Abstract
The comorbidity between Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) and Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) is well-established, and the two disorders share many similarities. However, there are also differences across disorders: most notably, BPD is diagnosed more frequently in females and ASPD in males. We investigated if a) comorbidity between BPD and ASPD is attributable to two discrete disorders or the expression of common underlying processes, and b) if the model of comorbidity is true across sex. Using a clinical sample of 1400 drug users in residential substance abuse treatment, we tested three competing models to explore whether the comorbidity of ASPD and BPD should be represented by a single common factor, two correlated factors, or a bifactor structure involving a general and disorder-specific factors. Next, we tested whether our resulting model was meaningful by examining its relationship with criterion variables previously reported to be associated with BPD and ASPD. The bifactor model provided the best fit and was invariant across sex. Overall, the general factor of the bifactor model significantly accounted for a large percentage of the variance in criterion variables, whereas the BPD and AAB specific factors added little to the models. The association of the general and specific factor with all criterion variables was equal for males and females. Our results suggest common underlying vulnerability accounts for both the comorbidity between BPD and AAB (across sex), and this common vulnerability drives the association with other psychopathology and maladaptive behavior. This in turn has implications for diagnostic classification systems and treatment.
General scientific summary
This study found that, for both males and females, borderline and antisocial personality disorders show a large degree of overlap, and little uniqueness. The commonality between BPD and ASPD mainly accounted for associations with criterion variables. This suggests that BPD and ASPD show a large common core that accounts for their comorbidity.
Keywords: borderline personality disorder, antisocial personality disorder, substance use disorders, comorbidity
The high comorbidity between borderline personality disorder (BPD) and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) is well-established. Rates of comorbidity range between 5.6% and 27% in community samples, and are as high as 57% in clinical and forensic settings (Black et al., 2007; Black, Gunter, Loveless, Allen, & Sieleni, 2010; Blackburn, Logan, Donnelly, & Renwick, 2003; Grant et al., 2008; McGlashan et al., 2000; Tadic et al., 2009). Unsurprisingly, multiple studies indicate that BPD and ASPD share etiology and developmental course. The two disorders have similar prevalence rates in both community (~1–2% for BPD, ~1–3% for ASPD) (Torrens, Gilchrist, Domingo-Salyany, & PsyCoBarcelona, 2011) and clinical (between 10–30% for both BPD and ASPD) samples (Black et al., 2007; Kessler et al., 1994; Lenzenweger, Lane, Loranger, & Kessler, 2007; Trull, Jahng, Tomko, Wood, & Sher, 2010). A meta-analysis of seven family studies of BPD found that the median prevalence of ASPD in relatives of BPD probands was 7%—twice that in the general population (White, Gunderson, Zanarini, & Hudson, 2003). Results from quantitative genetic studies indicate that BPD and ASPD symptoms have common genetic and nonshared environmental influences (Hunt, Bornovalova, & Patrick, 2015; Kendler et al., 2011) even after accounting for other cluster B personality disorders (Torgersen et al., 2008). Likewise, BPD and ASPD show similar temperamental vulnerabilities of emotion dysregulation, weak inhibitory control, and social cognition deficits (Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge, & Gatzke-Kopp, 2009; Beeney et al., 2015; Hicks, Vaidyanathan, & Patrick, 2010; Scott et al., 2013) and are associated with similar environmental precursors, including but not limited to childhood trauma, maladaptive parenting, and stressful life events (Eitle & Turner, 2002; Hicks et al., 2010; Stepp, Olino, Klein, Seeley, & Lewinsohn, 2013; Zanarini et al., 1997). While many of these risk factors also appear in other psychopathology, certain aspects of social cognition (i.e., ability to predict someone else’s behavior based on their belief state, mentalization) are unique to BPD and ASPD (Beeney et al., 2015). Finally, the two disorders have similar developmental course: a steep increase in adolescence, a decrease or plateauing in young adulthood, and a “burning out” by late middle age (Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick, & Iacono, 2006; Bornovalova, Hicks, Iacono, & McGue, 2009).
Striking as these similarities may be, it is important to highlight several important differences between the two disorders. For instance, BPD gives considerably more emphasis to the construct of affective instability and/or emotion dysregulation, identity diffusion, and difficulties in interpersonal relationships (APA, 2013). In a cross-sectional multi-trait, multi-method study, Scott and colleagues (2013) reported that emotion dysregulation and preoccupied attachment were robustly related to both BPD and ASPD; however, both emotion dysregulation and preoccupied attachment shared significantly more variance with BPD than APSD. Likewise, disinhibition – although common to both disorders – frequently takes different forms. A recent study modeled the relationships between different aspects of impulsivity (namely, urgency, perseverance, premeditation, and sensation-seeking) with BPD and ASPD simultaneously. Urgency and lack of perseverance were uniquely related to BPD, whereas lack of premeditation and excitement seeking were uniquely related to ASPD (DeShong & Kurtz, 2013; see also Fossati, Krueger, Markon, Borroni, & Maffei, 2013). Most relevant to the current study, there are marked sex differences in the severity and prevalence of the disorders. In clinical samples, approximately 2/3 to 3/4 of all BPD diagnoses are in females (Johnson et al., 2003). In contrast, ASPD is three times more common in males (Alegria et al., 2013).
To date, there are at least two gaps in the literature on sex, BPD, and ASPD. First, do BPD and ASPD have a common underlying core (Beauchaine et al., 2009; Skodol & Bender, 2003)? Second, after accounting for the common variance between the two disorders, do sex differences (if any) stem from true differences in symptom severity or biased diagnostic indicators (Skodol & Bender, 2003)? In other words, is the relationship between the disorders the same for males and females?
The first question– that BPD and ASPD share core etiological vulnerabilities that are expressed differently for males and females – is not a new one. Most recently, a very similar idea is the foundation for DSM-5 Section III (Models and Measures for Further Study) framework for describing personality disorders. This alternative model of personality pathology should, in principle, account for both commonalities and unique characteristics of each disorder (APA, 2013). Likewise, the BPD-ASPD-sex question has been explored numerous times in conceptual (Beauchaine et al., 2009; Paris, 1997; Paris, Chenard-Poirier, & Biskin, 2013; Skodol & Bender, 2003) and, to a lesser degree, in empirical (Hunt, Bornovalova, Kimonis, Lilienfeld, & Poythress, 2015; Johnson et al., 2003; Sprague, Javdani, Sadeh, Newman, & Verona, 2012; Verona, Sprague, & Javdani, 2012; Zlotnick, Rothschild, & Zimmerman, 2002) literature. However, solid empirical tests of this question are scarce – in part, because it is surprisingly difficult to test if two separate constructs are actually the same construct. One way is to simultaneously model the relationship between putative vulnerabilities, BPD, and ASPD (across sex). If BPD and ASPD represent the same underlying psychopathology, then the relationships between putative vulnerabilities and each disorder should be equal. Taking it a step further, if the general vulnerability is colored differently by sex, then the relationships between putative vulnerabilities and ASPD should be stronger among males, while the same relationships with BPD should be stronger among females. Although the Scott et al (2013) study we described above did not examine the relationships by sex, it is one of the few published studies that simultaneously modeled the relationships between putative vulnerabilities, BPD, and ASPD.
Another way of approaching the BPD-ASPD-sex question is to fit different factor analysis models to the covariances of BPD and ASPD indicators and test the invariance of the best fitting model across sex groups. The benefit of this approach is its ability to test competing hypotheses. If BPD and ASPD reflect the same underlying form of psychopathology, then all BPD and ASPD diagnostic indicators should load on a single common factor. In contrast, if the disorders are truly separable, then a correlated two-factor model should fit best. Finally, if BPD and ASPD have distinguishing characteristics but also a common core, then a bifactor model involving a general psychopathology factor and a factor specific to each disorder will fit best (Reise, 2012). A benefit of this model is the ability to determine the proportions of variance associated with common and specific factors. To our knowledge, these competing hypotheses have not been empirically examined.
For the most part, work addressing the second question – that of potential measurement bias versus true sex differences – is also scant. Studies relying primarily on content review found little evidence of sex bias (Anderson, Sankis, & Widiger, 2001; Morey, Warner, & Boggs, 2002; Sprock, Crosby, & Nielsen, 2001). More recent work has used measurement invariance (MI) models which can separate observed sex differences into a component reflecting true differences and a component reflecting bias that results from interpreting diagnostic indicators differently for males and females. This work focuses almost exclusively on BPD, and suggests that few items show evidence of bias (Aggen, Neale, Roysamb, Reichborn-Kjennerud, & Kendler, 2009; Hoertel, Peyre, Wall, Limosin, & Blanco, 2014; Jane, Oltmanns, South, & Turkheimer, 2007; Sharp et al., 2014). Item by item invariance analysis in these studies provides interesting information; however, without information about the overall model fit, it is difficult to tell if the latent mean sex differences are meaningful. As ASPD has been modeled almost exclusively in the context of other externalizing psychopathology (see Jane et al, 2007 for exception), tests of this disorder alone are limited. More importantly, MI testing of BPD and ASPD does not take into account the high comorbidity between the two disorders. In sum, ambiguities in the MI BPD literature and the dearth of studies involving ASPD indicators suggests that more research is needed to determine whether sex differences in prevalence rates are artifactual or substantive.
Current Study
In summary, research is needed to explore the covariance structure of BPD and ASPD, as well as measurement invariance of this structure across sex. We chose a large clinical sample (N=1400 substance users in residential treatment) to model these relationships. Notably, both clinical and epidemiological samples have their own strengths and weaknesses. Epidemiological samples make use of systematic sampling techniques and easily generalize to the population “at large”; however, the relatively low base rates of BPD and ASPD (~1–4%) (Hamdi & Iacono, 2014; Trull et al., 2010) and resulting restricted variance limit the type of models that can be fit. In clinical and forensic samples – despite their limited generalizability – the prevalence and comorbidity of BPD and ASPD is considerably higher (Black et al., 2007; Black et al., 2010; Blackburn et al., 2003; Grant et al., 2008; Zanarini et al., 1998), and the disorders are particularly relevant to maladaptive outcomes (Kokkevi, Stefanis, Anastasopoulou, & Kostogianni, 1998; Torrens et al., 2011). Finally, we examined the predictive utility of the BPD and AAB factor scores with respect to other clinically relevant outcomes: major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders, substance use disorders, history of psychiatric and drug treatment, lifetime length of incarceration, and age of onset for drugs and alcohol (Trull et al., 2010; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Hennen, Reich, & Silk, 2004). Many of these criterion variables are associated with both BPD and AAB; however, others are unique to the specific disorders. For example, age of drug and alcohol onset as well as history of incarceration/criminal behavior are more closely linked with ASPD, whereas lifetime psychiatric treatments are more typically associated with BPD (Cloninger, Sigvardsson, & Bohman, 1996; Cohen, Chen, Crawford, Brook, & Gordon, 2007; Franken & Hendriks, 2000; Johnson, Cloninger, Roache, Bordnick, & Ruiz, 2000; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Khera, & Bleichmar, 2001).
Method
Patients (N = 1400; 481 = female; 919 = male) were sequential admissions into an inpatient substance use treatment facility. The mean age of the sample was 42.91 (SD = 10.64). The majority of the sample was court-mandated to treatment (67.5%). The sample was primarily African American (88%). 39.1% of the sample reported less than a high school education; 50.6% reported a high school diploma or equivalent; and 10.3% reported some college or above. Participants were required to submit a negative urine drug screen prior to admission; those with positive drug screens completed medically assisted detoxification before admission to the facility. Drug-testing occurred on a weekly basis; use was grounds for immediate discharge.
Recruitment and consent
Intake assessments were conducted by doctoral level graduate students/senior research staff upon patient’s first week in treatment. The assessments served two purposes: (1) to provide diagnostic information to treatment staff at the center, and (2) to gather data for the current study. Patients were invited to participate in research following the intake assessment and were provided details regarding how information collected during the assessment would be used. Data for the current study includes only cases where informed consent was obtained (< 5% of patients declined to provide informed consent). The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board.
Measures
Assessment of Borderline and Antisocial Personality Disorders
Information regarding ASPD was gathered using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV, SCID-IV (First, 2002). The Diagnostic Interview for Personality Disorders (DIPD) was used to assess Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), as it has been argued to be a more comprehensive measure of BPD than the SCID-IV (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Chauncey, & Gunderson, 1987). All BPD and ASPD items were administered to all patients. Notably, although the criteria for DSM-IV ASPD includes a diagnosis of conduct disorder (CD) prior to age 15, our analyses focused specifically on the adult criteria of ASPD (adult antisocial behavior, AAB) because CD and AAB are traditionally treated as separate scales. In addition, including CD items would have developmental implications that are inconsistent with the diagnostic description of BPD, and that could obscure our interpretations of factor structure. For both AAB and BPD, the indicator-level data were dichotomous responses provided by clinicians to indicate whether a patient exhibited or did not exhibit a symptom.
Diagnostic interviewers were extensively trained and comprehensively supervised to ensure the accuracy of diagnoses. Training included viewing the full video protocol for the SCID-IV, conducting two mock interviews using the SCID-IV and the DIPD, observing two full interviews by experienced interviewers, conducting a final certification practice interview, being observed while conducting two real interviews at the treatment center (with the SCID-IV and the DIPD), and participating in weekly supervision led by a clinical psychologist. When disagreements occurred, discussion continued until consensus was reached and changes were made (Lechner et al., 2013).
Criterion Variables
Information regarding other comorbid psychopathology was collected via the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV (SCID-IV; First et al., 1995), including major depressive disorder, panic disorder, social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, and various substance dependence (see below). All disorders were assessed for current and ever/past status, with the exception of generalized anxiety disorder which was assessed for the last six months only. Diagnostic status, rather than symptom counts was used for all psychopathology, as all interviews (with the exception of personality disorders) were administered with “skip-outs”. All psychopathology was coded to be lifetime (i.e., disorder endorsed either currently or ever/past) absent or present. All anxiety disorders were combined into a single dichotomous “lifetime anxiety disorder” variable, as we did not expect different patterns of results across individual disorders. Interviewers attended to the timeline of substance dependence, and psychopathology diagnoses were made only when symptoms could not be tied directly to acute substance intoxication or the effects of withdrawal from a substance. For SUDs, we only assessed substance dependence (not abuse, given the severity of the sample), and we report here on alcohol, cannabis, opioid, hallucinogen/PCP, and crack/cocaine dependence, as these were the most frequently used substances in this sample. As with non-substance psychopathology, substance use disorders were assessed dichotomously and coded to be lifetime absent or present. All drug categories were left separate (as opposed to combining into larger “drug” and “alcohol” categories) given the rich literature surrounding personality, personality disorders, and drug choice (Bornovalova, Daughters, Hernandez, Richards, & Lejuez, 2005; Conway, Kane, Ball, Poling, & Rounsaville, 2003; Hopwood, Baker, & Morey, 2008). Lifetime rates of psychopathology across sex are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Combined (N=1400) | Female (N=481) | Male (N=919) | |
---|---|---|---|
BPD symptoms | |||
abandonment avoidance | 12.0 | 14.9 | 20.3 |
unstable relationships | 19.0 | 22.6 | 29.6 |
identity disturbance | 9.4 | 10.8 | 13.5 |
impulsivity | 36.2 | 38.6 | 43.3 |
inappropriate anger | 17.3 | 20.1 | 25.5 |
self-harm/suicidality | 12.6 | 15.7 | 21.6 |
affective instability | 23.8 | 28.4 | 37.1 |
emptiness | 20.6 | 22.3 | 25.6 |
stress-related paranoia/dissociation | 10.6 | 12.9 | 17.4 |
AAB symptoms | |||
failure to conform to social norms | 47.0 | 43.5 | 36.6 |
deceitfulness | 28.9 | 27.5 | 24.8 |
consistent irresponsibility | 35.4 | 33.9 | 31.0 |
impulsivity | 32.1 | 31.1 | 29.2 |
irritability or aggressiveness | 25.9 | 24.8 | 22.8 |
reckless disregard for safety of self or others | 28.9 | 28.5 | 27.9 |
lack of remorse | 11.9 | 10.8 | 8.8 |
Criterion Variables | |||
Major Depressive Disorder | 50.5 | 59.3 | 45.9 |
Lifetime Anxiety Disorder | 15.6 | 18.3 | 14.1 |
Lifetime Alcohol | 42.4 | 43.9 | 43.2 |
Lifetime Cannabis | 28.1 | 19.1 | 32.8 |
Lifetime Hallucinogens | 23.9 | 24.7 | 23.5 |
Lifetime Heroin/Opiates | 29.1 | 25.2 | 31.2 |
Lifetime Crack/Cocaine | 55.5 | 60.9 | 52.7 |
Lifetime history of psychiatric treatment | 57.9 | 65.9 | 53.6 |
Lifetime history of drug and alcohol treatment | 69.4 | 68.0 | 70.1 |
Age of onset, alcohola | 15.25 (5.06) | 16.51 (5.77) | 14.62 (4.53) |
Age of onset, drugsa | 16.94 (5.89) | 18.38 (6.39) | 16.18 (5.46) |
Total time spent in jaila | 3.15 (1.74) | 2.38 (1.62) | 3.54 (1.67) |
For variables scored continuously (age of onset of drugs and alcohol, and total time spent in jail), means and standard deviations are presented.
Additional criterion variables were collected via a self-report treatment, substance use, and legal history questionnaire (Banducci et al., 2013). Specifically, we collected data on: age of onset for alcohol and other drugs; lifetime number of days spent in jail; lifetime history of drug or alcohol treatment, and lifetime history of psychiatric treatment. Lifetime number of days in jail was coded as: none (0); 30 days or less (1); 31–90 days (2); 91 days–one year (3); between 1–5 years (4); greater than 5 years (5). Lifetime drug or alcohol and lifetime psychiatric treatment were coded dichotomously (absent/present).
Statistical Analyses
All factor analysis models were estimated using a robust weighted-least-squares estimator (WLSMV) in Mplus 7.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2015). Although item-level MI analyses for BPD and AAB is not the main focus of this paper, the methods and results for these analyses may still be informative for many researchers and are provided in the supplemental materials.
Modeling the Comorbidity between BPD and AAB
To answer the research questions regarding the covariance of BPD and AAB and invariance of factor structure across males and females, we fit three increasingly complex confirmatory factor analysis models for the combined sample: (1) a unidimensional model, (2) a correlated two-factor model, and (3) a bifactor model. The goodness of fit of each model was assessed using the comparative fit index (CFI) and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA). Values ≥ .95 and values ≤ .08 respectively for CFI and RMSEA indicate reasonable model-data fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). For these nested model comparisons, we used the likelihood ratio test (i.e., Δχ2/Δdf). Because Δχ2 cannot be directly employed for nested models when the WLSMV estimator is used, the Δχ2 was computed using the DIFFTEST procedure in Mplus.
MI testing for AAB and BPD factor structure
After identifying the best-fitting model based on the combined sample, we tested for MI across sex groups. Females were defined as the reference group, and males were defined as the focal group. We followed the procedure for MI testing with binary data recommended by Muthén and Muthén (1998–2015) which involves a comparison of configural and scalar invariance models. To allow every item to be compared across reference and focal groups, we identified the scale of measurement by constraining the variance of the general and group-specific factors to 1 in both groups. In the configural model, we fixed the residuals to 1 and the latent means to 0 in both groups for identification, while factor loadings and thresholds were free to vary. In the scalar model, we constrained the loadings and thresholds to be equal across groups while freely estimating the focal group’s residuals and latent means. We compared the goodness of fit statistics for the configural and scalar models using three criteria: likelihood ratio test Δχ2/Δdf), changes in CFI (ΔCFI) and changes in RMSEA (ΔRMSEA). In accordance with interpretive guidelines (Chen, 2007), ΔCFI ≥ .01 and ΔRMSEA ≥ .015 indicated a significant decrement in fit.
Unique contributions of general and specific factors in predicting criterion variables
As is seen in the results below, we identified the bifactor model as the best-fitting model; this model showed MI across sex. Consequently, we conducted a series of structural equation models (SEMs) to test whether the BPD and AAB specific factors would account for variance in criterion variables beyond the general factor. Each criterion was modeled separately. For each criterion variable, we fit two models. The full model estimated direct paths from both the general and specific factors on the criterion variable. In the constrained model, we fixed the path coefficients of the specific factors on the criterion variable to zero. Next, we evaluated the significance of the BPD and AAB specific factors (i.e., the decision to keep the full versus the constrained model). The formal test was done via nested model comparisons (full versus constrained) based on Δχ2/Δdf. However, we also required that the change in variance accounted for (ΔR2) should be meaningful. We defined “meaningful” by an increase in variance accounted for of at least 2.25% (Hunsley &Meyer, 2003). Thus, to proceed beyond the constrained model, the full model should demonstrate a significant Δχ2/Δdf and a ΔR2 ≥ .0225.
Finally, we conducted a multi-group SEM to examine whether the structural relationships of the general and specific factors with the criterion variables were significantly different across sex. Statistical significance was evaluated using Wald tests. If the specific BPD and AAB factors failed to provide evidence for meaningful incremental contribution, sex differences were only examined for the general factor. Given the number of tests, significance was set at .01 for all SEMs.
Results
Diagnostic and comorbidity rates
The percentages of males and females exhibiting each BPD and AAB symptom are reported in Table 1. Diagnostic and comorbidity rates were as follows. Overall, 14.1% of participants (24.4% females and 10.3% males) met criteria for BPD. Likewise, 36.8% of the participants (32.9% females and 38.8% males) met criteria for AAB. Of the entire sample, 9.6% met criteria for both BPD and AAB (13.5% females; 7.6% males). Rates and mean levels of psychopathology and maladaptive behavior of interest are also reported in Table 1.
MI testing for AAB and BPD factor structure
Model fit indices for the unidimensional, correlated two-factor, and bifactor models are presented in Table 2 for the male, female, and combined samples. In each case, the bifactor model provided the best fit. All 16 AAB and BPD indicators loaded on the general factor in addition to their respective specific factors1,2.
Table 2.
Model | χ2 | df | CFI | RMSEA | Comparison: Δχ2(Δdf) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total sample (N = 1,400) | |||||
Model 1: Unidimensional model | 937.41*** | 104 | .88 | .08 | Model 1 vs. 2: 164.97*** (1) |
Model 2: Correlated factor model | 348.91*** | 103 | .97 | .04 | Model 1 vs. 3: 460.75*** (15) |
Model 3: Bifactor model | 199.62*** | 89 | .98 | .03 | Model 2 vs. 3: 111.02*** (14) |
Female (N = 481) | |||||
Model 1: Unidimensional model | 379.25*** | 104 | .91 | .08 | Model 1 vs. 2: 53.22*** (1) |
Model 2: Correlated factor model | 188.83*** | 103 | .97 | .04 | Model 1 vs. 3: 156.80*** (15) |
Model 3: Bifactor model | 153.01*** | 89 | .98 | .04 | Model 2 vs. 3: 36.55*** (14) |
Male (N = 919) | |||||
Model 1: Unidimensional model | 544.16*** | 104 | .88 | .07 | Model 1 vs. 2: 101.44*** (1) |
Model 2: Correlated factor model | 251.41*** | 103 | .96 | .04 | Model 1 vs. 3: 270.90*** (15) |
Model 3: Bifactor model | 133.50*** | 89 | .99 | .02 | Model 2 vs. 3: 86.04*** (14) |
Note.
p < .001.
Table 3 shows the standardized parameter estimates for the bifactor model for males and females. Notably, after controlling for the general factor, the group factor loadings were lower in both groups. As seen in Table 4, the general factor accounted for nearly 66% of the common variance in females, and 60% in males. The specific factors – BPD and AAB – only accounted for 14% and 20% (respectively) of the common variance in females and for 23% and 18% of the common variance in males.
Table 3.
Female (F)
|
Male (M)
|
|F-M|
|
||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
τ | λG | λB | λA | υ2 | τ | λG | λB | λA | υ2 | Δτ | ΔλG | ΔλB | ΔλA | |
BPD symptoms | ||||||||||||||
abandonment avoidance | .83 | .72 | .28 | .41 | 1.17 | .63 | .41 | .44 | .34 | .09 | .13 | |||
unstable relationships | .54 | .71 | .29 | .42 | .89 | .69 | .26 | .46 | .35 | .02 | .03 | |||
identity disturbance | 1.11 | .67 | .39 | .39 | 1.32 | .50 | .54 | .46 | .21 | .18 | .15 | |||
Impulsivity | .18 | .90 | .00 | .19 | .35 | .83 | .00 | .32 | .18 | .07 | .00 | |||
inappropriate anger | .66 | .75 | .27 | .37 | .95 | .62 | .25 | .55 | .28 | .13 | .02 | |||
self-harm/suicidality | .79 | .47 | .43 | .60 | 1.16 | .51 | .45 | .54 | .37 | .04 | .02 | |||
affective instability | .34 | .65 | .43 | .40 | .71 | .61 | .46 | .42 | .38 | .04 | .03 | |||
Emptiness | .66 | .52 | .60 | .38 | .83 | .41 | .69 | .37 | .16 | .11 | .09 | |||
stress-related paranoia/dissociation | .94 | .66 | .40 | .40 | 1.24 | .47 | .64 | .37 | .30 | .19 | .24 | |||
AAB symptoms | ||||||||||||||
failure to conform to social norms | .35 | .43 | .67 | .37 | .09 | .39 | .59 | .50 | .26 | .04 | .08 | |||
deceitfulness | .69 | .42 | .48 | .60 | .56 | .47 | .39 | .63 | .13 | .05 | .09 | |||
consistent irresponsibility | .55 | .65 | .41 | .41 | .47 | .55 | .44 | .51 | .09 | .10 | .03 | |||
impulsivity | .50 | .60 | .43 | .46 | .38 | .55 | .43 | .51 | .11 | .04 | .01 | |||
irritability or aggressiveness | .75 | .50 | .51 | .49 | .65 | .38 | .57 | .54 | .10 | .12 | .06 | |||
reckless disregard for safety of self or others | .59 | .55 | .60 | .34 | .55 | .54 | .41 | .54 | .04 | .01 | .19 | |||
lack of remorse | 1.36 | .52 | .45 | .53 | 1.18 | .57 | .30 | .59 | .18 | .05 | .15 | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Gen | BPD | AAB | υ2 | Gen | BPD | AAB | υ2 | |||||||
| ||||||||||||||
% Total Variance | .38 | .08 | .12 | .42 | .31 | .12 | .09 | .48 | ||||||
% Common Variance | .66 | .14 | .20 | .60 | .23 | .18 | ||||||||
Ωh | .77 | .71 | ||||||||||||
Ωs | .19 | .43 | .30 | .38 |
Note. τ is a threshold parameter, and λB, λA and λG represent the standardized factor loadings on BPD, ASPD, and general factor. υ2 represents a uniqueness (residual) term (i.e., variance not accounted for by λB, λA and λG). Δτ, ΔλA, and ΔλB represent the absolute difference of each parameter estimate between female and male samples. All loadings are statistically significant. Ωh and Ωs represent coefficient omega hierarchical for general and specific group factors, respectively
Table 4.
Model fit statistics
|
Standardized estimate (SE)
|
R2 | ΔR2 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
χ2 (df) | RMSEA | CFI | Δχ2 (Δdf) | General | BPD | AAB | ||||
Age of onset: Alcohol | M1 | 285.44*** (104) | .04 | .98 | 44.48*** (2) | −.27a (.03) | .07 | .05 | ||
M2 | 211.10*** (102) | .03 | .99 | −.11a (.04) | −.13a (.05) | −.30a (.04) | .12 | |||
Age of onset: Drugs | M1 | 306.68*** (104) | .04 | .97 | 60.76*** (2) | −.32a (.03) | .10 | .05 | ||
M2 | 217.83*** (102) | .03 | .98 | −.17a (.04) | −.07 (.06) | −.34a (.05) | .15 | |||
Total time spent in jail | M1 | 395.86*** (104) | .05 | .96 | 80.69*** (2) | .10a (.04) | .01 | .16 | ||
M2 | 223.15*** (102) | .03 | .98 | −.03 (.04) | −.01 (.05) | .41a (.04) | .17 | |||
Major Depressive Disorder | M1 | 332.32*** (104) | .04 | .97 | 71.10*** (2) | .64a (.03) | .40 | .12 | ||
M2 | 205.58*** (102) | .03 | .99 | .39a (.04) | .61a (.06) | .07 (.05) | .52 | |||
Lifetime Anxiety Disorder | M1 | 265.01*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 32.71*** (2) | .51a (.04) | .26 | .03 | ||
M2 | 211.41*** (102) | .03 | .99 | .36a (.05) | .39a (.06) | −.01 (.06) | .29 | |||
Lifetime Alcohol Dependence | M1 | 221.60*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 11.41*** (2) | .37a (.04) | .14 | −.01 | ||
M2 | 205.96*** (102) | .03 | .99 | .30a (.05) | .21a (.06) | .01 (.05) | .13 | |||
Lifetime Cannabis Dependence | M1 | 208.02*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 5.15(2) | .20a (.04) | .04 | .00 | ||
M2 | 203.95*** (102) | .03 | .99 | .13a (.05) | .08 (.07) | .14a (.06) | .04 | |||
Lifetime Hallucinogenic Dependence | M1 | 223.15*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 6.01 (2) | .25a (.05) | .07 | .00 | ||
M2 | 217.59*** (102) | .03 | .98 | .15a (.05) | .17a (.07) | .13a (.06) | .07 | |||
Lifetime Heroin/Opiates dependence | M1 | 257.14*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 11.08** (2) | .13a (.04) | .02 | .02 | ||
M2 | 243.45*** (102) | .03 | .98 | .07 (.05) | .02 (.07) | .18a (.06) | .04 | |||
Lifetime Crack/Cocaine | M1 | 217.88*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 4.66 (2) | .30a (.04) | .09 | −.01 | ||
M2 | 215.67*** (102) | .03 | .98 | .23a (.05) | .08 (.06) | .13a (.05) | .08 | |||
Lifetime history of psychiatric treatment | M1 | 324.02*** (104) | .04 | .97 | 63.13*** (2) | .43a (.04) | .18 | .17 | ||
M2 | 206.26*** (102) | .03 | .99 | .19a (.05) | .55a (.06) | .08 (.05) | .35 | |||
Lifetime history of drug and alcohol treatment | M1 | 235.78*** (104) | .03 | .98 | 12.69** (2) | .19a (.05) | .04 | .04 | ||
M2 | 216.71*** (102) | .03 | .98 | .06 (.05) | .16a (.07) | .23a (.06) | .08 |
Note. M1 = constrained model; M2 = Full model; General = general factor; BPD = BPD group specific factor; AAB = AAB group specific factor; R2 = proportion of variance explained in the criterion by the model; ΔR2 = Change in R2; f2 = the proportion of variance uniquely counted for by the specific factors above and beyond the general factor.
p < .001
p < .01.
Denotes standardized estimates falling within the 95% CI.
Additional statistics, coefficient omega hierarchical (Ω h) and omega-subscale (Ωs), were calculated for both sexes (Reise, 2012; Zinbarg, Revelle, Yovel, & Li, 2005)3. Ω h estimates the proportion of total variance attributable to the general factor, whereas the Ωs represents the proportion of variance accounted for by the specific factor after controlling for the general factor. Ωh were .77 and .71 for females and males (respectively), indicating that 77% and 71% of the variance of the sum scores across the sixteen diagnostic criteria could be attributed to variance on the general factor. Ωs were .19 and .43 for BPD and AAB indicators, respectively, among females, and.30 and .38, respectively, among males4.
Finally, after determining that the bifactor model provided the best fit in the combined sample, we tested for MI across sex groups. The results were as follows: configural invariance, χ2 (df = 178) = 286.82, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .03; scalar invariance, χ2 (df = 206) = 290.23, CFI =.99, RMSEA =.02. All three criteria supported scalar MI (Δχ2/Δdf = 27.35 (28), p >.05; ΔCFI =.00 and ΔRMSEA=.01), indicating that the comorbidity between AAB and BPD reflects equivalent processes for males and females. Also, females had higher general (.34 units) and BPD-specific (.29 units) factor means, and males had a higher mean on the AAB-specific factor (.52 units)5.
Unique contributions of general and specific factors in predicting criterion variables
Table 4 presents fit from a series of nested SEM models examining the unique contribution of BPD and AAB specific factors to criterion variables. First, the fit of all constrained models (criterion on general factor only; criterion on BPD and AAB fixed at zero) met benchmark CFI and RMSEA criteria for overall good model fit. Each constrained model revealed that the general factor significantly predicted all criterion variables. Variance accounted for by the general factor varied a great deal across criterion variables. The largest effects were found for (in descending order): lifetime major depressive disorder (R2=.40), lifetime anxiety disorder (R2=.26), lifetime history of psychiatric hospitalization (R2=.18), and lifetime alcohol dependence (R2=.14). The variance accounted for by the general factor in age of onset, heavy substance use, and various dependence diagnoses was generally small to moderate (R2=.02 for heroin dependence through .10 for onset of drug use). The smallest effect size was for total time spent in jail: the general factor only accounted for 1% of the variance in this criterion.
Next, the fit of all full models (criterion on general as well as BPD and AAB specific factors) also met benchmark model fit criteria, demonstrating adequate fit to the data. When comparing the full models against the constrained models, however, the full model did not fit better (as indicated by the Δχ2/Δdf) in three cases (lifetime crack/cocaine, cannabis dependence; and hallucinogenic dependence). Two additional variables (lifetime heroin and alcohol dependence) failed to pass the Hunsley & Meyer (2003) benchmark criteria of meaningful variance accounted for (Hunsley & Meyer, 2003) (ΔR2 > = .0225). In sum, for all drug and alcohol use disorders, the BPD and AAB specific factors failed to add incremental variance above the general factor, suggesting that most of the effect is accounted for by the common variance between BPD and AAB.
On the other hand, for three criterion variables (namely, total time spent in jail; lifetime major depressive disorder; and lifetime history of psychiatric treatment), criterion variables, the specific factors accounted for a large percentage of incremental variance (ΔR 2= .12–.17). Unsurprisingly, the effect on total time spent in jail was driven by the AAB specific factor, whereas the effect on lifetime major depressive disorder and lifetime history of psychiatric treatment was driven by the BPD specific factor.
For the remaining four criterion variables, the specific factors accounted for a small to moderate proportion of the variance (ΔR 2=.03–.05). The association with age of onset of drug and alcohol use was driven by the AAB specific factor (although the BPD factor also predicted age of onset of drug and alcohol use to a smaller degree). The association with lifetime anxiety disorder was driven by the BPD specific factor. Both BPD and AAB contributed to the with lifetime history of drug and alcohol treatment.
Multi-group bifactor SEM models to test sex differences in structural relationships
Table 5 presents results from multi-group SEM models testing sex differences in the overall effects reported above. Wald tests indicated the association of the general factor with each of 12 criterion variables was equal for males and females, suggesting that the relationship between the general and specific factors with criterion variables is not sex-specific.
Table 5.
Model fit indices
|
Standardized estimate (SE)
|
Wald test χ2 (df) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
χ2 (df) | RMSEA | CFI | General | BPD | AAB | |||
Age of onset: Alcohol | 320.96 (232)*** | .023 | .987 | Female | −.18a (.07) | −.16 (.08) | −.17a (.07) | 2.55 (3) |
Male | −.12a (.04) | −.16a (.07) | −.33a (.06) | |||||
Age of onset: Drugs | 329.06 (232)*** | .024 | .986 | Female | −.31a (.06) | −.02 (.09) | −.22a (.08) | 7.17 (3) |
Male | −.13a (.05) | −.17a (.07) | −.35a (.06) | |||||
Total time spent in jail | 324.43 (232)*** | .024 | .987 | Female | .10 (.06) | −.02 (.08) | .20a (.07) | 7.59 (3) |
Male | −.03 (.05) | .08 (.07) | .47a (.06) | |||||
Lifetime Major Depressive Disorder | 312.92 (232)*** | .022 | .989 | Female | .41a (.07) | .52a (.09) | .10 (.09) | 1.92 (3) |
Male | .37a (.06) | .66a (.08) | .08 (.07) | |||||
Lifetime Anxiety Disorder | 319.14 (232)*** | .023 | .988 | Female | .37a (.08) | .41a (.10) | .02 (.10) | .38 (3) |
Male | .35a (.07) | .39a (.09) | −.04 (.09) | |||||
Lifetime Alcohol Dependence | 337.08 (236)*** | .025 | .985 | Female | .43a (.06) | 1.05 (1) | ||
Male | .34a (.05) | |||||||
Lifetime Cannabis Dependence | 318.22 (236)*** | .022 | .988 | Female | .28a (.08) | .77 (1) | ||
Male | .20a (.05) | |||||||
Lifetime Hallucinogens Dependence | 337.49 (236)*** | .025 | .985 | Female | .19a (.08) | 1.31 (1) | ||
Male | .30a (.06) | |||||||
Lifetime Heroin/Opiates Dependence | 355.99 (236)*** | .027 | .983 | Female | .03 (.07) | 3.87 (1) | ||
Male | .21a (.05) | |||||||
Lifetime Crack/Cocaine Dependence | 344.47 (236)*** | .026 | .984 | Female | .32a (.07) | .28 (1) | ||
Male | .28a (.05) | |||||||
Lifetime history of psychiatric treatment | 311.85 (232)*** | .022 | .989 | Female | .31a (.07) | .58a (.09) | .23a (.09) | 8.97 (3) |
Male | .12 (.06) | .54a (.09) | .06 (.07) | |||||
Lifetime history of drug and alcohol treatment | 328.92 (232)*** | .024 | .986 | Female | .04 (.08) | .30a (.10) | .34a (.10) | 3.19 (3) |
Male | .09 (.06) | .08 (.10) | .17a (.08) |
Note: General = general factor; BPD = BPD group specific factor; AAB = AAB group specific factor;
p < .001
p < .01.
Denotes standardized estimates falling within the 95% CI.
Discussion
The current study investigated if BPD and AAB have a substantial common underpinning, in addition to characteristics that uniquely differentiate the two disorders. We also examined the relative importance of the common and disorder specific variance by examining associations with other relevant clinical constructs. Finally, we examined whether the covariance structure of BPD and AAB shows evidence of sex bias by investigating MI of the models across sex, and tested whether the relationships between general and disorder-specific factors with clinical criterion variables differ across males and females. We found that a bifactor model involving a fairly strong general psychopathology factor and two disorder-specific factors provided the best description of the data. This is consistent with previous findings indicating that BPD and AAB show phenotypic, genetic, and environmental overlap, above and beyond other psychopathology (Torgersen et al., 2008). This suggests that the general factor may be a more reliable index of sex differences than either latent means of BPD or AAB. Thus, it may be more parsimonious to combine BPD and AAB into a single syndrome in diagnostic classification systems as well as studies of etiology and treatment. Likely, this factor heavily represents impulsive and disinhibited tendencies – and to a lesser degree, interpersonal dysfunction and emotion dysregulation. While this makes our general factor unlike other studies that generally extract a large factor of negative affectivity, emotional dysregulation, or interpersonal dysfunction (Hopwood et al., 2011; Jahng et al., 2011; Livesley, 2011; Pincus & Wiggins, 1990), it is reflective of both the common item content between the disorders and the empirical findings that BPD and AAB both load on the externalizing spectrum (Eaton et al., 2011; James & Taylor, 2008; Kendler et al., 2011).
Although the variances accounted for by the disorder-specific factors were small in comparison to the general factor, it may be still worthwhile to ask what those variances represent. For BPD, across sex, the items that most strongly loaded on the disorder-specific factor represent feelings of emptiness and cognitive disturbance (stress-related paranoia/dissociation). For AAB, the remaining variance was accounted by items that represent lack of socialization/lack of conformity to rules (Hicks & McGue, 2014). It may be possible to think of these criteria as ones that “color” the expression of BPD and AAB, and possibly account for sex differences in the respective disorders.
Next, we tested the measurement invariance of this bifactor model. Notably, previous work examining MI has a) generally focused on BPD; b) took little consideration of AAB or its covariance with BPD. Although previous studies used slightly different MI methods than ours (individual-item invariance versus global model invariance), we appear to have produced findings consistent with epidemiological samples (Aggen et al., 2009; Hoertel et al., 2014; Jane et al., 2007; Sharp et al., 2014). Similar (and a very limited number, see supplemental materials) items produced item-level non-invariance. Overall, our bifactor model was invariant across sex, suggesting little evidence of bias in our data. The females demonstrated a higher mean on the general and BPD factor, whereas the males demonstrated a significantly higher AAB factor. While the sex patterns in BPD and AAB specific factors are expected, it is difficult to make much of the mean differences in the general factor at this time. The female > male mean difference may be a local phenomenon due to sample characteristics or number and type of BPD and AAB items in the model
The incremental validity analyses lined up with the rest of our results. The general factor of the bifactor model is a pivotal and sole predictor of five criterion variables: namely, all dependence diagnoses. For all alcohol and drug dependence, the general factor significantly accounted for a large percentage of the variance, whereas the BPD and AAB specific factors added little to the models. Our results strong support to the notion that a common underlying vulnerability accounts for both the comorbidity between BPD and AAB, and this common vulnerability drives the association with all substance use problems (but not necessarily age of onset of drug and alcohol use, or drug and alcohol treatment). Thus, despite the traditional clinical, diagnostic, and statistical distinction between the two disorders, the general factor is much more important for prediction of comorbidity and treatment planning of at least, drug and alcohol treatment (see below for implications of these results for interpretation of the general factor).
In several cases, however, the specific factors produced incremental prediction to the variance that could not be ignored. The BPD specific factor incrementally predicted lifetime major depressive disorder and lifetime history of psychiatric treatment, whereas the AAB factor predicted lifetime number of days in jail. While these results intuitively make sense and are consistent with previous work (Zanarini et al., 2001), it is unclear how a factor representing emptiness and cognitive distortions interacts with the liabilities representing the general factor to produce depression and psychiatric treatment history. Likewise, the association between AAB specific factor and incarceration is not surprising. The highest factor loading on the AAB specific factor is an indicator reading “failure to conform to social norms with respect to lawful behaviors as indicated by repeatedly performing acts that are grounds for arrest” (APA, 1994, 2013); the association of this factor with incarceration may simply reflect the same construct. More notable are the associations of the AAB specific factor with drug and alcohol age of initiation; early age of alcohol initiation, for instance, has been long thought to be a marker for an alcoholism subtype marked by a high genetic loading and high criminal behavior (Cloninger et al., 1996; Johnson et al., 2000). It is possible that we are tapping into this latent dimension with the specific AAB factor.
Implications, Limitations, and Future Directions
Our results should be considered in the broader scope of personality disorder and normal personality literature. As we note in the introduction, both our questions and our results are consistent with current conceptualization of personality disorders which propose both general impairments in personality (common to all PDs), and specific manifestations of each phenotype which are captured by maladaptive trait profiles. (APA, 2013). Likewise, our results are consistent with recent reports of both normal personality (Biderman, 2013; Biderman, Nguyen, Cunningham, Chen, & Watson, 2013) and personality (and non-personality) pathology (Hengartner, Ajdacic-Gross, Rodgers, Mueller, & Roessler, 2014; Hopwood et al., 2011; Jahng et al., 2011; Scott et al., 2013; Sharp et al., 2014; Wolf, Miller, & Brown, 2011). To illustrate, a recent study by Sharp and others (2015) reported that a general underlying factor accounted for common variance across personality disorders, and six specific factors accounted unique variance in personality disorders. What is also notable is Sharp et al reported that there was no specific “factor” of BPD; rather, BPD symptoms loaded appreciably onto the general underlying factor. Other researchers have replicated this phenomenon: while the specific factors frequently differ, a general factor – usually representing interpersonal dysfunction – accounts for much of the covariance across personality pathology symptoms and traits (Hengartner et al., 2014; Jahng et al., 2011; Wolf et al., 2011).
What is not clear, at this point, is where our bifactor model of BPD-AAB falls within these frameworks. In other words, is there something unique to BPD-AAB comorbidity that is not accounted for by a common liability to all personality (or non-personality) disorders? Or, like Sharp et al, does BPD generally represent a general liability to personality pathology, and our model reflects the tendency of BPD to sink into a general factor? The first possibility is not implausible. Jahng and colleagues (2011) reported that retaining a specific factor characterized by cluster B comorbidity –representing information about behavioral disinhibition/impulsivity – significantly improved model fit over the model that included just the general factor representing interpersonal dysfunction. All other specific personality disorder factors (i.e., Cluster A and C) did not improve model fit. More importantly, the specific cluster B factor accounted for considerably more variance in alcohol, drug, and nicotine dependence than the interpersonal factor. This is highly consistent with our own results, indicating that our general factor was the sole predictor of all drug and alcohol use disorders. Unsurprisingly, we also interpret our general factor to contain information about behavioral disinhibition. The notion that a common core of behavioral disinhibition largely accounts for the comorbidity between BPD, AAB, and substance use disorders is consistent with several twin (Distel et al., 2012; James & Taylor, 2008; Kendler et al., 2011) and neuroimaging (Sauder et al., 2016; Beck et al., 2009; Jentsch & Taylor, 1999) studies.
Nevertheless, our study is not equipped to distinguish between these two alternative hypotheses. A clear next step is to replicate the current study (using a multi-method, multi-sample approach, see below). This type of study would a) model whether there is a specific factor BPD-AAB after accounting for the covariance across all personality disorders (i.e., the interpersonal dysfunction factor) and the disorder-specific variance; b) investigate if this BPD-AAB factor is clinically meaningful, i.e., shows incremental criterion validity. In this context, it is also possible to test if this BPD-AAB specific factor simply picks up externalizing psychopathology, or some other characteristic unique to the two disorders.
Several limitations of the current paper set the stage for follow-up studies. First, our study was cross-sectional and utilized an adult sample, and therefore conclusions cannot be advanced regarding development or causality. Thus longitudinal studies examining the covariance structure of BPD and AAB are needed. Likewise, future work should examine the incremental predictive utility of the general and specific factors in predicting developmental course and response to treatment. Second, the symptoms of BPD and AAB were entirely clinician-rated. This is a non-trivial point: the clinician-rated, diagnostic indicators have, in the past, produced different sex patterns than the psychometrically-validated BPD and AAB instruments– and the clinical and self-report instruments have been shown to contribute unique information to the constructs (DeShong & Kurtz, 2013; Hopwood et al., 2008; Trull, 2001). Related to this point, the lack of trait-based instruments such as the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 is a significant limitation, as trait-based scales that are specifically designed to account for comorbidity between personality disorders may produce a different factor structure (Krueger et al, 2012). Thus, both the sex difference and psychometric structure question should be investigated using a multi-method procedure that combines a) self-report measures of BPD and AAB, b) traits hypothesized to underlie them (Few et al., 2013); c) clinical/interview measures of the two constructs.
Next, it is possible that females in a residential treatment facility had a higher “threshold” of severity than males. On the other hand, this type of ascertainment difference would have produced measurement noninvariance; yet, the model was invariant. More importantly, there is a very real possibility that the participants were selected (court-mandated into treatment) based on the behavior that was one of the primary variables of interest in our study (adult antisocial behavior). Because the resulting participants would be systematically different from the rest of the population, a clear next step is to rule out the possibility of measurement noninvariance of the current model across clinical and epidemiological samples. Third, the current set of results should be replicated in a representative epidemiological sample, as previous work shows that community samples show a very different pattern of sex differences (and lack thereof), and possibly different patterns of MI and patterns of comorbidity (Bornovalova, Hicks, Patrick, Iacono, & McGue, 2011). Finally, the current analyses should be replicated in a genetically informed (e.g., twin) sample. This in turn would allow researchers to examine whether the factor common to BPD and AAB also reflects common etiology. Exploration of these questions will provide further insight into etiological influences on BPD and AAB as well as their comorbidity.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Data for this project were collected at the University of Maryland, with approval from the university’s IRB. The work was supported by grant DA 19405 from NIDA. All authors had full access to the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. No conflict of interest exists for any of the authors.
Footnotes
In all bifactor models, the general and specific factors were modeled as orthogonal.
It is important to note that the loading of one of the BPD criteria (i.e., “impulsivity”) on the corresponding specific factor was fixed to zero for solving a convergence problem. To ensure that this constraint did not substantially alter our results, we repeated the analyses without the BPD impulsivity item. The factor structure and subsequent interpretations did not change.
Reise (2012) illustrates how to compute variance accounted for by general and specific factors. In this case, , where G, B, and A represent the general and specific factors, and represents the sum of the item error variances (uniquenesses) over all items. For specific factor AAB, but, in this case, represents the sum of the error variances for the items measuring AAB (a similar calculation is used for BPD items).
Although we focus on describing the relationship with adult AAB symptoms, we also examined whether the model would hold after accounting for childhood symptoms of conduct disorder. Results indicated that, using 22 indicators of ASPD (15 CD; 7 AAB) and 9 indicators of BPD, consistent with the main results, a bifactor model fit the best. The model fit was as follows: unidimensional model: χ2 (df = 434) = 2811.92, CFI = .82, RMSEA = .06; correlated two-factor model, χ2 (df = 433) = 1617.99, CFI = .91, correlated three-factor model, χ2 (df = 431) = 1287.389, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .04; bifactor with two specific factors, ASPD and BPD, χ2 (df = 404) = 1183.28, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .04; bifactor with three specific factors, CD, AAB, and BPD, χ2 (df = 404) = 1237.00, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .04. We also conducted MI tests across sex. As in the main analyses, we tested for MI across sex groups: configural invariance, χ2 (df = 808) = 1405.71, CFI = .95, RMSEA = .03; scalar invariance, χ2 (df = 866) = 1464.48, CFI =.95, RMSEA = .03. Although likelihood ratio test was significant (Δχ2/Δdf = 123.76 (58), p <.05), ΔCFI =.00 and ΔRMSEA=.00 were not.
We examined a more exploratory (and thereby less restrictive) version of the bifactor model as well: the bifactor ESEM. As with CFA, the bifactor ESEM fit well [χ2 (df = 75) = 149.21, CFI = .99], the general factor accounted for >60% of the common variance among the indicators, and the conclusions about the factor structure did not change. We also explored MI of the ESEM model. There was evidence of scalar invariance as indicated by all three criteria (Δχ2 (Δdf), ΔRMSEA, ΔCFI) [configural: (χ2 (df = 150) = 228.65, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .03; scalar: χ2 (df = 186) = 241.56, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .02;Δχ2/Δdf = 33.78 (36), p >.05; ΔCFI =.00; ΔRMSEA=.01. The latent mean gender differences were nearly the same, with women scoring higher on the general factor, and men scoring higher on the AAB specific factor.
References
- Aggen SH, Neale MC, Roysamb E, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Kendler KS. A psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV borderline personality disorder criteria: age and sex moderation of criterion functioning. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39(12):1967–1978. doi: 10.1017/s0033291709005807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alegria AA, Blanco C, Petry NM, Skodol AE, Liu SM, Grant B, Hasin D. Sex Differences in Antisocial Personality Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2013;4(3):214–222. doi: 10.1037/a0031681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson KG, Sankis LM, Widiger TA. Pathology versus statistical infrequency: Potential sources of gender bias in personality disorder criteria. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2001;189(10):661–668. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200110000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banducci AN, Dahne J, Magidson JF, Chen K, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. Clinical characteristics as a function of referral status among substance users in residential treatment. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38(4):1924–1930. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, Klein DN, Crowell SE, Derbidge C, Gatzke-Kopp L. Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A Biology× Sex× Environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21(03):735–770. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beeney JE, Stepp SD, Hallquist MN, Scott LN, Wright AGC, Ellison WD, … Pilkonis PA. Attachment and Social Cognition in Borderline Personality Disorder: Specificity in Relation to Antisocial and Avoidant Personality Disorders. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2015;6(3):207–215. doi: 10.1037/per0000110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biderman M. Applications of bifactor models to Big Five Data. Paper presented at the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology; Houston, TX. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Biderman M, Nguyen N, Cunningham C, Chen Z, Watson P. Method factors, bifactors, and item valence 2013 [Google Scholar]
- Black DW, Gunter T, Allen J, Blum N, Arndt S, Wenman G, Sieleni B. Borderline personality disorder in male and female offenders newly committed to prison. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2007;48(5):400–405. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black DW, Gunter T, Loveless P, Allen J, Sieleni B. Antisocial personality disorder in incarcerated offenders: Psychiatric comorbidity and quality of life. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 2010;22(2):113–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackburn R, Logan C, Donnelly J, Renwick S. Personality disorders, psychopathy and other mental disorders: co-morbidity among patients at English and Scottish high-security hospitals. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology. 2003;14(1):111–137. doi: 10.1080/1478994031000077925. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG. Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: A longitudinal-biometric study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115(1):85–95. doi: 10.1037/0021-943x.115.1.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova MA, Daughters SB, Hernandez GD, Richards JB, Lejuez CW. Differences in impulsivity and risk-taking propensity between primary users of crack cocaine and primary users of heroin in a residential substance-use program. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2005;13(4):311–318. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.4.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova MA, Hicks BM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Stability, change, and heritability of borderline personality disorder traits from adolescence to adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21(4):1335–1353. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova MA, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG, McGue M. Development and validation of the Minnesota Borderline Personality Disorder scale. Assessment. 2011;18(2):234–252. doi: 10.1177/1073191111398320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling-a Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14(3):464–504. [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Bohman M. Type I and type II alcoholism: An update. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1996;20(1):18–23. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen P, Chen H, Crawford TN, Brook JS, Gordon K. Personality disorders in early adolescence and the development of later substance use disorders in the general population. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88:S71–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway KP, Kane RJ, Ball SA, Poling JC, Rounsaville BJ. Personality, substance of choice, and polysubstance involvement among substance dependent patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2003;71(1):65–75. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(03)00068-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeShong HL, Kurtz JE. Four factors of impulsivity differentiate antisocial and borderline personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2013;27(2):144–156. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.2.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton NR, Krueger RF, Keyes KM, Skodol AE, Markon KE, Grant BF, Hasin DS. Borderline personality disorder co-morbidity: relationship to the internalizing-externalizing structure of common mental disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41(5):1041–1050. doi: 10.1017/s0033291710001662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eitle D, Turner RJ. Exposure to community violence and young adult crime: The effects of witnessing violence, traumatic victimization, and other stressful life events. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2002;39(2):214–237. [Google Scholar]
- Few LR, Miller JD, Rothbaum AO, Meller S, Maples J, Terry DP, … MacKillop J. Examination of the Section III DSM-5 Diagnostic System for Personality Disorders in an Outpatient Clinical Sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122(4):1057–1069. doi: 10.1037/a0034878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer Robert L, Gibbon Miriam, Williams Janet BW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP) New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Fossati A, Krueger RF, Markon KE, Borroni S, Maffei C. Reliability and Validity of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5): Predicting DSM-IV Personality Disorders and Psychopathy in Community-Dwelling Italian Adults. Assessment. 2013;20(6):689–708. doi: 10.1177/1073191113504984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken IHA, Hendriks VM. Early-onset of illicit substance use is associated with greater axis-II comorbidity, not with axis-I comorbidity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2000;59(3):305–308. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00132-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Huang B, Stinson FS, Saha TD, … Ruan WJ. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;69(4):533–545. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamdi NR, Iacono WG. Lifetime prevalence and co-morbidity of externalizing disorders and depression in prospective assessment. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44(2):315–324. doi: 10.1017/s0033291713000627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hengartner MP, Ajdacic-Gross V, Rodgers S, Mueller M, Roessler W. The joint structure of normal and pathological personality: Further evidence for a dimensional model. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):667–674. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hicks BM, Vaidyanathan U, Patrick CJ. Validating Female Psychopathy Subtypes: Differences in Personality, Antisocial and Violent Behavior, Substance Abuse, Trauma, and Mental Health. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2010;1(1):38–57. doi: 10.1037/a0018135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoertel N, Peyre H, Wall MM, Limosin F, Blanco C. Examining sex differences in DSM-IV borderline personality disorder symptom expression using Item Response Theory (IRT) Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2014;59:213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Baker KL, Morey LC. Personality and drugs of choice. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(6):1413–1421. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Malone JC, Ansell EB, Sanislow CA, Gill CM, McGlashan TH, … Morey LC. Personality assessment in DSM-5: empirical support for rating severity, style, and traits. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2011;25(3):305–320. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2011.25.3.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Edelen MO, Shea MT, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, … Skodol AE. A comparison of interview and self-report methods for the assessment of borderline personality disorder criteria. Psychological Assessment. 2008;20(1):81–85. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.20.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L-t, Bentler PM. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling-a Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt E, Bornovalova MA, Patrick CJ. Genetic and environmental overlap between borderline personality disorder traits and psychopathy: evidence for promotive effects of factor 2 and protective effects of factor 1. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(7):1471–1481. doi: 10.1017/s0033291714002608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt E, Bornovalova MA, Kimonis ER, Lilienfeld SO, Poythress NG. Psychopathy Factor Interactions and Co-Occurring Psychopathology: Does Measurement Approach Matter? Psychological Assessment. 2015;27(2):583–595. doi: 10.1037/pas0000055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahng S, Trull TJ, Wood PK, Tragesser SL, Tomko R, Grant JD, … Sher KJ. Distinguishing General and Specific Personality Disorder Features and Implications for Substance Dependence Comorbidity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120(3):656–669. doi: 10.1037/a0023539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James LM, Taylor J. Revisiting the structure of mental disorders: Borderline personality disorder and the internalizing/externalizing spectra. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008;47:361–380. doi: 10.1348/014466508x299691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jane JS, Oltmanns TF, South SC, Turkheimer E. Gender bias in diagnostic criteria for personality disorders: An item response theory analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116(1):166–175. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.116.1.166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BA, Cloninger CR, Roache JD, Bordnick PS, Ruiz P. Age of onset as a discriminator between alcoholic subtypes in a treatment-seeking outpatient population. American Journal on Addictions. 2000;9(1):17–27. doi: 10.1080/10550490050172191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson DM, Shea MT, Yen S, Battle CL, Zlotnick C, Sanislow CA, … Zanarini MC. Gender differences in borderline personality disorder: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2003;44(4):284–292. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(03)0090-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Knudsen GP, Roysamb E, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The Structure of Genetic and Environmental Risk Factors for Syndromal and Subsyndromal Common DSM-IV Axis I and All Axis II Disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168(1):29–39. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao SY, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, … Kendler KS. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51(1):8–19. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokkevi A, Stefanis N, Anastasopoulou E, Kostogianni C. Personality disorders in drug abusers: Prevalence and their association with AXIS I disorders as predictors of treatment retention. Addictive Behaviors. 1998;23(6):841–853. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(98)00071-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lechner WV, Dahne J, Chen KW, Pickover A, Richards JM, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. The prevalence of substance use disorders and psychiatric disorders as a function of psychotic symptoms. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2013;131(1–2):78–84. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF, Lane MC, Loranger AW, Kessler RC. DSM-IV personality disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62(6):553–564. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livesley WJ. An empirically-based classification of personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2011;25(3):397–420. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2011.25.3.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Rivas GE, Stark S, Chernyshenko OS. The effects of referent item parameters upon DIF detection using the free-baseline likelihood ratio test. Applied Psychological Measurement. 2009;33:251– 265. [Google Scholar]
- McGlashan TH, Grilo CM, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Morey LC, … Stout RL. The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study: Baseline Axis I/II and II/II diagnostic co-occurrence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102(4):256–264. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004256.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey LC, Warner MB, Boggs CD. Gender bias in the personality disorders criteria: An investigation of five bias indicators. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2002;24(1):55–65. doi: 10.1023/a:1014005308914. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 7. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2015. [Google Scholar]
- Paris J. Antisocial and borderline personality disorders: Two separate diagnoses or two aspects of the same psychopathology? Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1997;38(4):237–242. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(97)90032-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paris J, Chenard-Poirier MP, Biskin R. Antisocial and borderline personality disorders revisited. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2013;54(4):321–325. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pincus AL, Wiggins JS. Interpersonal problems and conceptions of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. 1990:342–352. [Google Scholar]
- Reise SP. The Rediscovery of Bifactor Measurement Models. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2012;47(5):667–696. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2012.715555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott LN, Kim Y, Nolf KA, Hallquist MN, Wright AGC, Stepp SD, … Pilkonis PA. Preoccupied attachment and emotional dysregulation: Specific aspects of borderline personality disorder or general dimensions of personality pathology? Journal of Personality Disorders. 2013;27(4):473–495. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2013_27_099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp C, Michonski J, Steinberg L, Fowler JC, Frueh BC, Oldham JM. An Investigation of Differential Item Functioning Across Gender of BPD Criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2014;123(1):231–236. doi: 10.1037/a0035637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol AE, Bender DS. Why are women diagnosed borderline more than men? Psychiatric Quarterly. 2003;74(4):349–360. doi: 10.1023/a:1026087410516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprague J, Javdani S, Sadeh N, Newman JP, Verona E. Borderline Personality Disorder as a Female Phenotypic Expression of Psychopathy? Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2012;3(2):127–139. doi: 10.1037/a0024134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprock J, Crosby JP, Nielsen BA. Effects of sex and sex roles on the perceived maladaptiveness of DSM-IV personality disorder symptoms. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2001;15(1):41–59. doi: 10.1521/pedi.15.1.41.18648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark S, Chernyshenko OS, Drasgow F. Detecting differential item functioning with CFA and IRT: Toward a unified strategy. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2006;19:1292–1306. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp SD, Olino TM, Klein DN, Seeley JR, Lewinsohn PM. Unique Influences of Adolescent Antecedents on Adult Borderline Personality Disorder Features. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2013;4(3):223–229. doi: 10.1037/per0000015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tadic A, Wagner S, Hoch J, Baskaya O, von Cube R, Skaletz C, … Dahmen N. Gender Differences in Axis I and Axis II Comorbidity in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder. Psychopathology. 2009;42(4):257–263. doi: 10.1159/000224149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgersen S, Czajkowski N, Jacobson K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Roysamb E, Neale MC, Kendler KS. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV cluster B personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a multivariate study. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(11):1617–1625. doi: 10.1017/s0033291708002924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrens M, Gilchrist G, Domingo-Salyany A, PsyCoBarcelona G. Psychiatric comorbidity in illicit drug users: Substance-induced versus independent disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;113(2–3):147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ. Relationships of borderline features to parental mental illness, childhood abuse, axis I disorder, and current functioning. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2001;15(1):19–32. doi: 10.1521/pedi.15.1.19.18647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ, Jahng S, Tomko RL, Wood PK, Sher KJ. Revised NESARC personality disorder diagnoses: Gender, prevalence, and comorbidity with substance dependence disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2010;24(4):412–426. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verona E, Sprague J, Javdani S. Gender and Factor-Level Interactions in Psychopathy: Implications for Self-Directed Violence Risk and Borderline Personality Disorder Symptoms. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2012;3(3):247–262. doi: 10.1037/a0025945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang WC. Effects of anchor item methods on the detection of differential item functioning within the family of Rasch models. Journal of Experimental Education. 2004;72:221–261. [Google Scholar]
- White CN, Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC, Hudson JI. Family studies of borderline personality disorder: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2003;11(1):8–19. doi: 10.1080/10673220303937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf EJ, Miller MW, Brown TA. The Structure of Personality Disorders in Individuals With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment. 2011;2(4):261–278. doi: 10.1037/a0023168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Chauncey DL, Gunderson JG. The diagnostic interview for personality disorders: interrater and test-retest reliability. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1987;28(6):467–480. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(87)90012-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Dubo ED, Sickel AE, Trikha A, Levin A, Reynolds V. Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1998;39(5):296–302. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(98)90038-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Reich DB, Silk KR. Axis I comorbidity in patients with borderline personality disorder: 6-year follow-up and prediction of time to remission. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(11):2108–2114. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Khera GS, Bleichmar J. Treatment histories of borderline inpatients. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):144–150. doi: 10.1053/comp.2001.19749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini MC, Williams AA, Lewis RE, Reich RB, Vera SC, Marino MF, … Frankenburg FR. Reported pathological childhood experiences associated with the development of borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(8):1101–1106. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.8.1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zinbarg RE, Revelle W, Yovel I, Li W. Cronbach’s alpha, Revelle’s beta, and McDonald’s (omega H) : Their relations with each other and two alternative conceptualizations of reliability. Psychometrika. 2005;70(1):123–133. doi: 10.1007/s11336-003-0974-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zlotnick C, Rothschild L, Zimmerman M. The role of gender in the clinical presentation of patients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2002;16(3):277–282. doi: 10.1521/pedi.16.3.277.22540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wüstenberg T, Hein J, Kienast T, Kahnt T, … Heinz A. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological psychiatry. 2009;66(8):734–742. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Distel MA, Trull TJ, de Moor MMH, Vink JM, Geels LM, van Beek JHDA, … Boomsma DI. Borderline personality traits and substance use: Genetic factors underlie the association with smoking and ever use of cannabis, but not with high alcohol consumption. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2012;26(6):867–879. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2012.26.6.867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James LM, Taylor J. Revisiting the structure of mental disorders: Borderline personality disorder and the internalizing/externalizing spectra. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008;47:361–380. doi: 10.1348/014466508x299691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146(4):373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Knudsen GP, Røysamb E, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for syndromal and subsyndromal common DSM-IV axis I and all axis II disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168(1):29–39. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Must A, Szabó Z, Bódi N, Szász A, Janka Z, Kéri S. Sensitivity to reward and punishment and the prefrontal cortex in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2006;90(2):209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauder CL, Derbidge CM, Beauchaine TP. Neural responses to monetary incentives among self-injuring adolescent girls. Development and Psychopathology. 2016;28(01):277–291. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, et al. Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychological Medicine. 2012;42(9):1879–1890. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.