knowledge such as the depth of encoding and 2) experimentally induce familiarity within an experiment by using the same kind of novel stimuli.
Since we did not include a wake control group, i.e. a group in which memory cueing took place during wakefulness one could argue that the cueing effects are not limited to the sleep period. A number of previous studies showed no or even detrimental effects of cueing on memory consolidation including various kinds of wake retention intervals such as cueing during daytime wakefulness and cueing during night-time active or passive waking10,14,26,27. The reactivation of memories during sleep and wakefulness is assumed to result in the destabilization of these memories26,28. Due to the presence of slow oscillations, sleep spindles, a low cholingeric tone and the lack of interference these labile memory traces are again strengthened during sleep3. During wakefulness, all these factors are absent which on the one hand can explain the vulnerability of these memories to interference and on the other hand can explain why memory cueing during wakefulness is not effective. Thus, it appears to be very unlikely that cueing during sleep and wakefulness lead to comparable effects on the consolidation of newly acquired memories with and without prior knowledge.
Methods
Participants
Sixteen adults between 18–25 yrs (M = 20.25, SEM = 1.95 yrs; 11 females) participated in the study. Participants were recruited via advertisements placed at the university, at the children’s hospital and in local newspapers. Interviews as well as standardized questionnaires ensured that the participants had no behavioural problems, cognitive impairments or sleep disorders. More specifically, we assessed in a telephone screening interview (for recruitment) and in a more elaborate interview (at the beginning of the adaption night) the information of life-time and current irregularities in sleeping behaviour, learning behaviour and general diseases. All participants were asked for individual sleep habits, i.e., usual time to go to bed, time getting up, etc. in order to schedule both nights in the sleep laboratory in accordance with their usual sleep habits. Also, participants took notes on their daily activities, sleep time and time to get up in a sleep diary for the seven days prior to the experiment which was additionally compared to and supplemented by actigraphical monitoring. Participants did not take any medication at the time of the experiment, and the ingestion of caffeine or alcohol was not allowed on experimental days. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Kantonale Ethikkommission Zürich) and procedures were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines. Participants gave written informed consent before participating.
Design and Procedure
Participants were adapted to polysomnographic recordings on a night preceding the experiment proper. Experimental and adaptation night in the sleep lab were separated by at least one night at home to exclude possible effects of bad sleep quality in the adaptation night on sleep in the experimental night. In the experimental night, participants came to the sleep lab around 2.5 hrs before participants’ habitual bedtime. First, the electrode net was placed. Immediately thereafter, participants performed the learning task (including learning and immediate recall of picture-word associations) and went to bed afterwards. During NonREM sleep, half of the learnt words were presented via loudspeaker placed behind the participants’ head to stimulate the reactivation of the respective picture-word associations. The next morning, participants were awakened at their habitual wake time. Memory retention was tested ~45 min later to avoid any modulating effect of sleep inertia on recall performance (see Fig.1 for the experimental procedure).
Associative memory task
The associative memory task included forty pairs of objects (20 familiar objects and 20 novel objects) and pseudo-words (see Fig. 1 for example of pairs). All pseudo-words were disyllabic, phonotactically legal in German, were stressed on the first syllable as common in German, had a consonant–vowel onset, and had typical masculine or neuter endings. They were spoken slowly by a young woman with a mean duration of 809 ms per word, are digitized at a rate of 44.1 kHz, and were presented through a loudspeaker with an intensity of approximately 65 dB sound pressure level during learning. In the learning session, participants were instructed to memorize each picture-word pair for recall immediately after learning and for later recall the next morning. All picture-word pairs were presented three times in a randomized order. In the first two presentation trials (without feedback) every picture was shown on the screen for a total of 4000 ms. The associated word was additionally presented acoustically via loudspeakers 1500 ms after picture onset. Before presenting each picture, a fixation cross was shown on the screen for 800–1200 ms. In the third learning trial, all pictures were presented again on the screen for 2500 ms, followed by a question mark indicating that the participants had to recall the associated word. Thereafter, the correct word was presented again and participants had the possibility to memorize the picture-word pair for another 5500 ms before the next picture was presented. This last learning trial was followed by a cued recall test which was comparable to the last learning trial except for the fact that no feedback was given. In case of not reaching the criterion of 16 correct responses in this cued recall test, two additional learning trials without feedback and another cued recall test were administered. Memory retention was again tested in the morning in the same cued recall procedure. In all recall tests, participants were required to speak out loud the remembered word to the experimenter who stood next to them and who recorded memory performance. All words that were identical with the learnt word or included the word-stem of the learnt word were counted as correct. A second experimenter double-checked after the task was completed whether the first experimenter had correctly judged the accuracy of each word recall. As a measure of memory consolidation, we calculated the percentage of performance at recall with performance during learning set to 100% (110% means that a participant remembered 10% more words at recall than during learning).
Memory cueing during sleep
During post-learning periods of NonREM sleep (sleep stages N2 and N3), words that had been paired with a picture during the learning phase were presented again via loudspeaker (with a 55 dB sound pressure level). Half of the learnt words was cued, whereas the other half of the learnt words was not cued. Half of the cued words were randomly and individually chosen from the group of words that had been correctly remembered (“hits”) by an individual during the learning phase, whereas the other half was taken from the group of forgotten words (“misses”). For example, given that a participant correctly remembered twenty of the forty picture-word associations - half of them from each of the two categories - this would result in cueing of five correctly remembered stimuli related to prior knowledge, five forgotten stimuli related to prior knowledge, five correctly remembered stimuli not related to prior knowledge and five forgotten stimuli not related to prior knowledge. Words were presented with an inter-stimulus-interval of 6 s with a random jitter of 0 to 0.4 s at a maximum of 22 times for each word (mean number of cues related to prior knowledge: 204.69 ± 11.07, cues not related to prior knowledge: 205.31 ± 11.08; P > 0.56). Note that including the number of cue presentations as a covariate does not change the results (interaction ‘cueing’ x ‘condition’: P = 0.047). A jitter was included in order to ensure that cues reach the slow waves at variable points. Slow waves have been found to be causally related to memory consolidation during sleep29,30. Whether slow waves are also implicated in the stabilization of reactivated memories and whether presenting the cues at a specific phase of the slow wave (either up- or down state) is relevant in this regard and has previously been discussed17 but was not empirically tested so far. On this background, it appeared to be most reasonable to increase the variability of the temporal relationship between slow waves and auditory cues by including a jitter. An experienced experimenter inspected the EEG in real-time to determine sleep stages and to detect any indicator of an arousal. Word cueing was started when a participant had spent more than 10 min in NonREM stage N3, and it was immediately stopped whenever any sign of an arousal, waking up or any other change in sleep stages was observed by the experimenter. The acoustic presentation of word cues did not disturb nocturnal sleep (see Table 1 for descriptives on sleep parameters).
Table 1. Sleep parameters.
Absolute time (in min) | Percentage of time (in %) | |
---|---|---|
mean ± SEM | mean ± SEM | |
Wake | 26.94 ± 5.91 | 6.04 ± 1.29 |
Non REMS N1 | 37.50 ± 4.94 | 8.41 ± 1.09 |
Non REMS N2 | 232.69 ± 11.82 | 52.60 ± 2.58 |
Non REMS N3 | 58.06 ± 7.05 | 13.10 ± 1.56 |
REMS | 87.50 ± 6.25 | 19.86 ± 1.50 |
TST | 442.69 ± 5.72 |
Sleep parameters for all participants are given in mean ± SEM of absolute time in minutes and percentage of total sleep time; REMS = rapid eye movement sleep, TST = total sleep time.
Sleep EEG
Sleep in the experimental and the adaptation night was recorded using high-density sleep EEG (Electrical Geodesics Sensor Net for long-term monitoring, 128 channels, referenced to a vertex electrode) including electromyographic and electrooculographic recordings. Data were sampled at 500 Hz (0.01–200 Hz). For sleep scoring, the EEG was bandpass filtered (0.5–50 Hz) and downsampled to 128 Hz. The EEG was visually scored by two trained individuals for sleep stages Wake, N1, N2, N3 and REM sleep at frontal, central and occipital electrodes (20 s epochs) based on American Academy of Sleep Medicine standard criteria31.
Analyses of power changes in response to cues
EEG signals were preprocessed using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany). Preprocessing included re-referencing of the raw EEG data to the average of the two mastoids and low-pass (30 Hz, roll-off 24 dB per octave) and high-pass filtering (0.1 Hz, roll-off 12 dB per octave). EEG data were epoched into segments of 1.9 s before to 2.9 s after word onset. For each individual, segments were categorized in stimuli that were and stimuli that were not remembered in the test session in the morning (i.e. referred to as “later remembered” and “later forgotten” stimuli). Succeeding EEG oscillatory analyses were performed using the open source Fieldtrip toolbox32 running on Matlab R2014a (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Time-frequency analysis was computed for each trial by using a 7-cycle Morlet wavelet decomposition, ranging from 2 to 30 Hz in 0.5 Hz steps. A sliding window with a step size of 10 ms was applied across the entire length of the epochs. Single trials were normalized with respect to a pre-stimulus time window ranging from −1.000 ms to −100 ms.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analysis of memory retention was based on 2 × 2 analyses of variance (ANOVA) including the repeated measures factors ‘cueing’ (cued, uncued) and ‘condition’ (stimuli related to prior knowledge “PriorKnow”, stimuli not related to prior knowledge “noPriorKnow”). Post-hoc comparisons as well as all statistical comparisons with only one level were performed using student t-tests. Correlation analyses were conducted using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. The level of significance was set to P ≤ 0.05. Statistical analyses of the EEG data were performed with a nonparametric randomization test using cluster correction33 as implemented in FieldTrip. The cluster alpha was set to 0.05 and 500 randomizations were conducted for all tests. Clusters were considered significant at P < 0.05 (two-sided).
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Groch, S. et al. Prior knowledge is essential for the beneficial effect of targeted memory reactivation during sleep. Sci. Rep. 7, 39763; doi: 10.1038/srep39763 (2017).
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Barbara Suter for her support with data acquisition and data analysis, Flavia Wehrle and Angelina Maric for helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript and Manuela Friedrich for providing parts of the stimulus material. This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (Nr. 320030_153387 and P2ZHP1_164994), the Child Research Centre of the Children’s University Hospital, Zürich, Forschungskredit of the University of Zürich, grant no [NR FK-14-044] and the CRPP Sleep and Health. The founding sources were not involved in data collection, analysis, interpretation or the decision to submit the article for publication.
Footnotes
Author Contributions S.G., B.R., R.H. and I.W. designed the experiment; S.G., T.S. and I.W. analyzed the data; S.G., I.W., R.H. wrote manuscript. All authors commented on the manuscript.
References
- Nadel L., Hupbach A., Gomez R. & Newman-Smith K. Memory formation, consolidation and transformation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1640–5 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankland P. W. & Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 119–130 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasch B. & Born J. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse D. et al. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science 316, 76–82 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse D. et al. Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science 333, 891–895 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kesteren M. T., Fernández G., Norris D. G. & Hermans E. J. Persistent schema-dependent hippocampal-neocortical connectivity during memory encoding and postencoding rest in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 7550–7555 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer T. The Emergence of Knowledge and How it Supports the Memory for Novel Related Information. Cereb. Cortex Feb 23, 10.1093/cercor/bhw031 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis P. & Durrant S. Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 343–351 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inostroza M. & Born J. Sleep for preserving and transforming episodic memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 79–102 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasch B., Büchel C., Gais S. & Born J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science 315, 1426–1429 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bendor D. & Wilson M. A. Biasing the content of hippocampal replay during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1439–44 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudoy J. D., Voss J. L., Westerberg C. E. & Paller K. A. Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep. Science 326, 1079 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paller K. A. & Wagner A. D. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 93–192 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiner T. & Rasch B. Boosting Vocabulary Learning by Verbal Cueing During Sleep. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4169–4179 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiner T., Lehmann M. & Rasch B. Auditory feedback blocks memory benefits of cueing during sleep. Nat. Commun. 6, 8729; 10.1038/ncomms9729 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyhus E. & Curran T. Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1023–1035 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiner T. & Rasch B. The beneficial role of memory reactivation for language learning during sleep: A review. Brain Lang. Mar 29, 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.02.005 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamminen J., Payne J. D., Stickgold R., Wamsley E. J. & Gaskell M. G. Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. J Neurosci. 30, 14356–14360 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamminen J., Lambon Ralph M. A. & Lewis P. A. The role of sleep spindles and slow-wave activity in integrating new information in semantic memory. J Neurosci. 33, 15376–15381 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennies N., Lambon Ralph M. A., Kempkes M., Cousins J. N. & Lewis P. A. Sleep Spindle Density Predicts the Effect of Prior Knowledge on Memory Consolidation. J Neurosci. 36, 3799–810 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Kesteren M. T. R., Ruiter D. J., Fernández G. & Henson R. N. How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends in Neurosci. 35, 211–219 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh V. E. & Gilboa A. What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53, 104–114 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brod G., Werkle-Bergner M. & Shing Y. L. The influence of prior knowledge on memory: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Front Behav Neurosci. 7, 139. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00139 (2013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alba J. W. & Hasher L. Is Memory Schematic? Psychological Bulletin 93, 203–231 (1983). [Google Scholar]
- Creery J. D., Oudiette D., Antony J. W. & Paller K. A. Targeted Memory Reactivation during Sleep Depends on Prior Learning. Sleep 38, 755–63 (2015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diekelmann S., Büchel C., Born J. & Rasch B. Labile or stable: opposing consequences for memory when reactivated during waking and sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 381–6 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiner T. & Rasch B. Cueing vocabulary in awake subjects during the day has no effect on memory. Somnologie-Schlafforsch. und Schlafmedizin 19, 133–140 (2015). [Google Scholar]
- Nader K. & Hardt O. A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 224–34 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall L., Helgadottir H., Mölle M. & Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444, 610–613 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ngo H. V., Martinetz T., Born J. & Mölle M. Auditory closed-loop stimulation of the sleep slow oscillation enhances memory. Neuron 78, 545–553 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iber C., Ancoli-Israel S., Chesson A. & Quan S. F. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: Rules, terminology and technical specifications (1st ed.). Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2007). [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R., Fries P., Maris E. & Schoffelen J. M. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris E. & Oostenveld R. J. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]