Abstract
Background
We examined the association between cognitive domains and research consent capacity in PD. Our hypothesis was that research consent capacity is best predicted by executive function.
Methods
A cohort of 90 PD patients and 30 normal older adults were administered the MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Clinical Research, Dementia Rating Scale-2, and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Experts classified patients as either “capable” or “not capable” of providing informed consent to participate in two clinical trials.
Results
MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Clinical Research Reasoning scores for both clinical trial types were most associated with executive functions and delayed recall. As scores on these domains improved, the odds of an expert rating of “capable of consent” increased.
Conclusions
These results extend our previous findings by demonstrating that memory and executive abilities appear closely associated with capacity when evaluated using either a structured interview or expert judgment of that interview.
Keywords: Parkinson's disease, decision-making, capacity, cognitive impairment, informed consent
Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease (PD),1 in particular cognitive changes,2 may have significant impact on patient quality of life and daily function, including the ability to make decisions.3 Using two research scenarios to examine the capacity to consent to research, we found that PD patients with normal cognition generally show preserved decisional abilities while patients with borderline range cognitive impairment had clinically significant deficits in their decisional abilities.4 Although such associations between global cognition and capacity are informative, the cognitive domains that underpin these decision-making impairments remain not well understood.
Given their relevance for goal-directed behaviors, executive functions (i.e., attention, planning, organizing, working memory, and inhibitory control) have been linked to a number of everyday life contexts in PD, including motor control, multitasking, medication management, and driving (see 5 for review). Thus, the goal of the current study was to build on our previous study 4 by examining relationships between cognitive domain performances and research consent capacity. The hypothesis for the current investigation was that research consent capacity in PD is best predicted by executive function. We used the MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Clinical Research (MacCAT-CR), a well-validated measure of decisional capacity6, and two well known measures of cognitive performance in PD - the Dementia Rating Scale 2 (DRS-2)7 and Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) 8.
Methods
Participants
We used non-proportional stratified sampling to enroll 90 patients with PD separated into three groups of 30 patients each using cut-points based on DRS-2 performance. Thirty neurologically normal older adults served as a reference for all three patient groups' capacity scores. Eligible PD participants had: (1) a diagnosis of idiopathic PD by a movement disorder neurologist at the Udall Center for Parkinson's Research at the University of Pennsylvania and (2) performance on the DRS-2 total age- and education-corrected scaled score in one of the following three categories: “normal” DRS-2 ≥9, “borderline” DRS-2 of 6 to 8, and “impaired” DRS-2 ≤5. All patients were interviewed “on” medication.
Procedure
Two trained research assistants collected data over two days to limit fatigue and assure that the capacity interviewer was blinded to cognitive data. Subjects underwent two MacCAT-CR interviews, one describing an early phase, double-blind, placebo-controlled study to test the safety and tolerability of a PD medication,9, 10 and the other describing a double-blind, sham-surgery controlled, randomized trial to test the safety and tolerability of injecting a growth factor gene into the brains of PD patients. Administration of the two MacCAT-CR interviews was counterbalanced to rule out order effects. Three physicians, each with at least five years of expertise in capacity assessment, viewed independently each capacity interview and rated whether the subject was capable of providing his or her own informed consent. 11, 12 A full description of our procedure, including descriptions of the MacCAT-CR, DRS-2, and MoCA domains is documented in the Supplementary Material.
Data Analyses
All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS v20.0. Our statistical approach is also documented in the Supplementary Material.
Results
Subject Characteristics
Demographic and clinical characteristics are documented in the Supplementary Material and Table S1. Because our normal controls showed nearly uniform research consent capacity and MoCA and DRS-2 scores at or near ceiling levels of performance, statistical analyses included only our patient sample (n=90).
DRS-2 Predictors of Capacity to Consent
MacCAT-CR and DRS-2
Spearman correlations between the DRS-2 subscales and patient scores on the MacCAT-CR drug and surgical trial capacity interviews (Table 1) suggest that the Understanding ability was consistently, and positively, related to all five DRS-2 subscale performances. Consistent with our expectations, however, the Initiation-Perseveration and Memory subscales were uniquely associated with the ability to Reason about the drug or surgical trials. Initiation-Perseveration and Memory subscales were also uniquely associated with Appreciation, but only on the drug trial.
Table 1.
Scenario | Drug Study | Surgical Study | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
MacCAT-CR Ability | MacCAT-CR Ability | |||||
| ||||||
DRS-2 Subscale | Understanding | Appreciation | Reasoning | Understanding | Appreciation | Reasoning |
Attention | .30** | .13 | .19 | .32** | .24* | .20 |
Initiation & Perseveration | .54** | .44** | .37** | .55** | .34** | .33** |
Construction | .22* | .11 | .14 | .25* | .33** | .04 |
Conceptualization | .29** | .26* | -.02 | .41** | .17 | .16 |
Memory | .58** | .57** | .29** | .51** | .39** | .36** |
p<.05,
p<.01 (Spearman Correlations)
Expert Ratings of Capacity and DRS-2 Subscale Performance
Overall, 60% (54/90) of PD patients were judged to be capable of giving informed consent for the drug trial by the expert raters. Direct logistic regression tested which of the five DRS-2 subscales were most associated with the expert evaluation of adequate decision-making capacity. A test of the full model with all five predictors was statistically significant, χ2 (5) = 51.94, p<0.001, indicating that the predictors, as a set, reliably distinguished between patients that the experts rated as capable or not capable. Overall classification accuracy by the DRS-2 subtests was moderate, with 75% and 83% of the patients correctly predicted as not capable or capable, respectively. Table 2 shows that only the DRS-2 Initiation-Perseveration and Memory subscales were significant predictors of expert rated capacity; the odds of being judged capable of consent by the expert raters increased 1.4× with each unit increase in the Initiation-Perseveration and or Memory subscales (DRS-2 Scaled Scores with Mean = 10, SD=3).
Table 2.
B | p-value | Odds Ratio | 95% CI % | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Drug Study | ||||
| ||||
Attention | .23 | .12 | 1.26 | 0.94-1.69 |
Initiation & Perseveration | .35 | .001 | 1.41 | 1.15-1.74 |
Construction | -.03 | .80 | .97 | .75-1.26 |
Conceptualization | .19 | .15 | 1.20 | .93-1.56 |
Memory | .31 | .002 | 1.36 | 1.12-1.66 |
| ||||
Surgical Study | ||||
| ||||
Attention | .27 | .10 | 1.31 | .95-1.80 |
Initiation & Perseveration | .31 | .003 | 1.37 | 1.11-1.68 |
Construction | .07 | .62 | 1.07 | .82-1.40 |
Conceptualization | .22 | .11 | 1.24 | .95-1.62 |
Memory | .31 | .002 | 1.36 | 1.12-1.66 |
The logistic regression results for the surgical setting, in comparison to the drug trial, indicated that the expert raters were more conservative in assigning adequate capacity in this scenario. Across all PD patients, only 48% were judged capable to consent (43/90). The overall model was again significant, χ2 (5) = 54.86, p< 0.001, indicating a significant effect of the 5 independent variables on the expert ratings of capacity (Table 2). Compared to the drug trial results, the DRS-2 subtests were slightly better at detecting patients that the experts judged to be not capable on the surgical trial, as the model identified 85% of patients judged to not have capacity. DRS-2 subtest prediction of capacity to consent was comparable to the drug trial at 79%. The Initiation-Perseveration and Memory subscales on the DRS-2 continued to predict expert capacity ratings. Overall, the odds of being judged capable of consent increased by 1.4× with each unit increase in the DRS-2 subscale scaled score (SS, Mean = 10, SD=3) for the Initiation-Perseveration or Memory subscales.
MoCA Predictors of Capacity to Consent
To test and validate our findings with the DRS-2 we conducted comparable analyses with the MoCA. Correlations between the MacCAT-CR abilities and MoCA subscales for PD participants (n=90) are reported in the Supplementary Material and Table S2. Spearman correlations between the MoCA subscales and patient scores on the MacCAT-CR capacity interview for both scenarios showed a pattern similar to the surgical trial results for the DRS-2 subscales. We subsequently confirmed with logistic regression that the MoCA Visuospatial/Executive subscale was a significant predictor of expert rated capacity. Overall, the odds of being judged capable of consent increased by 1.9× or 2.8× with each unit increase in Visuospatial/Executive score for the drug and surgical trial, respectively.
We then tested how well each of the MoCA subscales predicted expert rated capacity decisions with receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curves and area under the curve (AUC). We found that MoCA Visuospatial/Executive scores ≤ 3 (score range 0-5) showed sensitivity/specificity values of .86/.72 and .79/.79 for detecting the absence of expert-rated capacity for the drug and surgical trials, respectively. These results are further documented in the Supplementary Material.
Discussion
As noted in a meta-analytic review of executive function impairment in PD by Kudlicka and colleagues in this journal 13 such impairments are often observed in PD, yet anticipated empirical associations between those deficits and decision making in naturalistic contexts are “urgently needed”. Our research addresses this need by revealing associations between cognitive domains that are often disrupted in early PD and a real world context of critical importance to patients, families, and clinical researchers – research consent capacity. The results are also in accord with findings of reduced medical treatment decision abilities in PD patients with mild cognitive impairment14.
The DRS-2 Initiation and Perseveration and Memory subscales require acquisition and encoding of visual and verbal material, retrieval, organization, and cognitive flexibility7. These cognitive processes appear to overlap substantially with research decision-making abilities that include appraising study risks and benefits against the option to participate and consequences of participation. As such, the associations reported in the current study extend our previous results by showing that although overall cognitive total scores appear associated primarily with MaCAT-CR Understanding, executive functions and memory show more specific relationships with other demanding decision making abilities and predicted expert-rated capacity to consent to research. We subsequently validated our DRS-2 results using logistic regression and ROC curve analyses to show that the MoCA Visuospatial/Executive subscale was most predictive of expert capacity judgment. This suggests that this subscale may assess cognitive abilities deemed important by experts performing research consent capacity evaluation.
The value and relevance of our findings should be weighed against the following limitations. Data were gathered at an in-home interview in the context of two hypothetical clinical trials from participants who were familiar with research conducted in an academic medical center environment and who had approximately 15-16 years of education. In addition, we allowed subjects to retain the informed consent form during the capacity assessment. Other capacity assessment methods, such as giving persons a card with the relevant disclosure and then taking it away prior to assessing understanding, could yield different proportions of persons capable of consent and the degree to which memory or executive ability determines capacity performance.
Nevertheless, these results are important for research that recruits PD patients with borderline or mild cognitive impairment. They suggest the need to attend to deficits in memory and executive function, as subtle disruption in these areas increases the likelihood that a patient is not capable of giving consent. Indeed, people with PD MCI may also have reduced awareness of executive function impairment, potentially compromising safety and judgment in naturalistic settings 15. In such cases, study precautions should be considered, including a structured assessment of capacity and asking the patient to designate a study partner. We remind readers and investigators that a brief measure of executive function such as the DRS-2 Initiation-Perseveration subscale, Visuospatial/Executive subscale of the MoCA, or even a brief screening instrument such as the MoCA is not a sufficient measure of capacity. Low performance on these scales, however, may serve as a prompt to consider additional protections to guard against the possibility of mistakenly judging a patient who is not capable as capable.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Research Supported by: NINDS R01NS65087 and Morris K. Udall Center for Parkinson's Disease Research P50 NS053488 and NS062684, and NIA grant AG10124.
The authors thank the persons who participated in this study. The authors express their gratitude to Dr. Andrew Siderowf for his contributions to this project, especially his comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. The authors acknowledge Paige Brookstein, Abigail Darin, Eugenia Mamikonyan, James Minger, Jacqui Rick, and Baochan Tran for their assistance in subject recruitment and data gathering.
Footnotes
Financial Disclosures: S. Moelter is a neuropsychological consultant for Avid Radiopharmaceuticals and has received support from the NIH. D. Weintraub receives grant support from the NIH, Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, and Department of Veterans Affairs. He receives personal compensation from Teva Pharmaceuticals, Lundbeck Inc., Biogen, Pfizer, Avanir Pharmaceuticals, Merck & Co., UCB, CHDI Foundation, and the Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study. L. Mace reports no disclosures. M. Cary reports no disclosures. E. Sullo reports no disclosures. S. Xie reports no disclosures. J. Karlawish is a coholder of a license of an Integrated NeuroDegenerative Disease Database developed at the University of Pennsylvania.
References
- 1.Weintraub D, Doshi J, Koka D, et al. Neurodegeneration across stages of cognitive decline in Parkinson disease. Archives of neurology. 2011;68:1562–1568. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Troster AI, Troster AI. Neuropsychological characteristics of dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson's disease with dementia: differentiation, early detection, and implications for “mild cognitive impairment” and biomarkers. Neuropsychology Review. 2008;18:103–119. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9055-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Litvan I, Aarsland D, Adler CH, et al. MDS Task Force on mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: critical review of PD-MCI. Mov Disord. 2011;26:1814–1824. doi: 10.1002/mds.23823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Karlawish J, Cary M, Moelter ST, et al. Cognitive impairment and PD patients' capacity to consent to research. Neurology. 2013;81:801–807. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a05ba5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dirnberger G, Jahanshahi M. Executive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Journal of neuropsychology. 2013;7:193–224. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Appelbaum PS, Grisso T. The MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool - Clinical Research. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jurica PJ, Leitten CL, Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale - 2 (DRS-2) Professional Manual. Lutz, FL USA: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Karlawish JH, Casarett DJ, James BD. Alzheimer's disease patients' and caregivers' capacity, competency, and reasons to enroll in an early-phase Alzheimer's disease clinical trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2002;50:2019–2024. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50615.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rubright J, Sankar P, Casarett DJ, Gur R, Xie SX, Karlawish J. A memory and organizational aid improves Alzheimer disease research consent capacity: results of a randomized, controlled trial. The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;18:1124–1132. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181dd1c3b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Karlawish J, Kim SY, Knopman D, et al. Interpreting the clinical significance of capacity scores for informed consent in Alzheimer disease clinical trials. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16:568–574. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318172b406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kim SY, Appelbaum PS, Kim HM, et al. Variability of judgments of capacity: experience of capacity evaluators in a study of research consent capacity. Psychosomatics. 2011;52:346–353. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2011.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kudlicka A, Clare L, Hindle JV. Executive functions in Parkinson's disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2011;26:2305–2315. doi: 10.1002/mds.23868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Martin RC, Okonkwo OC, Hill J, et al. Medical decision-making capacity in cognitively impaired Parkinson's disease patients without dementia. Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2008;23:1867–1874. doi: 10.1002/mds.22170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kudlicka A, Clare L, Hindle JV. Awareness of executive deficits in people with Parkinson's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS. 2013;19:559–570. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.