Abstract
During the past decade, the disrupted connectivity theory has generated considerable interest as a pathophysiological model for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). This theory postulates that deficiencies in the way the brain coordinates and synchronizes activity amongst different regions may account for the clinical symptoms of ASD. This review critically examines the current structural and functional connectivity data in ASD and evaluates unresolved assumptions and gaps in knowledge that limit the interpretation of these data. Collectively, studies very often show group alterations in what are thought of as measures of cerebral connectivity, though the patterns of findings vary considerably. We argue that there are three principle needs in this research agenda. First, further basic research is needed to understand the links between measures commonly used (DTI, fMRI, EEG) and other (histological, computational) levels of analysis. Second, speculated causes of inconsistencies in the literature (age, clinical heterogeneity) demand studies that directly evaluate these interpretations. Finally, the field needs well-specified mechanistic models of altered cerebral communication in ASD whose predictions can be tested on multiple levels of analyses.
Keywords: autism, disrupted connectivity, functional, structural, review, future research
INTRODUCTION
The disrupted cerebral connectivity hypothesis is currently a leading pathophysiological model for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The most common version of this hypothesis proposes that individuals with ASD have weak connections between distant brain regions and increased connections within local regions; these abnormalities contribute to the social, cognitive and behavioral phenotype (1). Indeed, there are many data consistent with this view. Close examination of this body of work, however, reveals conflicting findings, untested assumptions and gaps in knowledge that leave the validity of this hypothesis open to question.
ASD is a chronic neurodevelopmental disorder affecting approximately 2.25% of children (2,3). Recognition of the high prevalence has accelerated research to identify causes, mechanisms and treatments. Thus far, a primary and unitary etiology for ASD, however, remains elusive.
This review critically evaluates the connectivity literature in ASD. We assert that several confounds of this work temper firm conclusions supporting the disrupted connectivity hypothesis, and we present approaches to transcend current limitations.
1. History of the disrupted connectivity hypothesis
The disrupted connectivity hypothesis had its origins in 1988 when Horwitz et al (4) found reduced correlation in PET signals among frontal, parietal, subcortical regions in adults with ASD. This initial finding was pursued almost 15 years later when Just et al found that individuals with ASD exhibit reduced frontal-posterior fMRI connectivity when performing a sentence comprehension task (5). The hypothesis that children with ASD have local overconnectivity originated separately, with the discovery that young children with ASD have increased head circumference (6–8). Subsequent investigations found that the increased white matter volume was specifically located in superficial (“radiate”) white matter, the fibers connecting neighboring regions, which suggests local overconnectivity (9). Also suggestive of local overconnectivity was the finding of tightly packed minicolumns (which alternatively may result from local inhibitory underconnectivity) (10, 11). This reduction may contribute to the higher risk for seizures and “sub-clinical” epileptiform EEG discharges (12). Belmonte et al (1) integrated accounts of global under- and local over-connectivity and posited that, together, they may together “reinforce each other by failing to differentiate signal from noise.”
Currently, there is modest evidence in support of this hypothesis. Many task-based fMRI studies show that individuals with ASD exhibit reduced functional connectivity between multiple cortical regions or between cortical and subcortical regions (13–15). Further, data from resting state studies indicate reduced connectivity within the default mode network (16–19) and other relevant brain networks (20, 21). There are, however, substantial data challenging the finding of long range underconnectivity in ASD. For example, several studies examining intrinsic connectivity reveal long range overconnectivity or mixed patterns of long range under- and overconnectivity (22–25). The resting state results also show long range overconnectivity or mixed patterns of connectivity (26–28). Similarly, when considering the hypothesis of local overconnectivity in ASD, fMRI data indicate a pattern of widespread local overconnectivity (28), but also a mixed picture of local over- and underconnectivity (29–32). A review of the DTI literature suggests that many studies demonstrate reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) in individuals with ASD although a handful of studies indicate an increase in FA or a combination or reduced and increased FA (33–39). These conflicting findings were usually present in younger children with ASD.
Collectively, the existing connectivity data in ASD are inconsistent. One possible explanation for the variability is that, while altered connectivity is in fact a pathogenic mechanism, we have insufficient specificity in our hypotheses, insufficient precision in our techniques, excessive sensitivity to confounds and/or insufficient power in our studies to correctly identify the supporting evidence. The other possibility is that alterations in connectivity are not relevant to ASD, and positive findings in so-called connectivity measures result from bias or variance for many of the same reasons.
2. What do we mean by “brain connectivity”?
Our notion of connectivity may be defined, somewhat circularly, by whatever is measured by so-called “connectivity metrics,” including anatomical (DTI) and physiological functional/effective (fMRI/MEG/EEG/TMS) connectivity analyses. Better yet, our conceptual notion of connectivity could be based on cellular (e.g., intact vs. broken axons; effective vs. impaired synapses), computational or information transmission characteristics (certain bandwidth and signal-to-noise ratio) (40). Optimally, we should be able to relate micro-scale phenomena to our meso- and macro-scale measures. However, the study of brain connectivity in ASD is inherently limited by gaps in basic knowledge linking the two scales and is influenced by the current state of connectomics (the “wiring diagram of the brain” and the techniques used to map it), and by theoretical models that relate network properties of the brain to behavior (41,42).
EEG and MEG measure electromagnetic activity, but local field potentials rather than action potentials. Each modality records only a very small percentage of total neural activity. Functional MRI measures neuronally-coupled metabolic activity at slow frequencies and is perhaps less directly reflective of neural computation than EEG/MEG (43). Basic science is only beginning to understand what information is coded in each form and how it is captured by EEG. Our connectivity metrics may not necessarily mean what we assume they do. For example, functional connectivity in EEG is typically operationalized as a statistical time-series correlation between the read-out of two macroscopic brain regions. EEG signals are generated by post-synaptic potentials (44), and it is possible that altered dendritic processing, rather than impaired axonal/synaptic transmission could influence so-called connectivity measures. Considerable effort is underway to understand how signals from these modalities can speak to information theoretical parameters, such as bandwidth and signal-to-noise ratio (45).
DTI is a macroscopic anatomical technique that examines the orientation and organization of white matter based on characteristics of water diffusion within an axon (46). Fractional anisotropy (FA), a DTI metric, represents the average direction of water diffusion in a given voxel. Higher FA values may reflect greater organization and orientation of fibers, which is hypothesized to reflect greater connectivity. Multiple factors can affect FA, one being that axons may have different orientations (i.e., crossing fibers), which when averaged can lower the FA value. As such, there is always some ambiguity as to whether low FA represents a true biological finding or a methodological artifact. Some have argued that there is no direct quantitative relationship between FA and connectivity (47). Newer methods, including diffusion spectrum imaging and multi-shell high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI), are currently being used to enhance the resolution and quantification of axonal fibers (48).
As an imperfect proxy to understanding precisely what these macro-level measures mean on a micro-scale, we can assess convergence, in health and disease, among the various techniques. Although substantial evidence is not yet available, preliminary reports demonstrate that some EEG connectivity measurements may correlate with fMRI and with anatomic measurements (49). Divergence of results, as discussed below, is useful when the mechanisms driving the results of different analyses are sufficiently well-understood and when a model is sufficiently nuanced to predict different results from different techniques.
Conversely, measures not typically labeled as “connectivity measures” may, in fact, have the potential to provide strong evidence for altered connectivity. For example, latencies of event-related potentials/fields (ERP/ERFs via EEG/MEG) can shed light on the speed of neural transmission. Roberts et al (50) found that children with ASD exhibit, on average, an 11ms latency increase in cortical auditory-evoked fields, which may speak to slowed conduction in the auditory system. Even behavioral results may have a role. Reaction time and error rate results from multi-sensory integration (MSI) paradigms could be altered as a result of disordered connectivity, as abnormal synchronization/binding of inputs from spatially distant brain regions could contribute to performance deficits (51, 52). Further, TMS is perhaps underutilized as a physiological measure of connectivity and is able to interrogate networks by perturbing the system (53).
LIKELY CONFOUNDS IN THE ASD CONNECTIVITY LITERATURE
1. Systematic artifacts may account for group differences within studies
Multiple types of artifacts could affect conclusions drawn from each connectivity measure. In fMRI, foremost is head motion, which has been shown to result in a pattern of long range underconnectivity and short range overconnectivity, analogous to that seen in ASD (54–56). In DTI, Koldewyn et al (57) demonstrated greater head motion in the ASD group than the typically developing group. Widespread reductions in FA were present in the ASD group when head motion was unaccounted for, however, only one tract exhibited reduced FA when the groups were stringently matched on head motion. Proposed strategies to reduce motion artifacts include specific preprocessing techniques (e.g., scrubbing) and precise group matching on motion measures (58).
In addition to technical factors, systematic differences in participant performance may artifactually affect group results. Group attentional differences can affect the magnitude of activation and therefore measures of connectivity (59). Discarding incorrect trials, using eye tracking methods, and adding a secondary task to assess attention to the primary task may control for group differences in attention (60). These “task-related” concerns are not absent in resting state studies. There is growing awareness that the resting state in fMRI studies is an active cognitive state. ASD and typically developing individuals may differ systematically in response to resting state instructions and environments (61).
Working memory and instruction comprehension may also be compromised in ASD (62). Tasks should be developed so as to minimize taxing of capacities that are known to differ between groups, except when these domains are the focus of study (63). Lastly, group differences in behavioral performance may create results that are more reflective of performance rather than the underlying disorder. Since the tasks used are typically relevant to the diagnosis, some performance differences are inevitable. Administering tasks with varying difficulty levels can facilitate comparison of physiological results while controlling for performance (64).
2. Methodological heterogeneity can lead to inconsistent findings across studies
When the theoretical basis of a series of studies has sufficient nuance to predict that some analytical methods will show positive results and others will show negative results, then “inconsistency” arising from multiple methods is expected and informative. For example, fMRI may register the same BOLD increases for both excitatory and inhibitory increases, whereas EEG and MEG are able to discern the two (65). If, however, choices about technique or processing parameters are irrelevant to a model, then inconsistency in results is reflective of noise. Replicability may be in question. In ASD, significant efforts have been made to disentangle the various processing parameters that may account for discrepant connectivity findings. Muller et al (66) reviewed 32 fMRI studies and found that several data analytic approaches, including pursuing a region of interest (versus whole brain) analysis and not removing task effects or using a low pass filter, increased the likelihood of finding reduced connectivity in the ASD group, whereas regressing task effects and using a low pass filter may eliminate findings of reduced connectivity. A subsequent study found similar results, indicating that task regression and applying a low pass filter were more likely to result in findings of overconnectivity whereas not performing these analyses resulted in underconnectivity (67).
Spatial scale is a specific analysis parameter that is critical to interpretation yet whose definitions vary greatly among studies. As many researchers have pointed out, there are no objective definitions for what constitutes “local” and what constitutes “global” and Studies often fail to define these terms explicitly (68). In general, “long range connectivity” has referred to connections across different lobes (69) or regions of the brain, usually between different cortical regions (e.g., Brodmann areas or other well-delineated regions) but also between cortical and subcortical regions (70). One study explicitly defined long range connectivity as any distance greater than one cubic centimeter irrespective of whether brain regions were spatially separate (68). Definitions of local connectivity, however, are more vague and can refer to spatial scales ranging from microns (e.g., the distance between minicolumns or about 50µm) to millimeters (71). Examples of the latter include approximately 3–5mm distances between neighboring voxels when using regional homogeneity methods (72), 14mm between nodes when using graph theoretical methods (29), and up to 35mm when using tract based analysis (73). Moving forward, it will be critical for investigators either to clearly express spatial scale in absolute terms or specify neuroanatomic structures under examination to facilitate more valid comparisons across studies.
3. Subject heterogeneity may contribute to inconsistent connectivity patterns across studies
Within ASD, there is great variability within both clinical phenotype and etiology. Clinical heterogeneity is present with respect to symptom severity, language ability, cognitive functioning, co-occurring diagnoses, and functional outcome. On the other end of the scale, hundreds of candidate genes/loci have been implicated in ASD, suggesting substantial etiological heterogeneity. Beyond that, connectivity findings could be influenced by the possibility that children with ASD may exhibit greater variability, one to another, than neurotypical children (74). To avoid averaging fallacies and increase specificity in our models, we can and likely should group/stratify by etiological or clinical factors.
Several groups are examining how specific ASD risk genes (e.g., MET variants, CNTNAP2 polymorphisms) affect brain connectivity patterns, particularly in social cognitive networks (75, 76). In some cases, mouse models have been developed for specific etiologies, and research with these models can provide invasive information that complements data obtained in humans. For example, preliminary evidence from mouse models of Fragile X suggests a pattern of increased structural connectivity in the visual cortex with reduced structural connectivity between the visual cortex and other cortical regions; these regions are hypothesized to play a role in sensory processing (77). By examining multiple genetic etiologies, we will likely find mechanisms and biological therapies specific to each genotype. By looking across genotypes that share an ASD phenotype, we may find mechanisms responsible for the cardinal symptoms of ASD.
We can overcome heterogeneity from the other side, by stratifying by clinical factors. This tactic is consistent with the RDoC (Research Domain Criteria) approach, in which behavioral dimensions (e.g., social communication) serve as the independent variables, rather than DSM diagnoses (78). Even within ASD-diagnostic research, however, subject heterogeneity needs to be assessed and controlled for.
Age is a critical demographic factor that may affect results. There is evidence that brain network connectivity evolves over ontogeny. It is therefore plausible that the role of network connectivity alterations in ASD may also evolve over the lifespan. As a consequence, age and pubertal stage should be considered with both theoretical accounts and with data (79–81). To date, much of the data comes from adolescents and adults with ASD who are male, limiting generalizability of conclusions to females and younger children. Additionally, many studies include broad age ranges, sometimes spanning over 10 years, which can obscure important developmental patterns.
On the positive side, some groups have begun to address developmental factors explicitly. For example, Uddin et al recently suggested that younger children with ASD exhibit patterns of overconnectivity whereas adolescents with ASD show underconnectivity (82). Preliminary evidence in support for this hypothesis is emerging (30, 83) and appears consistent with earlier findings suggesting early brain overgrowth in young children with ASD that appears to normalize by adulthood (8, 84).
The data on developmental trajectories have thus far been principally derived from cross-sectional samples. The most conclusive studies addressing connectivity changes during development will be longitudinal. Testing change over time, within subjects, eliminates aspects of inter-individual variability and allows for explicit determination of the contribution of development. One important area of longitudinal work focuses on identifying early markers of ASD in high risk infant siblings. Thus far, several potential biomarkers have emerged, including atypical trajectories of change in spectral power in high risk infants and reduced functional connectivity in high risk infants who go on to develop ASD compared to those who do not (85, 86).
Whereas confounds of heterogeneity refer to disparate connectivity findings within patients taken to be similar, poor discriminant validity refers conversely to similar connectivity findings in groups or diagnoses that may be different. The finding of reduced long range connectivity has been reported in other psychiatric and learning disorders, including ADHD (87), schizophrenia (88), depression (89) and dyslexia (90). The question arises as to why this pathophysiological finding seems prevalent across and how it could explain ASD if it manifests in other, clinically dissimilar disorders. It could be that these different disorders stem from unique etiological mechanisms that converge into common neural signatures, but these signatures represent epiphenomena to the causal stream that connects genetic/cellular mechanisms to behavioral symptoms. Alternatively, each of these disorders could be characterized by its own connectivity signature, which our techniques do not adequately differentiate. Examining connectivity patterns across different disorders might delineate which connectivity findings are unique to ASD and which may result from the impact of comorbid diagnosis.
4. Publication bias and lack of reproducibility can skew interpretations of the data
The scientific community is beginning to understand the importance of publishing negative findings. Sharing negative results is critical for several reasons. If we were to conduct a meta-analysis, we would need to know not only how many studies showed overconnectivity and how many showed underconnectivity, but also how many failed to reject the null hypothesis. Publishing null findings from high quality studies also prevents replication and allows for refining of hypotheses and methods. Well-substantiated negative findings may be more useful to validate an alternative hypothesis than positive studies that are merely “consistent with” the tested hypothesis (91). It should be kept in mind that statistically proving equivalence is a higher bar than merely failing to show a difference (i.e., failure to reject the null hypothesis) (92). As sharing of imaging datasets becomes more common, reanalysis of unpublished connectivity data could also allow for a less biased approach toward capturing negative findings.
Another “endemic problem” in neuroscience research is the low power and lack of reproducibility of studies (93, 94). Underpowered studies could play a significant role in the variability of findings and lack of reproducibility in ASD connectivity research. As a field, we will have to consider what constitutes adequate group sizes, the patterns of funding reproducible research, and the respect we give to replications of previous work.
FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
1. The field needs testable models that make specific predictions across levels of analysis
In mature sciences relating to complex phenomena, theoretical models are generated from reasoning and from preliminary observations, and the specific predictions of those models are tested. Competing models are tested explicitly against one another. In a very simple case, one could propose that for some particular reason, intrahemispheric connectivity should be affected in ASD, whereas interhemispheric connectivity is preserved (13). Using various modalities, one would then predict that measures of intrahemispheric connectivity would show group differences, and equivalent measures of interhemispheric connectivity would not. Another group could propose that, for other reasons, the reverse would be true. A single set of data could help validate one model and reject the other.
Better, models that cross levels of analysis allow for explanation rather than merely description. To date, most studies of altered connectivity in ASD have been descriptive rather than explanatory. Further, many studies have established links between genetic alteration and behavioral phenotypes, although the full causal stream of cellular mechanisms and computational processing in between has not been clearly demonstrated. What is needed in this next phase of research is the development of well-specified models that make predictions on multiple levels of analysis, from genetic mutations to altered cellular processes that affect anatomical and physiological connectivity mechanisms, which affect the nature of information transfer and ultimately behavioral processes. This work is only just beginning. Just et al serves as an example. This group has made clinical observations in the Tower of London test and has then constructed computational models that are sensitive to altered connectivity parameters (specifically bandwidth) (45). This model then makes behavioral and physiological (fMRI) predictions that can be tested with human data. Other groups have produced alternative models. Gepner and Feron (95) posit altered timing and synchronicity of downstream information flows from various sensory inputs. Uhlhaas and Singer (96) have similar notions. To date, these models perhaps occupy too broad a domain to come into direct competition with each other, but future work will necessarily move in this direction.
2. The disrupted connectivity hypothesis needs to be linked to other hypothesized mechanisms of ASD
Many “psychological” hypotheses have been proposed to explain the pathogenesis of ASD, including Theory of Mind (97), extreme male brain (98), executive dysfunction (99), weak central coherence (100), mnesic imbalance (101), altered complex information processing (102), intense world (103), and temporal binding window (104). There are also many other “biological” hypotheses beyond disrupted connectivity (GABAergic dysfunction/altered excitatory:inhibitory balance (11, 105), cerebellar (106, 107), minicolumnopathy (71)). For some of these hypotheses, particularly those in the psychological domain, physiological measures of connectivity can and are used in a task-related fashion, targeting networks known from basic science to be involved in the performance of the relevant task. These studies have targeted psychological domains that are theoretically linked to core ASD domains (i.e., language, social processing) (108). For example, executive function and cognitive control tasks and networks are well-described and have a theoretical relationship to ASD (109). Data are being collected to assess the relationship between executive function, repetitive behaviors and perseveration in ASD on one hand (110), and abnormalities in circuits involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the other (111–113).
In order to understand connectivity abnormalities in the context of other neurobiological frameworks, collaborative or competitive studies of the connectivity hypothesis will be necessary using a broader range of methods. For example, D’Mello and Stoodley (114) have mapped a specific and plausible role of the cerebellum in molding cortical network development, and how alterations of such development could lead to ASD symptoms. A test of this hypothesis might involve genetic along with longitudinal anatomical, physiological and behavioral/clinical testing of high-risk infants over the course of infancy and beyond, specifically relating morphology and physiology of the cerebellum and cerebellar-cerebral tracts to subsequent development of cortical regions and clinical symptoms.
Separately, the altered excitatory:inhibitory hypothesis can be seen as being in competition with the connectivity account, or as a mechanism for it, in the sense of altered synaptic function leading to impaired information transfer (40). In the former case, researchers might examine the relative contribution of inhibitory measures (e.g., EEG/MEG oscillations) vs. functional connectivity measures in explaining clinical features in ASD. In the latter case, it is likely that progress at the current time will be made in computational models, mouse models and cellular work.
SUMMARY
Disrupted connectivity has been a popular pathophysiological hypothesis of ASD, however, conflicting results abound. Future work will need to continue to directly assess experimental and analytical confounds. As the science matures, models that cross levels of analysis will be developed and either validated or rejected in favor of better models. These models will subsequently interact with other hypotheses to ultimately uncover the mechanisms responsible for the clinical features of ASD.
Acknowledgments
Dr. Vasa’s effort was supported by Autism Speaks grant # 8790.
Dr. Ewen’s effort was funded by K23 NS073626.
Dr. Mostofsky’s effort was supported by NIH grants: R01 NS048527-08 and R21 MH098228, and Autism Speaks grants #2506, 2384, 1739.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES: None
REFERENCES
- 1.Belmonte MK, Cook EH, Anderson GM, Greenough WT, Beckel-Mitchener A. Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9(7):646–663. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 6th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Zablotsky B, Black LI, Maenner MJ, Schieve LA, Blumberg SJ. Estimated prevalence of autism and other developmental disabilities following questionnaire changes in the 2014 national health interview survey. Natl Health Stat Report. 2014;13(87):1–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Grady CL, Rapoport SI. The cerebral metabolic landscape in autism: Intercorrelations of regional glucose utilization. Arch Neurol. 1988;45(7):749–755. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520310055018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004;127:1811–1821. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh199. doi: dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lainhart JE, Piven J, Qzorek M, Landa R, Santengelo SL, Coon H, et al. Macrocephaly in children and adults with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(2):282–290. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, Sparks BF, Singh N. Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology. 2002;59(2):175–183. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.2.175. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.59.2.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Courchesne E, Carper R, Akshoomoff N. Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. JAMA. 2003;290(3):337–344. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O’Brien LM, Lange N, Bakardjiev A. Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain. 2003;126:1182–1192. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg110. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Casanova MF, Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, Roy E. Minicolumnar pathology in autism. Neurology 12. 2002;58(3):428–432. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.3.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rubenstein JL, Merzench MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(5):255–267. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183x.2003.00037.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hughes JR, Melyn M. EEG and seizures in autistic children and adolescents: Further findings with therapeutic implications. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2005;36(1):15–20. doi: 10.1177/155005940503600105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kana RK, Keller TA, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: Decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(3):198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Atypical frontal-posterior synchronization of theory of mind regions in autism during mental state attribution. Soc Neurosci. 2009;24(2):135–152. doi: 10.1080/17470910802198510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cherkassky VL, Kana RK, Keller TA, Just MA. Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. Neuroreport. 2006;17(16):1687–1690. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239956.45448.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kennedy DP, Courchesne E. The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. Neuroimage. 2008;39(4):1877–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Weng SJ, Wiggins JL, Peltier SJ, Carrasco M, Risi S, Lord C, et al. Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1313:202–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Assaf M, Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Miller L, Stevens MC, Sahl R, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):247–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Von dem Hagen EA, Stoyanova RS, Baron-Cohen S, Calder AJ. Reduced functional connectivity within and between 'social' resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8(6):694–701. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ebisch SJH, Gallese V, Willems RM, Mantini D, Groen WB, Romani GL, et al. Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula regions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1013–1028. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mizuno A, Villalobos ME, Davies MM, Dahl BC, Muller RA. Partially enhanced thalamocortical functional connectivity in autism. Brain Res. 2006;1104(1):160–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.064. 16828063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Noonan SK, Haist F, Muller RA. Aberrant functional connectivity in autism: Evidence from low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations. Brain Res. 2009;1262:48–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Turner KC, Frost L, Linsenbardt D, McIlroy JR, Muller RA. Atypically diffuse functional connectivity between caudate nuclei and cerebral cortex in autism. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:34–45. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shih P, Shen M, Ottle B, Keehn B, Gaffrey MS, Muller RA. Atypical network connectivity for imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(10):2931–2939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Di Martino A, Kelly C, Grzadzinski R, Zuo XN, Mennes M, Mairena MA, et al. Aberrant striatal functional connectivity in children with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(9):847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nair A, Carper RA, Abbott AE, Chen CP, Solders S, Nakutin S, et al. Regional specificity of aberrant thalamocortical connectivity in autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:4497–4511. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Supekar K, Uddin LQ, Khouzam A, Phillips J, Gaillard WD, Kenworthy LE, et al. Brain hyperconnectivity in children with autism and its links to social deficits. Cell Rep. 2013;5(3):738–747. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Keown CL, Shih P, Nair A, Peterson N, Mulvey ME, Müller R. Local functional overconnectivity in posterior brain regions is associated with symptom severity in autism spectrum disorders. Cell Rep. 2013;5(3):567–572. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dajani DR, Uddin LQ. Local brain connectivity across development in autism spectrum disorder: A cross-sectional investigation. Autism Res. 2015 doi: 10.1002/aur.1494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Shuklaa D, Keehna B, Muller RA. Regional homogeneity of fMRI time series in autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci Lett. 2010;476:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.03.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Paakki JJ, Rahko J, Long X, Moilanen I, Tervonen O, Nikkinen J, et al. Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1321:169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ameis SH, Catani M. Altered white matter connectivity as a neural substrate for social impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Cortex. 2015;62C:158–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rane P, Cochran D, Hodge SM, Haselgrove C, Kennedy DN, Frazier JA. Connectivity in autism: A review of MRI connectivity studies. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2015;23(4):223–244. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bode MK, Mattila ML, Kiviniemi V, Rahko J, Moilanen I, Ebeling H, et al. White matter in autism spectrum disorders-evidence of impaired fiber formation. Acta Radiol. 2011;52:1169–1174. doi: 10.1258/ar.2011.110197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bashat DB, Kronfeld-Duenias V, Zachor DA, Ekstein PM, Hendler T, Tarrasch R, et al. Accelerated maturation of white matter in young children with autism: a high b value DWI study. Neuroimage. 2007;37:40–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Weinstein M, Ben-Sira L, Levy Y, Zachor DA, Ben Itzhak E, Artzi M, et al. Abnormal white matter integrity in young children with autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(4):534–543. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21042. 10.1002/hbm.21042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sivaswamy L, Kumar A, Rajan D, Behen M, Muzik O, et al. A diffusion tensor imaging study of the cerebellar pathways in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Neurol. 2010;25:1223–1231. doi: 10.1177/0883073809358765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cheng Y, Chou KH, Chen IY, Fan YT, Decety J, Lin CP. Atypical development of white matter microstructure in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2010;50:873–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bourgeron T. From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(9):551–563. doi: 10.1038/nrn3992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hoffman RE, McGlashan TH. Parallel distribute processing and the emergence of schizophrenic symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 1993;19:119–140. doi: 10.1093/schbul/19.1.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Elman JL. Connectionist models of cognitive development: where next? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(3):111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: A review. Brain Connect. 2011;1(1):13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Speckmann EJ, Elger CE. Introduction to the neurophysiological basis of the EEG and DC potentials. In: Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL, editors. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. 5th. New York: LWW; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Just MA, Keller TA, Malave, Kana RK, Varma S. Autism as a neural systems disorder: A theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1292–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mori S, Zhang J. Principles of diffusion tensor imaging and its applications to basic neuroscience research. Neuron. 2006;51:527–539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jones DK, Knoschec TR, Turner R. White matter integrity, fiber count and other fallacies: The do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage. 2013;73:239–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.O’Donnell LJ, Westin C. An introduction to diffusion tensor image analysis. Neurosurg Clin N Am. 2011;22(2):185–viii. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2010.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Chu CJ, Leahy J, Pathmanathan J, Kramer MA, Cash SS. The maturation of cortical sleep rhythms and networks over early development. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014;125(7):1360–1370. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Roberts TP, Khan SY, Rey M, Monroe JF, Cannon K, Blaskey L, et al. MEG detection of delayed auditory evoked responses in autism spectrum disorders: Towards an imaging biomarker for autism. Autism Res. 2010;3(1):8–18. doi: 10.1002/aur.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stevenson RA, Sieman JK, Woynaroski TG, Schneider BC, Eberly HE, Camarata SM, et al. Evidence for diminished multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(12):3161–3167. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2179-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ross LA, Del Bene VA, Molholm S, Frey HP, Foxe JJ. Sex differences in multisensory speech processing in both typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:185. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Karabanov AN, Chao CC, Paine R, Hallett M. Mapping different intra-hemispheric parietal-motor networks using twin Coil TMS. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(3):384–389. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):431–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Satterthwaite TD, et al. Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage. 2012;60(1):623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Koldewyn K, Yendiki A, Weigelt S, Gweon H, Julian J, Richardson H, et al. Differences in the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus but no general disruption of white matter tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(5):1981–1986. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1324037111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Deen, Pelphrey Perspective: brain scans need a rethink. Nature. 2012;491(7422):S20. doi: 10.1038/491s20a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Chien YL, Gau SS, Shang CY, Chiu YN, Tsai WC, Wu YY. Visual memory and sustained attention impairment in youths with autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Med. 2015;45(11):2263–2273. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714003201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control and Learning. 5th. Champagne, Il: Human Kinetics; 2011. Attention and performance; pp. 97–132. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Buckner RL. The brain's default network: origins and implications for the study of psychosis. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15(3):351–358. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/rbuckner. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Corbett BA, Constantine LJ, Hendren R, Rocke D, Ozonoff S. Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Res. 2009;166(203):210–222. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Price CJ, Friston KJ. Scanning patients with tasks they can perform. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8(2–3):102–108. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3<102::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ward NS, Brown MM, Thompson AJ, Frackowiak RS. Neural correlates of motor recovery after stroke: A longitudinal fMRI study. Brain. 2003;126(11):2476–2496. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg245. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Khan S, Gramfort A, Shetty NR, Kitzbichler MG, Ganesan S, Moran JM, et al. Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Feb 19;110(8):3107–3112. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214533110. (2013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Muller RA, Shih P, Keehn B, Deyoe JR, Leyden KM, Shukla DK. Underconnected, but how? A survey of functional connectivity MRI studies in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(10):2233–2243. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Nair A, Keown CL, Datko M, Shih P, Keehn B, Meuller RA. Impact of methodological variables on functional connectivity finding in autism spectrum disorders. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:4035–4080. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Vissers ME, Cohen MX, Geurts HM. Brain connectivity and high functioning autism: a promising path of research that needs refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral links. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:604–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Courchesne E, Pierce K. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr Opin Neurobio. 2005;15:225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Maximo JO, Cadena EJ, Kana RK. The implications of brain connectivity in the neuropsychology of autism. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24(1):16–31. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9250-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Buxhoeveden DP, Casanova MF. The minicolumn hypothesis in neuroscience. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 5):935–951. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf110. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf110935-951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Casanova MF, El-Baz AS, Suri JS. Imaging the Brain in Autism. New York: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Shukla DK, Keehn B, Smylie DM, Müller RA. Microstructural abnormalities of short-distance white matter tracts in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(5):1378–1382. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hahamy A, Behrmann M, Malach R. The idiosyncratic brain: Distortion of spontaneous connectivity patterns in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(2):302–309. doi: 10.1038/nn.3919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Dennis EL, Jahanshad N, Rudie JD, et al. Altered structural brain connectivity in healthy carriers of the autism risk gene, CNTNAP2. Brain Connect. 2011;1:447–459. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Rudie JD, Hernandez LM, Brown JA, et al. Autism-associated promoter variant in MET impacts functional and structural brain networks. Neuron. 2012;75:904–915. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Haberl MG, Zerbi V, Veltien A, Ginger M, Heerschap A, Frick A. Structural-functional connectivity deficits of neocortical circuits in the Fmr1 (-/y) mouse model of autism. Sci Adv. 2015;1(10):e1500775. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1500775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Morris SE, Cuthbert BN. Research domain criteria: Cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimension of behavior. Dialogues Clinic Neurosci. 2012;14(1):29–37. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.1/smorris. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Satterthwaite TD, Vandekar S, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, Roalf DR, Jackson C, et al. Sex differences in the effect of puberty on hippocampal morphology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(3):341–350.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Goddings AL, Mills KL, Clasen LS, Giedd JN, Viner RM, Blakemore SJ. The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. Neuroimage. 2014;88:242–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(6):477–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V. Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:458. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Nomi JS, Uddin LQ. Developmental changes in large-scale network connectivity in autism. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;7:732–741. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, Styner M, Chappell C, Smith RG, et al. Early brain overgrowth in autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(5):467–476. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Righi G, Tierney AL, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Functional connectivity in the first year of life in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: An EEG study. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e105176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Tierney AL, Gabard-Durnam L, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: An endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e39127. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Abnormal functional connectivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(5):443–450. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Wang H, Zeng LL, Chen Y, Yin H, Tan Q, Hu D. Evidence of a dissociation pattern in default mode subnetwork functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Sci Rep. 2015;5:14655. doi: 10.1038/srep14655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Pannekoak JN, Van dear Weff SJ, van Tol MJ, Veltman DJ, Aleman A, Zitman FG, et al. Investigating distinct and common abnormalities of resting-state functional connectivity in depression, anxiety, and their comorbid states. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Giraldo-Chica M, Schneider K. Differences in the anatomical connectivity patterns of the lateral geniculate nucleus between subjects with dyslexia and controls. J Vis. 2015;15(12):640. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Mole C, Klein C. Confirmation, refutation, and the evidence of fMRI. In: Hanson SJ, Bunzl M, editors. Foundational Issues in Human Brain Mapping. MA: The MIT Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Walker E, Nowacki AS. Understanding equivalence and noninferiority testing. J Gen Intern Med. 2011;26(2):192–196. doi: 10.1007/s11606-010-1513-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, et al. Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:365–376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Nosek BA, Alter G, Banks GC, Borsboom D, Bowman SD, Breckler SJ, et al. Promoting an open research culture. Scientific Standards. 2015;348(6242):1422–1425. doi: 10.1126/science.aab2374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Gepner B, Feron F. Autism: a world changing too fast for a miswired brain? Neurosci Biobehav R. 2009;33:1227–1242. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: Toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron. 2012;75(6):963–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “Theory of Mind”? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6(6):248–256. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01904-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ozonoff S, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32(7):1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Frith U. Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 101.Romero-Monguia MA. Mnesic imbalance: a cognitive theory about autism spectrum disorders. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2008;7:20. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-7-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;3(4):303–316. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Markram K, Markram H. The intense world theory-a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:224. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Foss-Feig JH, Kwakye LD, Cascio CJ, Burnette CP, Kadivar H, Stone WL, et al. An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Exp Brain Res. 2010;203(2):381–389. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2240-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Coghlan S, Horder J, Inkster B, Mendez MA, Murphy DG, Nutt DJ. GABA system dysfunction in autism and related disorders: From synapse to symptoms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(9):2044–2055. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Rogers TD, Dickson PE, McKimm E, Heck DH, Goldowitz D, Blaha CD, et al. Reorganization of circuits underlying cerebellar modulation of prefrontal cortical dopamine in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder. Cerebellum. 2013;12(4):547–556. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0462-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Courschene E, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press GA, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL. Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. N Engl J Med. 1988;318(21):1349–1354. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198805263182102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Dichter GS. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(3):319–351. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/gdichter. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Ozonoff S, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32(7):1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Boyd BA, McBeww M, Holtzclaw T, Baranek GT, Bodfish JW. (date): Relationships among repetitive behaviors, sensory features and executive functions in high functioning autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 3(4):959–966. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2009.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Ambrosino S, Bos DJ, van Raalten TR, Kobussen NA, van Belle J, Oranje B, Durston S. Functional connectivity during cognitive control in children with autism spectrum disorder: An independent component analysis. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2014;121(9):1145–1155. doi: 10.1007/s00702-014-1237-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Schmitz N, Rubia K, Daly E, Smith A, Williams S, Murphy DG. Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Solomon M, Ozonoff SJ, Ursu S, et al. The neural substrates of cognitive control deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2515–2526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.D'Mello AM, Stoodley CJ. Cerebro-cerebellar circuits in autism spectrum disorder. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:408. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]