Skip to main content
Tremor and Other Hyperkinetic Movements logoLink to Tremor and Other Hyperkinetic Movements
. 2016 Dec 15;6:431. doi: 10.7916/D86H4HRN

A Comparison Study of Cognitive and Neuropsychiatric Features of Essential Tremor and Parkinson’s Disease

Verónica Puertas-Martín 1, Alberto Villarejo-Galende 1,2, Sara Fernández-Guinea 3, Juan Pablo Romero 4, Elan D Louis 5,6,7, Julián Benito-León 1,2,8,*
Editor: Ruth Walker9
PMCID: PMC5233783  PMID: 28105386

Abstract

Background

Essential tremor (ET) and Parkinson’s disease (PD) are two of the most common movement disorders. Leaving aside their motor features, these two conditions share several non-motor features, including cognitive dysfunction and personality changes. However, there are few data comparing the cognitive and personality profiles of ET with PD. Here we compare the cognitive and personality profiles of the two diseases.

Methods

Thirty-two consecutive non-demented ET patients (13 females and 19 males) (67.7±9.8 years), 32 non-demented PD patients (13 females and 19 males) (67.7±9.5 years), and 32 healthy matched controls (14 females and 18 males) (67.9±10.1 years) underwent a neuropsychological test battery, including a global cognitive assessment and tests of attention, executive function, memory, language, and visuospatial function, as well as the Personality Assessment Inventory. Multivariable linear regression analyses were performed, adjusted for age, sex, years of education, medications that potentially affect cognitive function, number of medications, and the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale Total Score.

Results

Neuropsychological scores were similar in PD and ET patients, but patients with disease performed more poorly than control subjects in cognitive tasks such as attention, executive function, memory, and naming.

Discussion

ET and PD exhibited similar deficits in specific aspects of neuropsychological functioning, particularly those thought to rely on the integrity of the prefrontal cortex, and this suggests involvement of frontocerebellar circuits. These findings further challenge the traditional view of ET as a benign and monosymptomatic disorder.

Keywords: Essential tremor, Parkinson’s disease, neuropsychology, neurobehavioral manifestations, cognitive impairment, movement disorders, cerebellum, thalamus

Introduction

Essential tremor (ET) is one of the most common neurological diseases.1,2 Traditionally, it has been considered a benign and monosymptomatic disorder characterized primarily by kinetic tremor in the arms. However, an emerging view that is gaining wider support is that it may actually be a family of diseases unified by the presence of kinetic tremor, while also displaying etiological, pathologic, and clinical heterogeneity.35 The biological mechanisms that underlie ET are not entirely clear, although there is evidence that indicates that it may be a neurodegenerative disease.6

In addition to motor manifestations, ET is also associated with a number of non-motor manifestations, including depressive symptoms,7 changes in sleep patterns,8 and hearing impairment.9 Aside from the above non-motor features, a proportion of ET patients show mild cognitive deficits, mainly in attention and frontal executive function, verbal memory, and visuospatial processes, which might be explained by frontal cortical or frontal cortical–cerebellar pathway dysfunction.1014 Of interest is that these cognitive deficits in ET might not be static and appear to progress at a faster rate than those seen in normal older people.13 Furthermore, patients with ET (especially late-onset ET) appear to have an increased prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia15,16 and have a higher risk of incident dementia.17

Cognitive dysfunction, even in the early stages, is one of the most common non-motor features of another common movement disorder, Parkinson’s disease (PD).18 In this neurodegenerative disease, cognitive dysfunction is thought to be attributed to dysfunction of the basal ganglia circuit (i.e., the striatal-thalamic-cortico loop) triggered by deficits in dopaminergic nigrostriatal neurons.19 As reviewed in detail elsewhere,20 several epidemiological studies have reported an elevated odds or elevated risk of PD in patients with ET. These epidemiological studies, which estimate measures of association, provide significant controlled, quantitative evidence that ET is associated with PD and, more specifically, that baseline ET seems to increase the risk of developing PD by a factor of four to five.2123

Despite the links between these two conditions, there are a limited number of comparison studies of the cognitive profile of ET with PD.2430 Furthermore, these studies used small sample sizes and only two of them utilized a complete neuropsychological examination.24,26 Further, only one study compared the personality features of both diseases.31

In the present study, our aim was to compare the cognitive and personality profiles of individuals with ET and PD, using a healthy control group for additional comparison.

Methods

All procedures were approved by the ethical standards committees on human experimentation at the University Hospital “12 de Octubre” (Madrid). Written (signed) informed consent was obtained from all enrollees.

Participants

ET and PD patients were consecutively recruited from October 2012 to July 2013 from the outpatient neurology clinics of the University Hospital “12 de Octubre” in Madrid, Spain. Two neurologists with expertise in movement disorders (J.P.R. and J.B.-L.) examined the patients and used the Fahn–Tolosa–Marin Tremor Rating Scale to assign a total tremor score tor the ET patients,32 and the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (motor section) for those with PD.33 Diagnoses of ET and PD were assigned by these two neurologists using the Consensus Statement on Tremor by the Movement Disorder Society34 and the UK PD Society Brain Bank Clinical Diagnostic Criteria,35 respectively. Furthermore, all ET patients had a normal 123 I-labelled N-(3-fluoropropyl)-2ß-carbomethoxy-3ß-(4-iodophenyl)nortropane single photon emission computed tomography scan. Patients with a history of stroke, epilepsy, or head injury were excluded. Furthermore, based on a detailed clinical mental status examination, we excluded patients with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV criteria for dementia.36 All ET and PD patients underwent a detailed videotaped neurological examination. Each videotape was reviewed by a senior neurologist specializing in movement disorders (E.D.L.) who re-assessed ET or PD diagnosis using the Consensus Statement on Tremor by the Movement Disorder Society,34 and the UK PD Society Brain Bank Clinical Diagnostic Criteria,35 respectively. The ET and PD patients were also followed at regular intervals (3, 6, or 12 months, based on clinical need) and their clinical assessment, described above, was repeated.

Healthy controls were recruited either from relatives or friends of the health professionals working at the University Hospital “12 de Octubre”, Madrid (Spain), or among the relatives of patients who came to the neurological clinics for reasons other than ET or PD (e.g., headache, dizziness). None reported having a first-degree or second-degree relative with ET. Each control was examined by two neurologists (J.P.R .and J.B.-L.) to further rule out any neurological conditions.

Procedure

During recruitment, patients and controls were told that the purpose of the study was to complete a test battery to assess neuropsychological and personality status. After the study had been described to participants, informed consent to participate was obtained. Clinical characteristics were also obtained from review of records from their outpatient neurological care. All the neuropsychological and personality tests were performed on the same day by the same examiner (V.P.-M.).

All participants underwent a detailed neuropsychological assessment covering the domains of attention, executive function, verbal memory, visual memory, visuospatial ability, and language. These tests have previously been described.37,38 They were selected in part to avoid the effects of any hand tremor because they made minimal demands on motor processes. Individual cognitive measures were grouped into several cognitive domains, as described below.37,38

Global cognitive performance was evaluated with the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (higher scores indicate better cognitive performance).39

Attention and executive function were evaluated with a series of tests. First, participants underwent the Direct and Indirect Digit Span and the Coding-Digit Symbol subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS-III) (higher scores indicate better cognitive performance).40 In the first, the examinee is required to repeat three to nine digits forward (direct) and backward (indirect).40 In the second, the numbers one to seven have to be paired with symbols on a key presented to the examinee.40 Second, the Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III was also administered;40 in this test, which examines concrete, functional, and abstract concept formation, 19 items require the examinee to describe how two given things are alike.40 Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance.40 The Trail-making Test is a measure of visuomotor coordination in which subjects must connect circles in one form (A) on the basis of a simple rule of consecutive numbers and in the second form (B) by alternating between numerical and alphabetical sequences.41 For both forms, A and B, the time for completion is the primary index of performance. The score for this study was Trail B minus Trail A (lower scores indicate better cognitive performance). Third, the Stroop Color–Word Trial requires the participant to name the color of the ink in which a colored word is printed.42 The task involves three test cards, one containing rows of colored rectangles, with the task being to name the colors as quickly as possible, one containing rows of color words (printed in black ink), with the task being to read the words as quickly as possible, and the third “interference” test consisting of rows of color words printed in ink colors incongruent with the word represented, with the task being to name the ink colors as quickly as possible.42 The subject must ignore the word and name the color.42 The score for this study was the interference effect (scores close to zero indicate better cognitive performance). Fourth, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, a test of “set-shifting,” requires the examinee to discern the sort criterion of a set of cards based upon “correct” versus “incorrect” feedback given by the examiner.43 The score for this study was the number of errors and perseverations (higher scores indicate worse performance).43 Fifth, the Tower of London was administered, a well-known test used for the assessment of executive function specifically to detect deficits in planning.44 The test consists of two boards with pegs and several beads with different colors.44 The examiner uses the beads and the boards to present the examinee with problem-solving tasks.44 For this study, we recorded the time required to execute the test.44 Finally, the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB), a brief tool designed to assess frontal lobe function, including conceptualization, mental flexibility, motor programming, sensitivity to interference, inhibitory control, and environmental autonomy, was administered.45

To evaluate visuospatial ability, two tests were used. The first, the Benton Judgment of Line Orientation Test, is a standardized test of visuospatial skills, that measures a person’s ability to match the angle and orientation of lines in space.46 The second, the Hooper Visual Organization Test,47 is an instrument that measures visual organizational skills, and consists of line drawing of simple objects that have been cut into pieces and rearranged, such as in a puzzle. The examinee’s task is to name what the object would be if the pieces were put back together.47 In both tests, higher scores indicate better cognitive function.46,47

To evaluate verbal memory, we used the Wechsler Memory Scale—Third Edition (WMS-III) Word List,48 which included four learning trials of 12 unrelated words. World List 1 is derived from the sum of the four trials.48 A second list is then presented once for immediate recall, following which the examinee is asked to again recall the first list.48 Free recall and recognition (yes–no format) of the initial words are later assessed after a delay interval.48 Higher scores indicate better cognitive function.48

To evaluate visual memory, we used the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised.49 In three learning trials, the examinee views the stimulus page and is asked to draw as many of the figures as possible.49 A delayed recall trial is administered after a 25-minute delay.49 Last, there is a recognition trial, in which the examinee is asked to identify which of 12 figures were included among the original ones.49 Higher scores indicate better cognitive function.49

Language was evaluated using the following tests. First, the Boston Naming Test,50 which assesses the ability to name pictures of objects through spontaneous responses and the need for various types of cueing (lower scores indicate greater cognitive impairment). Second, participants were asked to name as many items as possible from a semantic category (animals) (semantic fluency) (lower scores indicate greater cognitive impairment).51 Finally, the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, a test that measures phonetic fluency, was administered.52 Participants are provided with three letters of the alphabet (F, A, and S), one letter at a time, and instructed to say as many words as possible that begin with this letter in a 60-second interval.52 Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance.52

Depression was assessed with the 17-item version of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale.53 Higher scores reflect more depressive symptoms.53 The 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale also includes six items that assess anxiety features: psychic anxiety (Item 10), somatic anxiety (Item 11), gastrointestinal somatic symptoms (Item 12), general somatic symptoms (Item 13), hypochondriasis (Item 15), and insight (Item 17).53

Psychopathology and personality symptoms were assessed using the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI), a widely used multidimensional 344-item self-report measure.54 The PAI consists of 22 non-overlapping scales: four validity scales, 11 clinical scales, five treatment consideration scales, and two interpersonal scales. For the present study, only clinical scales (somatic concerns, anxiety, anxiety-related disorders, depression, mania, paranoia, schizophrenia, borderline features, antisocial features, alcohol problems, and drug problems) were used, and higher scores reflect greater psychopathology.

All patients were using medications for their disease. Specifically, propranolol and/or primidone for ET, and levodopa, rasagiline and/or dopamine agonists for PD. The neuropsychological examination was performed while taking their regular treatment.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed in SPSS Version 21.0 (IBM Corp., NY, USA). All tests were two sided, and significance was accepted at the 5% level (alpha = 0.05). Comparison of means of groups was made by an analyses of variance (ANOVA) test for normally distributed data and by a Kruskal–Wallis test for non-normally distributed data, where appropriate. The chi-square test was used to analyze differences in categorical variables.

To assess differences between ET and PD patients, ET and control subjects, and PD and control subjects in neuropsychological and personality scores while adjusting for age, sex, years of education, medications that potentially affect cognition function (i.e., anxiolytics, stimulants, antipsychotics, antidepressants, antihistamines, antihypertensives, or antiepileptics drugs), total number of medications, and 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale Total Score, linear regression analyses were performed in which the outcome variables were each one of the neuropsychological and PAI scores.

All test scores were normally distributed (Kolmorov–Smirnov test, for all items, p<0.05), except for the MMSE, FAB, Direct and Indirect Digit Span Tests, delayed recognition of the WMS-III Word List, delayed recall and delayed recognition of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised, and perseverations of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. For these latter tests that were not normally distributed, a logarithmic transformation was performed prior to linear regression analyses.

Results

Ninety-six participants were evaluated, with 32 in each of the three groups. Clinical details of the patients and healthy controls are provided in Table 1. The 32 ET patients (13 females and 19 males) were compared with 32 PD patients (13 females and 19 males) and 32 healthy controls (14 females and 18 males). The three groups did not differ to a significant degree in terms of age, sex, years of education or intake of drugs with effect on cognition (Table 1). However, there were differences in disease duration (in years), as ET patients had had their disease for more time (13.7 years more) than PD patients. Further, both ET and PD patients were taking more medications than the control group. Our PD sample comprised mild cases: 100% patients had a Hoehn–Yahr stage of I or II.

Table 1. Comparison of Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Essential Tremor and Parkinson’s Disease Patients vs. Healthy Controls.

Essential Tremor Patients (N = 32) Parkinson’s Disease Patients (N = 32) Healthy Controls (N = 32) p
Sex (women) 13 (40.6%) 13 (40.6%) 14 (43.7%) 0.959
Age in years 67.7 (69.0)±9.8 (range 40–80) 67.7 (68.5)±9.5 (range 44–80) 67.9 (70.0)±10.1 (range 41–83) 0.994
Years of education 7.2 (8.0)±3.5 (range 1–15) 7.6 (6.5)±4.4 (range 2–19) 8.9 (8.5)±3.7 (range 2–15) 0.189
Number of medications 4.7 (4.5)±3.5 (range 0–14) 5.6 (5.0)±3.0 (range 2–14) 2.4 (1.0)±2.6 (range 0–9) <0.001
Taking a medication that potentially affects cognition function 10 (31.2%) 11 (34.4%) 6 (18.7%) 0.339
Disease severity1 32.2 (31.0)±14.1 (range 6–60) 14.9 (15.0)±6.8 (range 5–31)
Disease duration in years 20.1 (17.5)±14.8 (range 4–66) 6.4 (5.5)±3.3 (range 1–14) <0.001

Values are expressed as mean (median)±standard deviation, and range. Analysis of variance test or the Student t test were used for comparison of continuous data where appropriate, and the chi-square test for proportions.

1

Fahn–Tolosa–Marìn Tremor Rating Scale for essential tremor and the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (motor section) for Parkinson’s disease.

The raw mean scores on the different neuropsychological test are detailed in Table 2. Significant differences between ET and PD were not found in any test. In some tests, scores of ET patients were slightly better than scores of PD patients, especially in the Trail-making Test (Trail B minus Trail A), the Boston Naming Test, the Judgment of Line Orientation Test, and the Hooper Visual Organization Test. PD patients performed marginally better than the ET group in Similarities, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (perseverations), and phonetic fluency. In other tests, the scores were similar in both groups.

Table 2. Comparison of Cognitive and Neuropsychiatric Domains of Patients vs. Healthy Controls.

Essential tremor patients (N = 32) Parkinson’s disease patients (N = 32) Healthy controls (N = 32) p Bonferroni test
Global cognitive performance
Mini-Mental State Examination 33.0 (33.0)±1.8 32.4 (32.0)±2.0 34.2 (35.0)±1.2 <0.0011 ET<HC; PD<HC
Executive function and Attention
Direct Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III 5.2 (5.0)±1.2 5.2 (5.0)±1.2 5.7 (6.0)±1.2 0.2931 Not significant
Indirect Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III 3.6 (3.5)±1.1 3.6 (4.0)±1.2 4.4 (4.5)±1.0 0.0041 ET<HC; PD<HC
Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III 29.2 (25.5)±14.9 30.2 (27.5)±17.2 46.8 (46.0)±16.2 <0.001 ET<HC; PD<HC
Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III 12.4 (12.0)±5.7 13.7 (11.5)±7.0 17.7 (18.0)±5.4 0.002 ET<HC; PD<HC
Trail-making Test, B – A 147.9 (137.0)±101.6 178.3 (144.5)±152.4 78.1 (60.5)±71.8 0.004 EP<HC
Stroop Color–Word Trial (interference effects) -3.3 (-3.4)±6.8 -4.3 (4.2)±8.3 -1.7 (-2.5)±7.5 0.401 Not significant
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (errors) 60.6 (63.0)±26.2 60.8 (65.0)±22.3 59.1 (62.5)±24.6 0.954 Not significant
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (perseverations) 47.4 (31.0)±39.4 40.4 (35.0)±28.4 37.2 (32.0)±26.5 0.9021 Not significant
Tower of London (time of execution in seconds) 454.5 (415.0)±223.6 477.7 (456.0)±268.4 357.0 (317.0)±151.0 0.072 Not significant
Frontal Assessment Battery 15.4 (16.0)±2.0 15.3 (16.0)±2.1 17.2 (17.0)±0.7 <0.0011 ET<HC; PD<HC
Visuospatial ability
Benton Judgment of Line Orientation Test 9.6 (10.5)±3.1 8.3 (8.0)±3.6 10.1 (10.0)±2.7 0.072 Not significant
Hooper Visual Organization Test 33.0 (33.5)±8.4 28.4 (28.5)±11.8 36.5 (36.0)±9.0 0.007 PD<HC
Verbal memory
WMS-III Word List
 Learning 25.3 (26.0)±5.9 24.5 (24.0)±7.3 28.0 (27.5)±5.5 0.071 Not significant
 Immediate recall 5.1 (5.0)±2.3 5.6 (5.0)±2.3 6.5 (6.0)±1.9 0.047 ET<HC
 Delayed recall 4.6 (4.5)±2.1 4.9 (4.0)±2.2 6.1 (6.0)±2.3 0.020 ET<HC
 Delayed recognition 20.6 (21.0)±2.0 21.2 (21.5)±2.2 22.1 (22.0)±1.4 0.0081 ET<HC
Visual memory
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised
 Learning trials 19.5 (18.0)±8.7 20.2 (19.0)±7.9 27.9 (27.5)±5.3 <0.001 ET<HC; PD<HC
 Delayed recall trial 7.3 (8.0)±3.4 7.4 (7.0)±3.4 10.3 (10.5)±1.6 <0.0011 ET<HC; PD<HC
 Recognition trial 11.4 (12.0)±0.9 11.2 (12.0)±1.1 11.8 (12.0)±0.5 0.0251 PD<HC
Language
Boston Naming Test 44.6 (45.5)±10.0 43.1 (46.0)±11.7 52.1 (53.5) 5.4 <0.001 ET<HC; PD<HC
Total number of animals as possible in one minute 17.7 (15.0)±8.0 18.5 (17.0)±7.1 21.2 (21.0)±6.0 0.128 Not significant
Controlled Oral Word Association Test 23.6 (19.5)±13.1 26.7 (23.0)±17.2 37.6 (40.5)±12.8 <0.001 ET<HC; PD<HC
Depressive symptoms
17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale total score 6.4 (7.0)±4.5 5.5 (6.0)±4.16 5.0 (4.0)±5.0 0.736 Not significant
Personality and Psychopathology
Personality Assessment Inventory
 Somatic concerns 13.0 (12.0)±7.3 12.0 (11.0)±6.3 7.6 (6.0)±5.1 0.003 ET>HC; PD>HC
 Anxiety 11.0 (10.0)±6.9 8.2 (7.0)±4.9 6.0 (5.0)±5.3 0.005 ET>HC
 Anxiety related disorders 14.1 (13.0)±6.3 10.5 (10.0)±5.4 10.4 (10.0)±5.7 0.025 Not significant
 Depression 10.6 (8.0)±6.8 9.2 (8.0)±5.0 5.6 (5.0)±4.5 0.003 ET>HC; PD>HC
 Mania 8.3 (8.0)±5.5 6.2 (5.0)±4.7 6.5 (6.0)±4.2 0.207 Not significant
 Paranoia 10.5 (9.0)±4.4 8.7 (8.0)±4.6 8.9 (9.0)±4.3 0.218 Not significant
 Schizophrenia 7.5 (7.0)±5.9 4.6 (4.0)±3.9 4.9 (4.0)±3.5 0.029 Not significant
 Borderline features 8.6 (7.0)±5.4 5.3 (4.0)±4.0 6.3 (7.0)±3.9 0.018 ET>PD
 Antisocial features 3.2 (3.0)±2.7 2.7 (2.0)±3.0 1.9 (2.0)±1.6 0.150 Not significant
 Alcohol problems 0.6 (0.0)±1.4 0.2 (0.0)±0.7 0.2 (0.0)±0.6 0.140 Not significant
 Drug problems 0.2 (0.0)±0.6 0.1 (0.0)±0.5 0.4 (0.0)±1.0 0.199 Not significant

Abbreviations: ET, Essential Tremor; HC, Healthy Controls; PD, Parkinson’s Disease; WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition; WMS-III, Wechsler Memory Scale—Third Edition.

Significant values are in bold font.

Mean (median)±standard deviation is reported.

Analysis of variance test or 1Kruskal–Wallis U test.

The performance of the ET group was worse than the control group for most neuropsychological tests. These differences were significant (ANOVA) for several tests: the MMSE, Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III, Indirect Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III, verbal memory (immediate recall, delayed recall, and delayed recognition), visual memory (learning trials and delayed recall trial), verbal fluency, Boston Naming Test, FAB, and Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III. In addition, the performance of the PD group was worse than the control group for most neuropsychological tests. These differences were significant (ANOVA) for several tests: MMSE, Trail-making Test (Trail B minus Trail A), Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III, Indirect Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III, FAB, Hooper Visual Organization Test, visual memory (learning trials, delayed recall trial, and recognition trial), verbal fluency, Boston Naming test, and Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III.

In the linear regression analyses that were adjusted for age in years, sex, years of education, medications that potentially affect cognition, number of medications, and 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale total score, the results were similar to that of the ANOVA (Table 3). The ET group did not differ from the PD group, and scored slightly worse than the healthy control group in the FAB, Trail-making Test (Trail B minus A), Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III, verbal memory (immediate and delayed recall, and delayed recognition), visual memory (learning trials and delayed recall trial), verbal fluency, Boston Naming Test, and Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III. The PD group performed more poorly than healthy controls in the MMSE, FAB, Trail-making Test (Trail B minus Trail A), Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III, Hooper Visual Organization Test, visual memory (learning trials, delayed recall trial, and recognition trial), Boston Naming Test, and Tower of London (time of execution).

Table 3. Linear Regression Analyses Using Each Neuropsychological Test Score and the Personality Assessment Inventory as the Outcome Variable in Separate Adjusted Models1.

Essential Tremor vs. Healthy Controls Essential Tremor vs. Parkinson’s Disease Parkinson’s disease vs. healthy controls
β p β p β p
Global cognitive performance
Mini-Mental State Examination 0.221 0.097 –0.119 0.281 0.367 0.004
Executive function and attention
Direct Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III 0.012 0.929 –0.120 0.251 0.063 0.638
Indirect Digit Span subtest from the WAIS-III 0.216 0.086 –0.058 0.618 0.179 0.164
Coding-Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III 0.370 <0.001 0.018 0.831 0.343 <0.001
Similarities subtest from the WAIS-III 0.285 0.013 0.029 0.734 0.208 0.054
Trail-making Test, B – A –0.323 0.017 0.171 0.195 –0.417 0.002
Stroop Color–Word Trial (interference effects) 0.118 0.422 –0.108 0.390 0.258 0.068
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (errors) 0.108 0.389 0.050 0.690 0.049 0.697
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (perseverations) 0.103 0.440 0.055 0.683 –0.018 0.891
Tower of London (time of execution in seconds) –0.180 0.137 0.079 0.519 –0.266 0.043
Frontal Assessment Battery 0.318 0.011 –0.033 0.789 0.391 0.003
Visuospatial ability
Benton Judgment of Line Orientation Test 0.092 0.453 –0.216 0.063 0.187 0.176
Hooper Visual Organization Test 0.183 0.157 –0.193 0.114 0.366 0.002
Verbal memory
WMS-III Word List
 Learning 0.170 0.177 –0.079 0.472 0.176 0.154
 Immediate recall 0.265 0.044 0.135 0.247 0.038 0.783
 Delayed recall 0.279 0.026 0.065 0.576 0.174 0.187
 Delayed recognition 0.312 0.013 0.150 0.293 0.221 0.128
Visual memory
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised
 Learning trials 0.388 0.002 0.002 0.983 0.468 <0.001
 Delayed recall trial 0.380 0.003 0.002 0.989 0.430 <0.001
 Recognition trial 0.186 0.198 –0.208 0.123 0.318 0.018
Language
Boston Naming Test 0.297 0.013 –0.132 0.222 0.376 0.003
Category-cued Word Fluency 0.079 0.534 0.029 0.780 0.116 0.336
Controlled Oral Word Association Test 0.270 0.019 0.070 0.479 0.166 0.174
Personality and Psychopathology
Personality Assessment Inventory
 Somatic concerns –0.223 0.093 –0.154 0.200 –0.051 0.694
 Anxiety –0.230 0.033 –0.220 0.064 –0.022 0.874
 Anxiety related disorders –0.123 0.311 –0.274 0.028 0.209 0.154
 Depression –0.309 0.003 –0.134 0.186 –0.057 0.604
 Mania –0.069 0.636 –0.194 0.153 0.041 0.791
 Paranoia –0.114 0.413 –0.196 0.147 0.174 0.248
 Schizophrenia –0.068 0.591 –0.256 0.026 0.296 0.039
 Borderline features –0.073 0.559 –0.341 0.008 0.284 0.049
 Antisocial features –0.230 0.081 –0.086 0.516 –0.116 0.405
 Alcohol problems –0.120 0.421 –0.093 0.495 0.036 0.828
 Drug problems 0.199 0.179 –0.082 0.578 0.299 0.044

Significant values are in bold font.

1

Adjusted for age, sex, years of education, medications that potentially affect cognitive function, total number of medications, and 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale total score.

In the PAI, we observed differences in the ANOVA tests between the ET and the control group in somatic concerns, anxiety, and depression, where the ET group had higher scores (i.e., greater psychopathology) (Table 2). The last two domains remained different in regression models (Table 3). There were also differences between PD patients and the control group in somatic concerns and depression, but these differences were not observed in the regression analyses, except for schizophrenia, borderline features, and drug problems. When we compared the two diseases, there were differences in borderline features, both in the ANOVA and the regression analyses. In addition, there were differences between both diseases in anxiety-related disorders and schizophrenia in the regression analyses.

We recognize that we entered many variables into the regression models; however, our rationale was that many of these are classic variables that are generally used in research that evaluates cognition.55,56 In a sensitivity analysis, we removed several of the variables (age, sex, and years of education) and the results were similar (data not shown).

Discussion

In this study we characterized the cognitive performance of three different groups: 32 ET patients, 32 PD patients, and 32 healthy controls. Our goal was to compare the cognitive profile of PD and ET using a healthy control group as a reference point. The importance of this effort relies on the fact that that the three groups had similar age, sex, and education. Moreover, previous studies on this topic have been conducted in small samples, allowing us to benchmark these results with our larger sample of individuals.

We observed that both the ET and the PD groups performed worse than the control group. These results are in agreement with other studies.2426,29 ET and PD had similar deficits in specific aspects of neuropsychological functioning, particularly those thought to rely on the integrity of the prefrontal cortex, which suggests involvement of frontocerebellar circuits,57 characterized by worse performance in functions such as attention, executive function, memory, and naming. Other studies have also noted these similarities between ET and PD.26,28 Lombardi et al.24 studied 18 ET and 18 PD patients without dementia, and compared the results with normative data. The ET group showed a poorer performance only in verbal fluency tests and digit span, whereas the PD patients, in addition, had a significantly lower performance in visuospatial, memory, and attentional tasks.24 The authors suggested a frontosubcortical impairment for these findings.24 Gasparini et al.25 reported data from a sample of 27 ET patients (15 familial cases and 12 cases with a family history of PD), 15 PD patients, and 15 healthy controls, all of them without dementia. The ET patients showed significant impairments both in attentional and conceptual thinking tasks, similar to those observed in the PD group.25 The authors suggested the presence of frontal lobe dysfunction in ET.25 Higginson et al.26 studied 24 ET patients, 24 PD patients, and 21 healthy controls. The results indicated that the ET group performed significantly worse than controls across multiple cognitive domains, but performed remarkably similar to PD patients, consistent with frontosubcortical dysfunction. A more recent study by Benge et al.30 validated an executive dysfunction scale in a sample of deep brain stimulation candidates, including 15 PD patients and 11 ET patients. The PD group had a poorer performance than the ET in that scale and in the memory tests.30

In our study, PD patients performed more poorly than ET patients and the controls in tests measuring global cognition and frontal activities (i.e., FAB) and attentional, visuospatial, and denomination tasks. On the other hand, the ET group scored marginally worse than the PD group in memory, verbal fluency, and abstraction capacity. When we compared the ET patients with the control group, they performed less well in the same tasks as the PD group. However, in multivariate analyses adjusted for confounding effects of age, sex, years of education, number of medications, intake of drugs that may affect cognition, and 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale Total Score, the results were similar between ET and PD patients. An unexpected result, in multivariate analyses, was that schizophrenia scores were higher in ET than PD. We do not have a biological explanation for this result. As this was one of many differences, and it was not reproducible in the ANOVA models after a Bonferroni correction (see Table 2), it could be a spurious association. This is furthermore supported by the observation that no prior studies have reported an association between ET and schizophrenia/psychosis and that there is no compelling biological/mechanistic basis to suspect a higher incidence of psychotic disorders in ET patients.

Our findings suggest a similar cognitive profile for PD and ET groups in the absence of dementia and, interestingly, an overlap in the affected domains. In our opinion, these results highlight the existing view that the PD and ET clinical picture exceeds motor features, even at an early stage, where cognitive effects can be observed.14,18 Our PD sample included mostly mild cases (100% had a Hoehn–Yahr stage of I or II), reducing the possibility of cortical involvement, and the 5-year mean disease duration also minimized the chances of a misdiagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies.

In PD, the cognitive features have been attributed to the dysfunction of the basal ganglia circuit (i.e., the striatal-thalamic-cortico loop).19 Likewise, there is strong evidence that suggests a dysfunction of the cerebello-thalamo-cortical circuit in ET.37,58 The thalamus is thought to be highly implicated in modulation of cognitive performance, representing a fundamental subcortical relay to the prefrontal cortex.59 The connections with the frontal lobes could be impaired in both diseases and therefore explain the similar cognitive profile.18,19

The study was not without limitations. First, ET patients had a longer disease duration than PD patients. This suggests that cognitive impairment in PD might start before that in ET and that the cognitive decline could be slower in ET. Second, the sample size was relatively small. The literature, however, only includes studies with smaller sample sizes. Further, despite the small sample size, our sample was adequate to detect a number of robust differences between the patients and the healthy control group. Third, the patients in the current study may represent a selected group of ET or PD patients (i.e., patients seen in selected outpatient clinics), and hence our results may not necessarily be generalized to the entire ET or PD population. However, in Spain, healthcare is fully state-subsidized, and community-dwelling ET or PD patients are mostly seen by hospital-based and hospital-associated neurologists. This study also had several strengths. First, this is the first study that has assessed the cognitive and personality profile at the same time in ET and PD patients. Second, assessments were conducted prospectively in a standardized manner. Finally, the tests included are reported to be among the most sensitive neuropsychological measures to detect cognitive impairment in tremor disorders.

In conclusion, our results are important for the definition and characterization of the non-motor cognitive aspects of ET and PD. So far, this study represents one of the largest samples where both conditions were compared, hence being closer to the real cognitive performance of both populations. The possibility to adjust for known confounding covariates has also helped us to interpret these results. Interestingly, we confirmed that both entities exhibited poorer cognitive performance compared with healthy subjects, thus further challenging the old mono-symptomatic motor view of ET.

Footnotes

Funding: Dr. Benito-León is supported by the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA (NINDS #R01 NS39422), the Commission of the European Union (grant ICT-2011-287739, NeuroTREMOR), and the Spanish Health Research Agency (grant FIS PI12/01602). Dr. Louis has received research support from the National Institutes of Health: NINDS #R01 NS039422 (principal investigator), NINDS #R01 NS094607 (principal investigator), NINDS #R01 NS073872 (principal investigator), NINDS #R01 NS085136 (principal investigator), NINDS #R01 NS046346 (principal investigator), and NINDS #R01 NS088257 (principal investigator). Dr. Romero is supported by the Commission of the European Union (grant ICT-2011-287739, NeuroTREMOR).

Financial Disclosures: None.

Conflicts of Interest: The authors report no conflict of interest.

Ethics Statement: This study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards detailed in the Declaration of Helsinki. The authors’ institutional ethics committee has approved this study and all patients have provided written informed consent.

References

  • 1.Benito-León J. How common is essential tremor? Neuroepidemiology. 2009;32:215–216. doi: 10.1159/000195692. doi: 10.1159/000195692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Louis ED, Ferreira JJ. How common is the most common adult movement disorder? Update on the worldwide prevalence of essential tremor. Mov Disord. 2010;25:534–541. doi: 10.1002/mds.22838. doi: 10.1002/mds.22838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Benito-León J, Louis ED. Essential tremor: emerging views of a common disorder. Nat Clin Practice Neurol. 2006;2:666–78. doi: 10.1038/ncpneuro0347. quiz 2 p following 91. doi: 10.1038/ncpneuro0347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Benito-León J, Louis ED. Clinical update: diagnosis and treatment of essential tremor. Lancet. 2007;369:1152–1154. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60544-3. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60544-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Benito-León J, Louis ED. Update on essential tremor. Minerva Med. 2011;102:417–440. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Benito-Leon J. Essential tremor: one of the most common neurodegenerative diseases? Neuroepidemiology. 2011;36:77–78. doi: 10.1159/000323572. doi: 10.1159/000323572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Louis ED, Benito-León J, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Self-reported depression and anti-depressant medication use in essential tremor: cross-sectional and prospective analyses in a population-based study. Eur J Neurol. 2007;14:1138–1146. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2007.01923.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2007.01923.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Short sleep duration heralds essential tremor: a prospective, population-based study. Mov Disord. 2013;28:1700–177. doi: 10.1002/mds.25590. doi: 10.1002/mds.25590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Reported hearing impairment in essential tremor: a population-based case-control study. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29:213–217. doi: 10.1159/000112463. doi: 10.1159/000112463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Troster AI, Fields JA, Pahwa R, et al. Neuropsychological and quality of life outcome after thalamic stimulation for essential tremor. Neurology. 1999;53:1774–1780. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.8.1774. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.8.1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Population-based case-control study of cognitive function in essential tremor. Neurology. 2006;66:69–74. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000192393.05850.ec. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000192393.05850.ec. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Louis ED, Benito-León J, Vega-Quiroga S, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Cognitive and motor functional activity in non-demented community-dwelling essential tremor cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81:997–1001. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.202838. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.202838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Louis ED, Benito-León J, Vega-Quiroga S, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Faster rate of cognitive decline in essential tremor cases than controls: a prospective study. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17:1291–1297. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03122.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.20doi: 10.03122.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Sánchez-Ferro A, Bermejo-Pareja F. Rate of cognitive decline during the premotor phase of essential tremor: a prospective study. Neurology. 2013;81:60–66. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318297ef2b. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318297ef2b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Mitchell AJ, Bermejo-Pareja F. Elderly-onset essential tremor and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study (NEDICES) J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2011;23:727–735. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-101572. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-101572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Elderly-onset essential tremor is associated with dementia. Neurology. 2006;66:1500–1505. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000216134.88617.de. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000216134.88617.de. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bermejo-Pareja F, Louis ED, Benito-León J. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Risk of incident dementia in essential tremor: a population-based study. Mov Disord. 2007;22:1573–1580. doi: 10.1002/mds.21553. doi: 10.1002/mds.21553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Posada IJ, et al. Population-based case-control study of cognitive function in early Parkinson’s disease (NEDICES) J Neurolog Sci. 2011;310:176–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.06.054. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.06.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hacker CD, Perlmutter JS, Criswell SR, Ances BM, Snyder AZ. Resting state functional connectivity of the striatum in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2012;135((Pt 12)):3699–3711. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.LaRoia H, Louis ED. Association between essential tremor and other neurodegenerative diseases: what is the epidemiological evidence? Neuroepidemiology. 2011;37:1–10. doi: 10.1159/000328866. doi: 10.1159/000328866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Bermejo-Pareja F. Neurological Disorders in Central Spain Study G. Risk of incident Parkinson's disease and parkinsonism in essential tremor: a population based study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:423–425. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.147223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Labiano-Fontcuberta A, Benito-León J. [Essential tremor and Parkinson's disease: are they associated?] Rev Neurol. 2012;55:479–489. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Louis ED, Benito-León J, Faust PL. Essential tremor seems to be a risk factor for Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;26:82–83. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.02.026. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lombardi WJ, Woolston DJ, Roberts JW, Gross RE. Cognitive deficits in patients with essential tremor. Neurology. 2001;57:785–790. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.5.785. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.5.785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gasparini M, Bonifati V, Fabrizio E, et al. Frontal lobe dysfunction in essential tremor: a preliminary study. J Neurol. 2001;248:399–402. doi: 10.1007/s004150170181. doi: 10.1007/s004150170181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Higginson CI, Wheelock VL, Levine D, King DS, Pappas CT, Sigvardt KA. Cognitive deficits in essential tremor consistent with frontosubcortical dysfunction. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30:760–765. doi: 10.1080/13803390701754738. doi: 10.1080/13803390701754738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Frisina PG, Tse W, Halbig TD, Libow LS. The pattern of cognitive-functional decline in elderly essential tremor patients: an exploratory-comparative study with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2009;10:238–242. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2008.10.013. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2008.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ozen Barut B, Gunal DI, Turkmen C, Mollahasanoglu A, Ankarali H. Clinical and cognitive profiles of patients with both Parkinson’s disease and essential tremor. Acta Neurol Belg. 2013;113:117–125. doi: 10.1007/s13760-012-0124-z. doi: 10.1007/s13760-012-0124-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sengul Y, Sengul HS, Sural MK, Bakim B, Forta H. A comparison between rate of nonmotor symptom development in essential tremor and Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Belg. 2015;115:289–294. doi: 10.1007/s13760-014-0408-6. doi: 10.1007/s13760-014-0408-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Benge J, Phillips-Sabol J, Phenis R. The neuropsychological assessment battery categories test as a measure of executive dysfunction in patients with Parkinson’s disease and essential tremor: an exploratory study. Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;28:1008–1018. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.950985. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.950985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Poewe W, Karamat E, Kemmler GW, Gerstenbrand F. The premorbid personality of patients with Parkinson's disease: a comparative study with healthy controls and patients with essential tremor. Adv Neurol. 1990;53:339–342. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jankovic J. Parkinson’s disease and movement disorders. 2nd ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Martínez-Martín P, Gil-Nagel A, Gracia LM, Gómez JB, Martínez-Sarries J, Bermejo F. Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale characteristics and structure. The Cooperative Multicentric Group. Mov Disord. 1994;9:76–83. doi: 10.1002/mds.870090112. doi: 10.1002/mds.870090112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Deuschl G, Bain P, Brin M. Consensus statement of the Movement Disorder Society on Tremor. Ad Hoc Scientific Committee. Mov Disord. 1998;13(Suppl. 3):2–23. doi: 10.1002/mds.870131303. doi: 10.1002/mds.870131303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV. Washington: APA; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Benito-León J, Louis ED, Romero JP, et al. Altered functional connectivity in essential tremor: A resting-state fMRI study. Medicine. 2015;94:e1936. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000001936. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000001936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Benito-Leon J, Louis ED, Puertas-Martín V, et al. Cognitive and neuropsychiatric features of orthostatic tremor: A case-control comparison. J Neurol Sci. 2016;361:137–143. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.12.031. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.12.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lobo A, Ezquerra J, Gómez Burgada F, Sala JM, Seva Díaz A. [Cognocitive mini-test (a simple practical test to detect intellectual changes in medical patients)] Actas Luso Esp Neurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines. 1979;7:189–202. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.WAIS-III . Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WMS-III: Weschler memory scale: technical manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Greenlief CL, Margolis RB, Erker GJ. Application of the Trail Making Test in differentiating neuropsychological impairment of elderly persons. Percept Mot Skills. 1985;61((Pt 2)):1283–1289. doi: 10.2466/pms.1985.61.3f.1283. doi: 10.2466/pms.1985.61.3f.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. PhD thesis. Nashville: George Peabody College for Teachers. George Peabody College for Teachers. 1935 doi: 10.1037/h0054651. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Heaton RK. Wisconsin card sorting test manual. iv. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1993. p. 230. Revised and expanded edition. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Krikorian R, Bartok J, Gay N. Tower of London procedure: a standard method and developmental data. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994;16:840–850. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402697. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000;55:1621–1626. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Benton AL. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: a clinical manual. xiii. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983. p. 146. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hooper E. Hooper visual organization test (VOT): manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1983. p. 34. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale (WMS-III) 3rd ed. XI. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. p. 212. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Benedict RHB. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1997. p. 57. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1983. p. 60. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Isaacs B, Kennie AT. The set test as an aid to the detection of dementia in old people. Br J Psychiatry. 1973;123:467–470. doi: 10.1192/bjp.123.4.467. doi: 10.1192/bjp.123.4.467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Barry D, Bates ME, Labouvie E. FAS and CFL forms of verbal fluency differ in difficulty: a meta-analytic study. Appl Neuropsychol. 2008;15:97–106. doi: 10.1080/09084280802083863. doi: 10.1080/09084280802083863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Morey LC. Personality assessment inventory (PAI): professional manual. 2nd ed. xiii. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2007. p. 385. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Benito-León J, Mitchell AJ, Vega S, Bermejo-Pareja F. A population-based study of cognitive function in older people with subjective memory complaints. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2010;22:159–170. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100972. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Benito-León J, Contador I, Louis ED, Cosentino S, Bermejo-Pareja F. Education and risk of incident dementia during the premotor and motor phases of essential tremor (NEDICES) Medicine. 2016;95:e4607. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000004607. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000004607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Walterfang M, van de Warrenburg BP. Cognitive impairment in “Other” movement disorders: Hidden defects and valuable clues. Mov Disord. 2014;29:694–703. doi: 10.1002/mds.25849. doi: 10.1002/mds.25849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Benito-León J, Labiano-Fontcuberta A. Linking essential tremor to the cerebellum: clinical evidence. Cerebellum. 2016;15:253–262. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0741-1. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0741-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ferguson BR, Gao WJ. Development of thalamocortical connections between the mediodorsal thalamus and the prefrontal cortex and its implication in cognition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:1027. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Tremor and Other Hyperkinetic Movements are provided here courtesy of Ubiquity Press

RESOURCES