SUMMARY
Attention is critical to perception, serving to select behaviorally relevant information for privileged processing. To understand the neural mechanisms of attention, we must discern how attentional modulation varies by cell-type and across cortical layers. Here we test whether attention acts non-selectively across cortical layers or does it engage the laminar circuit in specific and selective ways. We find layer- and cell-class specific differences in several different forms of attentional modulation in area V4. Broad-spiking neurons in the superficial layers exhibit attention mediated increases in firing rate and decreases in variability. Spike-count correlations are highest in the input layer and attention serves to reduce these correlations. Superficial and input layer neurons exhibit attention dependent decreases in low-frequency (<10Hz) coherence, but deep layer neurons exhibit increases in coherence in the beta and gamma frequency ranges. Our study provides a template for attention-mediated laminar information processing that might be applicable across sensory modalities.
INTRODUCTION
Spatial attention is a critical component of our perceptual system. It mediates the enhancement of task-relevant signals, suppression of distractor signals and reduction of noise among sensory neurons. Traditional single-unit electrophysiology has provided key insights into the probable mechanisms of attentional filtering of sensory information by measuring the attentional modulation of signals in sensory areas such as visual area V4 (see (Knudsen, 2007; Reynolds and Chelazzi, 2004) for review). These include modulation of mean firing rate (McAdams and Maunsell, 1999; Reynolds et al., 2000), increased reliability in the firing of individual neurons (Mitchell et al., 2007) and reduction in co-variability among pairs of neurons (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009), all of which are thought to improve the signal to noise ratio of neurons encoding the sensory stimulus. These changes in neuronal response are thought to result from feedback signals generated in attentional control centers (such as in the prefrontal and parietal cortices; see (Squire et al., 2013) for a review), which impinge upon the neural circuits of the sensory cortices.
The mammalian sensory cortex with its columnar organization (Mountcastle, 1997) is a six-layered (‘laminar’) structure with a canonical circuit organization composed of excitatory and local inhibitory interneurons that have distinct patterns of projection within and between layers and to other cortical and sub-cortical areas (Callaway, 1998; Douglas and Martin, 2007; 2004). For example, input layer neurons project locally to superficial and deep layers; superficial layer neurons project to higher order visual areas and also locally to superficial and deep layers; deep layer neurons project primarily to sub-cortical nuclei that are involved in motor control. This anatomical organization is a repeated circuit motif that is replicated throughout the sensory neocortex (see (Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013) for a review). Understanding the functional role of this circuit could provide a template for canonical information processing principles in the neocortex.
To understand the cortical mechanisms of attention, it is therefore critical to investigate the laminar organization of attentional modulation of sensory information. Previous attempts to record from cortical columns in V4 and obtain reliable layer estimates have proved to be challenging, since dorsal V4 straddles a narrow gyrus, with only a narrow strip of cortex (~5mm) parallel to the calvarium (Gattass et al., 1988). We overcame this challenge by removing the opaque dura mater and replacing it with a transparent silicone based artificial dura (Fig 1A). This allowed us to precisely target laminar probes at specific cortical sites under visual guidance through a microscope, and thereby record neuronal signals with reliable estimates of laminar location.
Fig 1. Laminar electrophysiology through an artificial dura.
(A) An artificial dura (AD) chamber is shown over dorsal V4 in the right hemisphere of Monkey A. The native dura mater was resected and replaced with a silicone based artificial dura, thereby providing an optically clear window into the cortex. Scale bar = 5mm. (B) An enlarged view of the boxed region in A clearly shows the sulci and the microvasculature. sts = superior temporal sulcus, lu = lunate sulcus, io = inferior occipital sulcus. Area V4 lies on the pre-lunate gyrus between the superior temporal and lunate sulci. Scale bar = 2mm. (C) Electrophysiology setup: a plastic stabilizer with a circular aperture is secured in place inside the chamber such that the aperture is centered over the pre-lunate gryus. A 16-channel linear array electrode (electrode spacing 150 μm) is positioned over the center of the gyrus and lowered into the cortex under microscopic guidance. The microvasculature pattern was used a reference to target different cortical sites across recording sessions.
Given that the cortical layers exhibit different projection patterns, it is important to know how attentional modulation varies across layers. One possibility is that attention serves to improve signals in superficial layer neurons that project to higher cortical areas. Alternatively, attentional modulation might be strongest among deep layer neurons that project to sub-cortical nuclei involved in motor control. A third possibility is that modulation could alter local processing: primarily modulating the input layers that project locally within the cortical column.
RESULTS
We recorded neuronal responses from well-isolated single-units, multi-unit activity and local field potentials using linear array electrodes oriented normal to the surface of area V4 of two rhesus macaques. This was achieved using artificial dura chambers (see Experimental Procedures; Fig 1). We took advantage of the optical clarity of the artificial dura to target 16-channel linear array electrodes to cortical sites near the center of the pre-lunate gyrus where the cortex is maximally flat with respect to the calvarium. This resulted in penetrations that were perpendicular to the cortical surface, as indicated by excellent alignment of receptive fields through the entire depth of cortex (Fig 2D, Fig S1A) We used current source-density (CSD) analysis (Mitzdorf, 1985) to estimate the boundaries between different cortical layers. The CSD, defined as the second spatial derivative of local field potential (LFP) signals, produces a map of local current sinks and sources down the cortical depth as a function of time (Fig 2B, Fig S1B). This allows us to identify the superficial (Layers 1–3), input (Layer 4) and deep (Layers 5–6) layers of the cortex (Bollimunta et al., 2008; Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009; Schroeder et al., 1998). The location of the earliest current sink followed by a reversal to current source was identified as the input layer. The superficial and deep layers had complementary sink-source patterns (source followed by sink). Fig 2C shows line traces averaged across all channels classified by layer, illustrating the average sink-source pattern differences between layers. Isolated single units and multi-unit clusters (see Experimental Procedures) were then assigned to one of the three identified laminar compartments: superficial, input or deep. The high quality of unit isolation allowed us to differentiate between narrow-spiking (putative interneurons) and broad-spiking (putative pyramidal) neurons among the majority of neurons recorded across all cortical layers (Fig 3; see Experimental Procedures) (Mitchell et al., 2007).
Fig 2. Laminar identification and receptive fields in a V4 cortical column.
(A) Stimulus triggered local field potentials (LFPs) obtained by flashing 30ms high contrast ring stimuli in the receptive field of a V4 cortical column. LFP traces averaged across all stimulus repeats are shown color-coded as being part of either the superficial (green), input (gray) or deep (pink) layers. Layer assignment was done after current source-density analysis. (B) Current source-density (CSD) calculated as the second spatial derivative of the stimulus triggered LFPs and displayed as a colored map. The x-axis represents time from stimulus onset; the y-axis represents cortical depth oriented such that the pial surface is at the top and the white matter is at the bottom. Red hues represent current sink, blue hues represent current source. The input layer is identified at the first current sink followed by a reversal to current source. The superficial and deep layers have the opposite sink-source pattern. The CSD map has been spatially smoothed for visualization. (C) CSD represented as line traces averaged across all channels that are part of a layer (same color coding convention as in A). Upward deflections represent current sink; downward deflections represent current source. Mean +/− s.e.m. (D) Stacked contour plots show spatial receptive fields (RFs) mapped along each contact point in the laminar probe. The spatial receptive fields were obtained by applying reverse correlation to the LFP power evoked by sparse pseudo-random sequences of Gabor stimuli. The RFs are well aligned, indicating perpendicular penetration down a cortical column. Zero depth represents the center of the input layer as estimated from the CSD.
Fig 3. Single units recorded in V4 cortical columns.
(A) Example recording session in monkey C depicting single unit waveforms (mean +/− s.e.m.) isolated along the cortical column. Blue waveforms correspond to narrow-spiking putative interneurons, orange waveforms correspond to broad-spiking putative excitatory units and gray waveforms are single units that could not be classified as narrow or broad (see Experiment Procedures). (B) Average action potential waveforms for all 274 visually responsive single units in our population split by layer and neuronal class (blue=narrow, orange=broad, gray=unclassified). Waveform heights have been normalized for ease of comparison between the narrow- and broad-spiking units. The reference line (dotted) at 225 μs was used to classify the waveforms into the narrow and broad categories (see Experiment Procedures). (C) Distribution of action potential waveform widths (trough-to-peak duration) for all narrow- and broad-spiking units collapsed across layers. The distribution is clearly bimodal (p = 0.012, Hartigan’s dip test).
To investigate the laminar organization of attentional modulation, we recorded neuronal responses in two monkeys trained to perform an attention-demanding orientation-change detection task (see Experimental Procedures; Fig 4). Attention was cued to one of two spatial locations. In the ‘attend in’ condition, the monkeys were instructed to covertly attend to a spatial location within the area of receptive field (RF) overlap of the neurons recorded throughout the V4 cortical column, while maintaining fixation at a central fixation spot. In the ‘attend-away’ condition attention was cued to an equally eccentric location across the vertical meridian. During each trial, a sequence of oriented Gabor stimuli (baseline orientation optimized for each recording session) simultaneously flashed on and off at both spatial locations (200ms on, variable 200–400ms inter-stimulus intervals). At an unpredictable time (minimum 1s, maximum 5s), one of the two stimuli (95% probability at cued location; 5% probability at uncued location, ‘foil trials’) briefly changed in orientation (200ms) and the monkey was rewarded for making a saccade to the location of orientation change (Fig 4A). If no change occurred within 5s, the monkey was rewarded for holding fixation (‘catch trial’). We controlled task difficulty by varying the degree of orientation change and thereby obtained behavioral performance curves (psychometric functions) for each recording session (Fig 4B). Impaired performance (Fig 4B, square symbol) and slower reaction times (Fig 4C, square symbol) were observed for the foil trials, indicating that the monkey was indeed using the spatial cue in performing the task.
Fig 4. Attention task and behavior.
(A) While the monkey maintained fixation, two oriented Gabor stimuli (schematized as oriented bars) flashed on and off simultaneously at two spatial locations: one at the RF overlap region of the recorded V4 column, the other at a location of equal eccentricity across the vertical meridian. The monkey was cued to covertly attend to one of the two locations. At an unpredictable time, one of the two stimuli changed in orientation. The monkey was rewarded for making a saccade to the location of orientation change at either location (95% probability of change at cued location; 5% probability at non-cued location [foil trials]). If no change occurred (catch trials), the monkey was rewarded for maintaining fixation. (B) Example behavioral session showing performance (hit rate) as a function of task difficulty (size of orientation change). Square symbol: foil trial performance. Star symbol: catch trial performance. Error bars are std. dev. obtained by a jackknife procedure and corrected for the number of jackknives (20). The data has been fitted with a smooth logistic function. (C) Reaction time as a function of task difficulty. The data has been fitted with a linear regression line. Performance is degraded and reaction times are higher for the foil trials, indicating that the animal was indeed deploying attention to the spatially cued location.
While the monkey was performing the attention task, we simultaneously recorded neuronal data from V4 cortical columns (Fig S2). We analyzed the data from visually responsive single units (SU, n=274) and multi-unit activity (MUA, n=217) (see Inclusion criteria in Experimental Methods; Fig S3; Supp Table 1). To quantify the effects of attentional modulation, we focused our analyses on correct trials, where we had the best behavioral evidence that the monkey was deploying attention as instructed by the cue (see Experimental Procedures).
We find that attention positively modulates firing rate across all cortical layers (Fig 5A, SU and MUA, all distributions are significantly greater than zero, p ≪ 0.01), but that the magnitude varies significantly by layer, with its greatest effects in the input layers. Attentional modulation of mean firing rate is significantly larger in the input layer compared to the superficial and deep layers (pinput↔superficial ≪ 0.01, pinput↔deep = 0.01). Next, we examined cell-class specific rate modulation for broad- and narrow-spiking single-units. A 2-way ANOVA of the rate modulation indices with the factors ‘unit’ (narrow, broad) and ‘layer’ (superficial, input, deep) revealed a significant main effect of both factors (Funit = 12.33, p = 0.0005; Flayer = 7.39, p = 0.008), but no significant interactions between the factors. Further analysis reveals that the broad-spiking population is significantly modulated across all cortical layers (psuperficial = 0.01, pinput ≪ 0.01, pdeep ≪ 0.01), whereas the narrow-spiking population is significantly modulated in the input layer (psuperficial = 0.25, pinput = 0.003, pdeep = 0.35) (Fig 5B).
Fig 5. Attention mediated firing rate modulation is strongest in the input layer.
(A) Histograms depict the distribution of firing rate modulation indices (see Experimental Procedures) across layers for all units (single units, multi-unit activity) in a particular layer. Solid vertical lines depict the median values of each distribution. All distributions are significantly greater than zero (p ≪ 0.01). Rate modulation for the input layer is significantly higher than the other two layers (pinput↔superficial ≪ 0.01, pinput↔deep = 0.01). (B) Rate modulation indices for the narrow- and broad-spiking single unit populations (narrow=blue, broad=orange). The broad-spiking units exhibit significant positive rate modulation due to attention across all layers (psuperficial = 0.01, pinput ≪ 0.01, pdeep ≪ 0.01). The narrow-spiking units exhibit significant modulation in the input layer (psuperficial = 0.25, pinput = 0.003, pdeep = 0.35). Star symbols in A and B indicate statistically significant modulations.
We calculated trial-to-trial variability in individual units by estimating the Fano-factor (trial-to-trial spike-count variance divided by the mean). We find that the broad-spiking population in the superficial layers exhibits significant reduction in Fano-factor due to attention (p < 0.01, Fig 6, top panel). This reduction cannot be attributed to differences in firing rate between the two attention conditions (Fig S5C). The broad-spiking population in the input layer also exhibits a trend in reduction of variability due to attention, but the reduction is not statistically significant (p = 0.06, Fig 6, middle panel).
Fig 6. Attention reduces trial-to-trial variability among broad-spiking cells in the superficial layer.
Histograms depict the distribution of Fano-factor modulation indices (see Experimental Procedures) across layers for the narrow- and broad-spiking single unit populations (narrow=blue, broad=orange). The broad-spiking population in the superficial layer exhibits significant reduction in Fano-factor (i.e. reduced trial-to-trial variability) due to attention (p < 0.01). Although the broad-spiking units in the input layer also exhibit a reduction in variability, this reduction is not significant (p = 0.06). Star symbol indicates statistically significant modulation.
We next examined the laminar profile of spike-count correlations among simultaneously recorded single units in V4. A 3-way ANOVA of spike-count correlations with the factors ‘attention’ (attend-in, attend-away), ‘epoch’ (inter-stimulus period, stimulus-evoked period) and ‘layer’ (superficial, input, deep) revealed a significant main effect of all three factors (Fattention = 4.03, p = 0.04; Fepoch = 36.17, p ≪ 0.01; Flayer = 32.85, p ≪ 0.01), but no significant interactions between the factors. Upon detailed analysis, we find two surprising results (Fig 7). First, correlated variability in V4 is highest in the input layer, resulting in an inverted “U” profile. This is true for both the inter-stimulus period (pinput↔superficial ≪ 0.01, pinput↔deep ≪ 0.01; Fig 7A, blue-bordered bars) and the stimulus-evoked period (pinput↔superficial = 0.005, pinput↔deep ≪ 0.01; Fig 7B, blue-bordered bars) in the attend-away condition. The same inverted “U” profile holds for the pre-stimulus period in the attend-in condition (pinput↔superficial ≪ 0.01, pinput↔deep ≪ 0.01; Fig 7A, red-bordered bars). This is in contrast to the laminar profile of correlated variability in V1, where co-variability is highest in the superficial and deep layers and lowest in the input layer, resulting in a “U” shaped profile across cortical layers (Hansen et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). If the laminar profile of correlated variability had a canonical organization in the visual cortex (Hansen et al., 2012), we would have expected to find a similar organization in V4. The second surprising finding is that attention reduces spike-count correlations predominantly in the presence of a stimulus in the input layer (p = 0.03; Fig 7B). This is in contrast to expectations setup by prior studies which find strong evidence that deployment of attention reduces correlated variability among V4 neurons (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009). These two studies estimated on theoretical grounds that this decorrelation accounted for about 80% of the perceptual benefit due to attention. Since these signals presumably affect perception by transmitting improved signals to other brain areas, a natural expectation would be that decorrelation would be pronounced in the output layers of the cortex. The higher co-variability that we observe in the input layer cannot be attributed to differences in firing rate across layers (Fig S5A), nor can it be attributed to differences in cortical distance between recorded pairs of neurons across layers (Fig S5B). Moreover, our results are consistent across monkeys (Fig S6).
Fig 7. Attention reduces correlated variability most prominently in the input layer.
Spike count correlations (rSC) between pairs of simultaneously recorded single units within a layer are shown as a function of attention condition for two different temporal windows: an inter-stimulus period 200ms before stimulus onset (A) and a stimulus-evoked period 60–260ms after stimulus onset (B). A noteworthy aspect is that the laminar profile of rSC in V4 has an inverted U shape, with the highest levels of correlated variability in the input layer. This is in contrast to the laminar profile reported in upstream V1, which has a U shape with correlations highest in superficial and deep layers (Hansen et al., 2012). Correlated variability is reduced in the stimulus-evoked period across all layers. Attention significantly reduces correlated variability in the stimulus-evoked period in the input layer (p = 0.03). (C) Spike-spike coherence (SSC) – a frequency resolved measure of the degree to which the spiking activity of one unit fluctuates with that of a second unit – among pairs of simultaneously recorded single units is plotted as a function of layer and attention condition. The dotted lines represent the baseline coherence that is expected solely due to trends in firing time-locked to stimulus identity. Attention reduces low-frequency SSC in the superficial and input layers. The reduction is most prominent in the input layer. Attention increases high-frequency SSC in the deep layer. Mean +/− s.e.m. (D) Same data as in C, but plotted as a modulation index: (SSCin − SSCaway)/SSCin + SSCaway) for the indicated frequency bands.
To investigate the laminar aspects of co-variability further, we examined the spike-spike coherence (SSC) among pairs of simultaneously recorded single-units. The SSC is a frequency resolved measure of the degree to which the spiking activity of one unit fluctuates with that of a second unit. We chose SSC over spike-field coherence (SFC) analyses used in some other studies (Buffalo et al., 2011; Chalk et al., 2010; Fries et al., 2001), so as to allow direct comparison of the present results with those of Mitchell et al, 2009. Moreover, the short window of analyses did not lend itself well to evaluating SFC. Consistent with the modulation of spike count correlation, we find that the previously reported reduction in low-frequency SSC (Mitchell et al., 2009) due to attention is strongest in the input layer with significant, but weaker modulation in superficial layers (Fig 7C). This attention-dependent modulation is better appreciated as a modulation index, where we see a larger negative modulation in the input layer (Fig 7D, middle panel, p ≪ 0.01) compared to the superficial layers for frequencies less than 10Hz (Fig 7D, top panel; p = 0.05). Interestingly, the deep layer exhibits a different pattern of modulation. Here we see an increase in SSC due to attention for frequencies above 10Hz (Fig 7C, lower panel), with positive modulation in the beta (15–25Hz, p ≪ 0.01) and gamma frequency (> 30Hz; p = 0.003) bands (Fig 7D, lower panel). That is, in the deep layers, attention increases, rather than decreases coherence.
To gain insight into the possible neural mechanisms underlying the observed variation in spike-count correlations across layers, we examined how correlations change as a function of the strength of excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) connections in a conductance-based model of spiking neurons (see Experimental Procedures). This investigation was driven by the observation that spiking activity was highest in the input layer in both the broad- and narrow-spiking populations (Fig S4), suggesting a strongly coupled E-I network in that layer. We set up networks of E and I units that were mutually coupled (Fig 8A) and performed simulations that generated spiking activity in the network in response to a step input (Fig 8B). We calculated spike-count correlations across repeated simulations of the network. We find that the strength of the feedback loop between the E and the I populations (WEI, WIE) is a critical factor in determining the strength of correlated activity in such a network. Holding the self-excitation and self-inhibition parameters fixed (WEE = 16, WII = −1), we see that a strong inhibitory feedback loop (larger absolute values of WEI and WIE) leads to higher correlated activity in the network (Fig 8C). A strong inhibitory feedback loop acts as a common signal that drives the correlated activity in the network. This result is robust and holds for a wide range of values of WEE and WII, especially so when inhibition is high in the network (low absolute values of WII; Fig S7B). The pattern of results also holds for different network connection probabilities (Fig S7C), although spike-count correlations decrease with increasing sparseness. This suggests a model in which the local circuit in the input layer in V4 is a tightly coupled E-I network leading to stronger correlated activity in this layer. On the other hand, the E-I local circuits in the superficial and deep layers are weakly coupled leading to weaker correlations in these layers (Fig 8D).
Fig 8. A computational model of differentially coupled E-I networks explains the laminar profile of correlated activity.
(A) Schematic of a local conductance based E-I network with mutually coupled excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) units. WEE = self-excitation among E units; WII = self-inhibition among I units; WIE = excitation provided by E units to I units; WEI = inhibition provided by I units to E units. (B) Simulation of a network of 800 E and 200 I units (WEE = 16, WII = −1, WIE = 8, WEI = −14). The raster plot shows the spiking activity for all units in the model (blue=E, orange=I) in response to a step input. The middle traces show the population-spiking rate of the E and I units. The box indicates a 200ms window used for calculating spike-count correlations among the units. (C) Spike-count correlations as a function of different values of the E-I coupling parameters WIE and WEI, while holding WEE and WII fixed. The shaded bars indicate the direction of stronger coupling. Spike-count correlations were calculated across 2000 repeats of identical stimulation to the network and averaged over four consecutive and non-overlapping 200ms windows (first window indicated by box in B). The solid white rectangle corresponds to the model parameters used for the simulation in B. A strong inhibitory feedback loop (larger absolute values of WEI and WIE) increases correlated activity. Conversely, weaker inhibitory cross-coupling decorrelates the network. The dotted white rectangle depicts a possible shift to a low-correlation regime due to attention (see Discussion). (D) Proposed E-I local circuits in a V4 cortical column. The empirical data and the model simulations suggest a strongly coupled E-I network in the input layer leading to stronger correlations in this layer. Weakly coupled E-I networks in the superficial and deep layers lead to weak correlations in these layers. The widths of the links indicate coupling strength. The gray arrows indicate the primary information flow pathways in a canonical columnar circuit.
DISCUSSION
We find layer- and cell-class specific differences in attentional modulation of mean firing rate, Fano factor, spike-count correlations and spike-spike coherence in area V4 of the macaque. Attention increases firing rates across all cortical layers, but this modulation is highest in the input layer. Broad-spiking units are significantly modulated across all layers; narrow-spiking units are significantly modulated in the input layer. Further, broad-spiking units in the superficial layer show a significant reduction in variability due to attention. Since the superficial layers project to downstream cortical areas, an elevation in firing rate and reduction in spiking variability among these units suggest their functional role in improving the signal quality of encoded signals under attention.
It is important to note that the method we have used to distinguish putative excitatory and inhibitory neurons (based on spike-waveform duration), does not unambiguously distinguish interneurons from pyramidal neurons. Although a majority of pyramidal cells do have broad action potential widths (McCormick et al., 1985; Nowak et al., 2003; Povysheva et al., 2006), there is evidence that pyramidal tract neurons in the motor cortex can fire “thin” spikes (Vigneswaran et al., 2011). Similarly, although parvalbumin positive interneurons with the morphology of basket cells and chandelier cells, which make up 75% of interneurons in the primate, have narrow action-potentials, the remaining 25% of interneurons have broad action potentials (Cauli et al., 1997; Connors and Gutnick, 1990; Kawaguchi, 1995; Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997).
Consistent with earlier studies, we find that correlated variability among neurons in area V4 is weaker than in the primary visual cortex (Smith and Sommer, 2013), and that the presence of a stimulus reduces spike count correlations (Fig 7A–B) (Kohn and Smith, 2005). However, one of the surprising results of our study is the inverted-“U” laminar profile of spike-count correlations in V4, which is in contrast with the “U” shaped profile in V1 (Hansen et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). This suggests that there might not be a canonical laminar organization of correlated variability in the sensory neocortex. The higher levels of correlated activity in the input layer could be attributed to two factors. Part of the correlated activity could be inherited as common inputs from the supra-granular layers of V1. Neurons in the supra-granular layers of V1 have the highest levels of correlated variability (Hansen et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013) and these layers include neurons that project to the parafoveal regions of V4 (Ungerleider et al., 2008) from which our data was collected. This higher level of correlated activity could also reflect the dynamics of a strongly coupled local E-I network in the input layer, as suggested by our data (Fig S4) and explored in our model (Fig 8 and Fig S7). Our model suggests a possible active mechanism for reducing these spike-count correlations in the superficial and deep layers by weakly coupled local E-I networks in these layers. According to this model, correlated variability results from local patterns of cortical circuit connectivity. In contrast to earlier work demonstrating extremely low correlations in recurrent network models where pre-synaptic activity is instantaneously integrated (Renart et al., 2010), our conductance-based model exhibits correlations that are consistent with our data.
Prior studies (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009) estimated on theoretical grounds that the reduction in correlated variability accounted for a large fraction of the perceptual benefit due to attention. For these reductions to benefit decision making in other parts of the brain, this reduction in correlated variability would need to be localized to the output layers of the cortical column in V4. Neurons in the output layers of V4 transmit signals to downstream areas for further processing and decorrelation among these neurons would be expected to improve the signal to noise of the neural code. To the contrary, while we do find modest changes in gain and correlation in the superficial layers, we find that attention reduces correlated variability primarily in the input layer. This unexpected result forces us to reconsider the functional role of decorrelation in mediating the perceptual benefit due to attention. Rather than serving to directly improve the signal to noise of the transmitted neural code, our results suggest that decorrelation in V4 is a local computation that serves to remove correlations from the inputs received from the earlier visual cortices. Further studies are needed to examine whether such a reduction plays a causal role in behavior.
A framework of the V4 laminar circuit
Microstimulation of Frontal Eye Field (FEF) neurons produces improvements in perception of targets appearing among distracters and also causes changes in V4 responses that mirror the effects of attention in V4 (Armstrong et al., 2006; Armstrong and Moore, 2007; Moore and Armstrong, 2003; Moore and Fallah, 2001). Together, these findings implicate FEF feedback to V4 as a key source of attentional modulation. These afferents originate mainly from superficial layer cells in FEF (Barone et al., 2000; Markov et al., 2011; Pouget et al., 2009). Our finding that attention significantly modulated the firing rate of broad-spiking neurons across all layers is consistent with anatomical evidence that FEF projections terminate in all cortical layers in V4, and that the labeled synapses of these projections are predominantly excitatory (Anderson et al., 2011). Direct anatomical pathways from FEF to inhibitory neurons in V4 are rare, making up only 4% of the total number of FEF synapses (Anderson et al., 2011). We thus speculate that the larger attentional modulation of putative parvalbumin expressing interneurons (narrow-spiking units) as compared to broad-spiking units (Mitchell et al., 2009) and that we observe in our data in the input layer (Supp Table 1, Fig S4), are likely mediated indirectly via excitatory neurons. These E-I interactions may mediate shifts in the local network to a decorrelated state (Fig 8C, arrow and dotted white rectangle). Such a shift, if most prominent in the input layer, would contribute to the suppression of input correlations to V4. Anderson et al. also found that individual FEF axons terminating in the V4 input layer formed sprays of collaterals, raising the possibility that these collaterals may play a prominent role in mediating attentional modulation. Finally, the differential modulation of coherence that we observe in our data is in agreement with the recent proposal that alpha and gamma band activity characterize feedback and feedforward processing respectively (van Kerkoerle et al., 2014), although we observe elevated gamma band activity only in the deep layers.
Relationship to prior studies of attention
Our data is consistent with prior studies in several aspects, but differs from them in important aspects of the laminar organization. Consistent with (Mitchell et al., 2007), we found that narrow-spiking neurons had higher firing rates than broad-spiking neurons, though this difference was only apparent in the input layer (compare Fig S4, middle panel, to Fig 2C of (Mitchell et al., 2007)). Data in (Mitchell et al., 2007) was collected with single tungsten electrodes, which might have biased the data toward units with higher firing rates. Our more unbiased exploration with linear array electrodes reveal that overall firing rates are higher in the input layer with the rate differences between broad- and narrow-spiking units consistent with (Mitchell et al., 2007). However, the superficial and deep layers with lower firing rates overall do not have this cell-class specific difference (Fig S4). The reduction in variability in our data is modest compared to (Mitchell et al., 2007). This could be due to differences in task and measurement durations between the two studies – in the stimulus tracking task of Mitchell et. al, the stimuli paused in the RF for one second, whereas in our task the stimuli were briefly flashed for 200ms. Our experimental paradigm was essentially similar to (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009) who used flashed gabors in an orientation discrimination task and reported a modest decrease in variability, consistent with the present findings.
(Mitchell et al., 2009) found a strong reduction in low-frequency correlated variability due to attention. We find a similar reduction in the superficial layers and the input layer, with the effect being larger in the input layer. However, we also find an attention mediated increase in coherence in the beta and gamma frequency range in the deep layers, consistent with earlier reports in V4 (Fries et al., 2001). Gamma band activity has been correlated with perceptual performance (Womelsdorf et al., 2006), while beta band activity has been implicated in motor behavior and top-down signaling (see (Wang, 2010) for a review). Activity in both bands have been also been implicated in modulating the efficiency of the oculomotor system underlying saccadic eye-movements (Bartlett et al., 2011; Drewes and VanRullen, 2011).
(Cohen and Maunsell, 2009) report a decrease in spike-count correlations with attention, which is consistent with our finding in the input layer. However, we do not find a significant attention-mediated change in spike-count correlations in the superficial and deep layers. (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009) recorded with array electrodes (“UTAH” arrays) with 1mm long electrodes and might have sampled primarily from the input layer of V4 (Bjornsson et al., 2006).
Our coherence results are somewhat at odds with those reported by (Buffalo et al., 2011). They report a decrease in alpha-band coherence and no appreciable change in gamma band coherence in the deep layers. We find decreases in low-frequency band coherence in the superficial and input layers and an increase in beta/gamma band coherence in the deep layers. This discrepancy could be due to differences in experimental paradigms. In the Buffalo et al. study, monkeys were attending to a high contrast slowly drifting grating, to detect a change in velocity. A second difference is the temporal window used for coherence analyses: Buffalo et al. used the sustained epoch with constant visual stimulation ignoring the first 300ms after stimulus onset for calculating SFC; in our study, we used the stimulus-evoked period 60–260ms after stimulus onset for calculating SSC among single-units only. These differences along with the fact that we used smaller, lower contrast stimuli are probable reasons why we do not see a peak in higher frequencies as in other studies (Buffalo et al., 2011; Chalk et al., 2010; Fries et al., 2001). A third key difference is in the methods used for layer estimation: layer assignment in (Buffalo et al., 2011) was based upon estimated penetration depth of single tungsten electrodes, while in the current study we have used the current-source density profile to determine layer identity.
Conclusion
We find that different aspects of attention-mediated modulation of neuronal activity – increase in firing rate, decreases in variability and co-variability – are functionally segregated in different layers and among different cell classes within a cortical column in V4. This suggests a division of labor that serves to improve both the fidelity of transmitted information and the quality of local computation. Our study provides a template for how attention influences laminar information processing that might be applicable across sensory modalities.
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
Surgical Procedures
Surgical procedures have been described in detail previously (Nassi et al., 2015; Ruiz et al., 2013). In brief, an MRI compatible low-profile titanium chamber was placed over the pre-lunate gyrus, on the basis of preoperative MRI imaging in two rhesus macaques (right hemisphere in Monkey A, left hemisphere in Monkey C). The native dura mater was then removed and a silicone based optically clear artificial dura (AD) was inserted, resulting in an optical window over dorsal V4 (Fig 1). All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed to NIH guidelines.
Electrophysiology
At the beginning of each recording session a plastic insert, with an opening for targeting electrodes, was lowered into the chamber and secured. This served to stabilize the recording site against cardiac pulsations. Neurons were recorded from cortical columns in dorsal V4 using 16-channel linear array electrodes (‘laminar probes’, Plexon Inc., Plexon V-probe). The laminar probes were mounted on adjustable X–Y stages attached to the recording chamber and positioned over the center of the pre-lunate gyrus under visual guidance through a microscope (Zeiss Inc.) (Fig 1C). This ensured that the probes were maximally perpendicular to the surface of the cortex and thus had the best possible trajectory to make a normal penetration down a cortical column. Across recording sessions, the probes were positioned over different sites along the center of the gyrus in the parafoveal region of V4 with receptive field (RF) eccentricities between 2 and 7 degrees of visual angle. Care was taken to target cortical sites with no surface micro-vasculature and in fact, the surface micro-vasculature was used as reference so that the same cortical site was not targeted across recording sessions. The probes were advanced using a hydraulic microdrive (Narishige Inc.) to first penetrate the AD and then through the cortex under microscopic visual guidance. Probes were advanced until the point that the top-most electrode (toward the pial surface) registered local field potential (LFP) signals. At this point, the probe was retracted by about 100–200 μm to ease the dimpling of the cortex due to the penetration. This procedure greatly increased the stability of the recordings and also increased the neuronal yield in the superficial electrodes.
The distance from the tip of the probes to the first electrode contact was either 300 μm or 700 μm. The inter-electrode distance was 150 μm, thus negating the possibility of recording the same neural spikes in adjacent recording channels. Neuronal signals were recorded extracellularly, filtered, and stored using the Multichannel Acquisition Processor system (Plexon Inc.). Neuronal signals were classified as either multi-unit clusters or isolated single units using the Plexon Offline Sorter program. Single units were identified based on two criteria: (a) if they formed an identifiable cluster, separate from noise and other units, when projected into the principal components of waveforms recorded on that electrode and (b) if the inter-spike interval (ISI) distribution had a well-defined refractory period. Single-units were classified as either narrow-spiking (putative inter-neurons) or broad-spiking (putative pyramidal cells) based on methods described in detail previously (Mitchell et al., 2007). Specifically, only units with waveforms having a clearly defined peak preceded by a trough were potential candidates. The distribution of trough-to-peak duration was clearly bimodal (Fig 3C, Hartigan’s Dip Test, p = 0.012) (J. A. Hartigan and P. M. Hartigan, 1985). Units with trough-to-peak duration less than 225 μs were classified as narrow-spiking units; units with trough-to-peak duration greater than 225 μs were classified as broad-spiking units (Fig 3; blue=narrow, orange=broad). Units that did not have the prototypical biphasic shape were unclassified (Fig 3, gray waveforms) and only included in some of the analyses where neuronal class identity was not considered.
Data was collected over 32 sessions (23 sessions in Monkey A, 9 in Monkey C), yielding a total of 413 single units (128 narrow-spiking, 209 broad-spiking, 76 unclassified) and 296 multi-unit clusters. Per session unit yield was considerably higher in Monkey C compared to Monkey A, resulting in a roughly equal contribution of both monkeys toward the population data.
Task, Stimuli and Inclusion Criteria
Stimuli were presented on a computer monitor placed 57 cm from the eye. Eye position was continuously monitored with an infrared eye tracking system (ISCAN ETL-200). Trials were aborted if eye position deviated more than 1° (degree of visual angle, ‘dva’) from fixation. Experimental control was handled by NIMH Cortex software (http://www.cortex.salk.edu/).
Receptive Field Mapping
At the beginning of each recording session, neuronal RFs were mapped using subspace reverse correlation in which Gabor (eight orientations, 80% luminance contrast, spatial frequency 1.2 cycles/degree, Gaussian half-width 2°) or ring stimuli (80% luminance contrast) appeared at 60 Hz while monkeys maintained fixation. Each stimulus appeared at a random location selected from an 11×11 grid with 1° spacing in the appropriate visual quadrant. Spatial receptive maps were obtained by applying reverse correlation to the evoked local field potential (LFP) signal at each recording site. For each spatial location in the 11×11 grid, we calculated the time-averaged power in the stimulus evoked LFP (0–200ms after each stimulus flash) at each recording site. The resulting spatial map of LFP power was taken as the spatial RF at the recording site. For the purpose of visualization, the spatial RF maps were smoothed using spline interpolation and displayed as stacked contours plots of the smoothed maps (Fig 2D, Fig S1A). All RFs were in the lower visual quadrant (lower-left in Monkey A, lower-right in Monkey C) and with eccentricities between 2 and 7 dva.
Current Source Density Mapping
In order to estimate the laminar identity of each recording channel, we used a current source-density (CSD) mapping procedure (Mitzdorf, 1985). Monkeys maintained fixation while 100% luminance contrast ring stimuli were flashed (30ms) centered at the estimated RF overlap region across all channels. The size of the ring was scaled to about three-quarters of the estimated diameter of the RF. CSD was calculated as the second spatial derivative of the flash-triggered LFPs (Fig 2A). The resulting time-varying traces of current across the cortical layers can be visualized as CSD maps (Fig 2B, Fig S1B; maps have been spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel for aid in visualization). Red regions depict current sinks in the corresponding region of the cortical laminae; blue regions depict current sources. The input layer (Layer 4) was identified as the first current sink followed by a reversal to current source. The superficial (Layers 1–3) and deep (Layers 5–6) layers had opposite sink-source patterns. LFPs and spikes from the corresponding recording channels were then assigned to one of three layers: superficial, input or deep.
Attention task
In the main experiment, monkeys had to perform an attention-demanding orientation change-detection task (Fig 4A). While the monkey maintained fixation, two achromatic Gabor stimuli (orientation optimized per recording session, spatial frequency 1.2 cycles/degree, 6 contrasts randomly chosen from an uniform distribution of luminance contrasts, c = [10, 18, 26, 34, 42, 50%]) were flashed on for 200ms and off for a variable period chosen from a uniform distribution between 200–400ms. One of the Gabors was flashed at the receptive field overlap region, the other at a location of equal eccentricity across the vertical meridian. At the beginning of a block of trials, the monkey was spatially cued (‘instruction trials’) to covertly attend to one of these two spatial locations. During these instruction trials, the stimuli were only flashed at the spatially cued location. At an unpredictable time (minimum 1s, maximum 5s, mean 3s), one of the two stimuli changed in orientation. The monkey was rewarded for making a saccade to the location of orientation change. The monkey was rewarded for only those saccades where the saccade onset time was within a window of 100–400ms after the onset of the orientation change. The orientation change occurred at the cued location with 95% probability and at the uncued location with 5% probability (‘foil trials’). We controlled task difficulty by varying the degree of orientation change (Δori), which was randomly chosen from one of the following: 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 10 and 12°. The orientation change in the foil trials was fixed at 4°. These foil trials allowed us to assess the extent to which the monkey was using the spatial cue, with the expectation that there would be an impairment in performance (Fig 4B) and slower reaction times (Fig 4C) compared to the case in which the change occurred at the cued location. If no change occurred before 5s, the monkey was rewarded for maintaining fixation (‘catch trials’, 13% of trials). We will refer to all stimuli at the baseline orientation as ‘non-targets’ and the stimulus flash with the orientation change as the ‘target’.
Inclusion criteria
Of the 413 single units and 296 multi-unit clusters, we included only a subset of neurons that were visually responsive for further analysis. For each neuron we calculated its baseline firing-rate for each attention condition (attend into RF [‘attend in’ or ‘IN’], attend away from RF [‘attend away’ or ‘AWAY’]) from a 200ms window before a stimulus flash. We also calculated the neuron’s contrast response function for both attention conditions (Fig S3). This was calculated as the firing rate over a window between 60–200ms after stimulus onset and averaged across all stimulus flashes (restricted to non-targets) of a particular contrast separately for each attention condition. A neuron was considered visually responsive if any part of the contrast response curves exceeded the baseline rate by 4 standard deviations for both attention conditions. This left us with 274 single units (76 narrow-spiking, 161 broad-spiking, 37 unclassified) and 217 multi-unit clusters for further analysis.
Data analysis
Behavioral Analysis
For each orientation change condition Δori, we calculated the hit rate as the ratio of the number of trials in which the monkey correctly identified the target with a saccade over the number of trials in which the target was presented. The hit rate as a function of Δori, yields a behavioral psychometric function (Fig 4B). Psychometric functions were fitted with a smooth logistic function (Palamedes MATLAB toolbox). Error bars were obtained by a jackknife procedure (20 jackknives, 5% of trials left out for each jackknife). Performance for the foil trials were calculated similarly as the hit rate for trials in which the orientation change occurred at the un-cued location (Fig 4B, square symbol). Performance for the catch trials was calculated as the fraction of trials in which the monkey correctly held fixation for trials in which there was no orientation change (Fig 4B, star symbol).
Firing-rate modulation index
As in the contrast response functions, we calculated the average firing rate across all non-target flashes of a particular contrast (time window 60–260ms after stimulus onset), separately for each attention condition (FRin(c), FRaway(c)). We only included stimulus flashes from correct trials (hit trials in which the monkey correctly detected a target or correct catch trials) for this and all subsequent analyses. The attentional rate modulation index (Fig 5) was calculated as the average modulation index across all contrast levels:
Fano-factor modulation index
Trial-to-trial variability was estimated by the Fano-factor, which is the ratio of the variance of the spike counts across trials over the mean of the spike counts. The Fano factor was calculated over non-overlapping 20ms time bins. The average Fano factor over a time window between 122–260ms after non-target flash onset was calculated separately for each attention condition (FFin, FFaway), to determine the attentional Fano-factor modulation index (Fig 6):
We examined the degree to which neuronal firing was correlated among pairs of simultaneously recorded units in two different ways:
Spike-count correlations (rSC)
We calculated the Pearson correlation of spike counts across trials for every pair of simultaneously recorded single units where both units in the pair were in the same cortical layer (superficial, input, deep). In order to remove the influence of confounding variables like stimulus strength, spike counts were z-scored using the mean and standard deviation for repetitions of each stimulus type. Ordered pairs of z-scored spike counts were collapsed across contrast conditions and the Pearson correlation was calculated from these ordered pairs. This was done for each attention condition and also for two different counting windows: an inter-stimulus 200ms period before non-target flash onset (Fig 7A) and a stimulus evoked period between 60–260ms after non-target flash onset (Fig 7B). We only considered those pre-stimulus periods where the inter-stimulus interval was greater than 500ms (in other words, the interval between onset of the stimulus and the offset of the previous stimulus was greater than 300ms), so as to minimize artifacts due to stimulus offset.
Spike-spike coherence
We computed the coherence between simultaneously recorded single unit pairs within a cortical layer using multi-taper methods (Mitra and Pesaran, 1999), over the 200ms window between 60–260ms after non-target flash onset. Spike trains were tapered with a single Slepian taper, giving an effective smoothing of 5Hz for the 200ms window (NW=1, K=1). Magnitude of coherence estimates depend on the number of spikes used to create the estimates (Zeitler et al., 2006). To control for differences in firing rate, we adopted a rate matching procedure similar to Mitchell et al., 2009. In order to obtain a baseline for the coherence expected solely due to trends in firing time-locked to stimulus onset, we also computed coherence in which trial identities were randomly shuffled (Fig 7C).
Computational model
We set up a conductance based model of NE excitatory and NI inhibitory neurons with all-to-all connectivity with the following synaptic weights (Fig 8A):
We simulated models of NE = 800 excitatory and NI = 200 inhibitory spiking units. The spiking units were modeled as Izhikevich neurons (Izhikevich, 2003) with the following dynamics:
v is the membrane potential of the neuron and u is a membrane recovery variable. I is the current input to the neuron (synaptic and injected DC currents). The parameters a, b, c and d determine intrinsic firing patterns and were chosen as follows:
Presynaptic spikes from excitatory units generated fast (AMPA) and slow (NMDA) synaptic currents, while presynaptic spikes from inhibitory units generated fast GABA currents:
where VAMPA = 0, VNMDA = 0, VGABA = −70 are the respective reversal potentials (mV). The synaptic time courses g(t) were modeled as a difference of exponentials (Fig S7A):
where τl, τr and τd are the latency, rise and decay time constants with the following parameter values (ms) (Brunel and Wang, 2003):
τl | τr | τd | |
---|---|---|---|
AMPA | 1 | 0.5 | 2 |
NMDA | 1 | 2 | 80 |
GABA | 1 | 0.5 | 5 |
The NMDA to AMPA ratio was chosen as 0.45 (Myme et al., 2003).
The network was stimulated by a DC step current (IDC = 4) of duration 1.5s (Fig 8B). Synaptic noise was simulated by drawing from a normal distribution (Isyn–noise~𝒩(μ = 0, σ = 3)). We calculated spike-count correlations from z-scored spike counts within 200ms counting windows after the initial transient response (results were averaged across 4 such consecutive non-overlapping windows) across 2000 repeats of the stimulation. Spike-count correlations for different choices of network parameters (WEE, WII, WEI, WIE) were reported as the average across all possible pairs of excitatory spiking units (Fig 8C, Fig S7).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NIH R01 EY021827 to J.H.R. and A.S.N., the Gatsby Charitable Foundation to J.H.R., fellowship from the NIH T32 EY020503 training grant and the NARSAD Young Investigator Grant to A.S.N., The Salk Institute Excellerators Fellowship Program and the NARSAD Young Investigator Grant to J.J.N. and by a NEI core grant for vision research P30 EY019005 to the Salk Institute. We would like to thank Ed Callaway, Amanda Casale, Bryan Hansen, Monika Jadi and Jude Mitchell for helpful discussion, Rob Teeuwen for assistance with animal training and Catherine Williams and Mat LaBlanc for excellent animal care. We would like to dedicate this to the memory of John C. Curtis.
Footnotes
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
A.S.N., J.J.N. & J.H.R. designed the experiments. A.S.N. collected and analyzed the data. A.S.N. developed the computational model. A.S.N., J.H.R. and J.J.N. wrote the manuscript.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Anderson JC, Kennedy H, Martin KAC. Pathways of attention: synaptic relationships of frontal eye field to V4, lateral intraparietal cortex, and area 46 in macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10872–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0622-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong KM, Fitzgerald JK, Moore T. Changes in visual receptive fields with microstimulation of frontal cortex. Neuron. 2006;50:791–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong KM, Moore T. Rapid enhancement of visual cortical response discriminability by microstimulation of the frontal eye field. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:9499–504. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701104104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barone P, Batardiere A, Knoblauch K, Kennedy H. Laminar distribution of neurons in extrastriate areas projecting to visual areas V1 and V4 correlates with the hierarchical rank and indicates the operation of a distance rule. J Neurosci. 2000;20:3263–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-09-03263.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett AM, Ovaysikia S, Logothetis NK, Hoffman KL. Saccades during object viewing modulate oscillatory phase in the superior temporal sulcus. J Neurosci. 2011;31:18423–32. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4102-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjornsson CS, Oh SJ, Al-Kofahi YA, Lim YJ, Smith KL, Turner JN, De S, Roysam B, Shain W, Kim SJ. Effects of insertion conditions on tissue strain and vascular damage during neuroprosthetic device insertion. J Neural Eng. 2006;3:196–207. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/3/3/002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollimunta A, Chen Y, Schroeder CE, Ding M. Neuronal mechanisms of cortical alpha oscillations in awake-behaving macaques. J Neurosci. 2008;28:9976–88. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2699-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunel N, Wang XJ. What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation-inhibition balance. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:415–30. doi: 10.1152/jn.01095.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, Desimone R. Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:11262–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011284108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaway EM. Local circuits in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1998;21:47–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauli B, Audinat E, Lambolez B, Angulo MC, Ropert N, Tsuzuki K, Hestrin S, Rossier J. Molecular and physiological diversity of cortical nonpyramidal cells. J Neurosci. 1997;17:3894–906. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03894.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chalk M, Herrero JL, Gieselmann MA, Delicato LS, Gotthardt S, Thiele A. Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1. Neuron. 2010;66:114–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MR, Maunsell JHR. Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1594–600. doi: 10.1038/nn.2439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connors BW, Gutnick MJ. Intrinsic firing patterns of diverse neocortical neurons. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13:99–104. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90185-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas RJ, Martin KAC. Mapping the matrix: the ways of neocortex. Neuron. 2007;56:226–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas RJ, Martin KAC. Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:419–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drewes J, VanRullen R. This is the rhythm of your eyes: the phase of ongoing electroencephalogram oscillations modulates saccadic reaction time. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4698–708. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4795-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattass R, Sousa AP, Gross CG. Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque. J Neurosci. 1988;8:1831–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-01831.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen BJ, Chelaru MI, Dragoi V. Correlated variability in laminar cortical circuits. Neuron. 2012;76:590–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Cortical connectivity and sensory coding. Nature. 2013;503:51–8. doi: 10.1038/nature12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartigan JA, Hartigan PM. The dip test of unimodality. The Annals of Statistics 1985 [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich EM. Simple model of spiking neurons. IEEE Transactions on neural networks. 2003 doi: 10.1109/TNN.2003.820440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y. Physiological subgroups of nonpyramidal cells with specific morphological characteristics in layer II/III of rat frontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1995;15:2638–55. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-02638.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y, Kubota Y. GABAergic cell subtypes and their synaptic connections in rat frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:476–86. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.6.476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knudsen EI. Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:57–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohn A, Smith MA. Stimulus dependence of neuronal correlation in primary visual cortex of the macaque. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3661–73. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5106-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markov NT, Misery P, Falchier A, Lamy C, Vezoli J, Quilodran R, Gariel MA, Giroud P, Ercsey-Ravasz M, Pilaz LJ, Huissoud C, Barone P, Dehay C, Toroczkai Z, Van Essen DC, Kennedy H, Knoblauch K. Weight consistency specifies regularities of macaque cortical networks. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:1254–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams CJ, Maunsell JHR. Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4. J Neurosci. 1999;19:431–41. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00431.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick DA, Connors BW, Lighthall JW, Prince DA. Comparative electrophysiology of pyramidal and sparsely spiny stellate neurons of the neocortex. J Neurophysiol. 1985;54:782–806. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.4.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4. Neuron. 2009;63:879–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron. 2007;55:131–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitra PP, Pesaran B. Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophys J. 1999;76:691–708. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77236-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitzdorf U. Current source-density method and application in cat cerebral cortex: investigation of evoked potentials and EEG phenomena. Physiol Rev. 1985;65:37–100. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1985.65.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T, Armstrong KM. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature. 2003;421:370–3. doi: 10.1038/nature01341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T, Fallah M. Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:1273–76. doi: 10.1073/pnas.021549498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle VB. The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain. 1997;120(Pt 4):701–22. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.4.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myme CIO, Sugino K, Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. The NMDA-to-AMPA ratio at synapses onto layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons is conserved across prefrontal and visual cortices. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:771–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00070.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nassi JJ, Avery MC, Cetin AH, Roe AW, Reynolds JH. Optogenetic Activation of Normalization in Alert Macaque Visual Cortex. Neuron. 2015;86:1504–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowak LG, Azouz R, Sanchez-Vives MV, Gray CM, McCormick DA. Electrophysiological classes of cat primary visual cortical neurons in vivo as revealed by quantitative analyses. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:1541–66. doi: 10.1152/jn.00580.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouget P, Stepniewska I, Crowder EA, Leslie MW, Emeric EE, Nelson MJ, Schall JD. Visual and motor connectivity and the distribution of calcium-binding proteins in macaque frontal eye field: implications for saccade target selection. Front Neuroanat. 2009;3:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.05.002.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Povysheva NV, Gonzalez-Burgos G, Zaitsev AV, Kröner S, Barrionuevo G, Lewis DA, Krimer LS. Properties of excitatory synaptic responses in fast-spiking interneurons and pyramidal cells from monkey and rat prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:541–52. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renart A, de la Rocha J, Bartho P, Hollender L, Parga N, Reyes A, Harris KD. The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science. 2010;327:587–90. doi: 10.1126/science.1179850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L. Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:611–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JH, Pasternak T, Desimone R. Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron. 2000;26:703–14. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81206-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz O, Lustig BR, Nassi JJ, Cetin A, Reynolds JH, Albright TD, Callaway EM, Stoner GR, Roe AW. Optogenetics through windows on the brain in the nonhuman primate. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110:1455–67. doi: 10.1152/jn.00153.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, Lakatos P. Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, Mehta AD, Givre SJ. A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaque. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8:575–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.7.575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Jia X, Zandvakili A, Kohn A. Laminar dependence of neuronal correlations in visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:940–7. doi: 10.1152/jn.00846.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Sommer MA. Spatial and temporal scales of neuronal correlation in visual area V4. J Neurosci. 2013;33:5422–32. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4782-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire RF, Noudoost B, Schafer RJ, Moore T. Prefrontal contributions to visual selective attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2013;36:451–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerleider LG, Galkin TW, Desimone R, Gattass R. Cortical connections of area V4 in the macaque. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:477–99. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis MA, Poort J, van der Togt C, Roelfsema PR. Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:14332–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402773111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vigneswaran G, Kraskov A, Lemon RN. Large identified pyramidal cells in macaque motor and premotor cortex exhibit “thin spikes”: implications for cell type classification. J Neurosci. 2011;31:14235–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3142-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:1195–268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature. 2006;439:733–6. doi: 10.1038/nature04258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeitler M, Fries P, Gielen S. Assessing neuronal coherence with single-unit, multi-unit, and local field potentials. Neural computation. 2006;18:2256–81. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.9.2256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.