Skip to main content
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health logoLink to Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health
. 2017 Jan 13;11:4. doi: 10.1186/s13034-016-0142-3

Temperament and character traits in female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury disorder with and without comorbid borderline personality disorder

Taru Tschan 1,#, Claudia Peter-Ruf 1,#, Marc Schmid 2, Tina In-Albon 1,
PMCID: PMC5237331  PMID: 28101133

Abstract

Background

Temperament and character traits of adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury disorder (NSSI) might differentiate those- with and without comorbid borderline personality disorder (BPD).

Methods

Participants were 57 female adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD (NSSI − BPD), 14 adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD (NSSI + BPD), 32 clinical controls (CC), and 64 nonclinical controls (NC). Temperament and character traits were assessed with the Junior Temperament and Character Inventory, and impulsivity with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale and a Go/NoGo task.

Results

Adolescents with NSSI disorder scored significantly higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness than CC. The NSSI + BPD group scored even higher than the NSSI − BPD group on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence and cooperativeness (d ≥ 0.72). Adolescents with NSSI reported higher levels of impulsivity than the CC and NC group. However, this difference was not found in a Go/NoGo task.

Conclusions

The results provide further evidence for a distinct diagnostic entity of NSSI disorder.

Keywords: Nonsuicidal self-injury, Borderline personality disorder, Temperament, Character, Impulsivity, Go/NoGo

Background

Due to the inclusion of nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5) [1] as a research diagnosis in section III, further studies are needed to enable a better understanding of this behavior. Independent of classification discussions, high prevalence and comorbidity rates [24], low quality of life [5], and increased risk of suicidality [6] highlight the importance of further research on NSSI. Special attention should be paid to adolescents, as NSSI often has its onset during this time [4, 7]. Previously, NSSI was generally assessed as one of the nine symptoms of Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), however only a minority of adolescents with NSSI suffer from BPD [5, 8]. Several differences in the phenomenology and functions of NSSI can be found between patients with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD) and patients with NSSI without BPD (NSSI − BPD). Patients with NSSI + BPD show more frequent and severe NSSI, greater diagnostic comorbidity, more severe depressive symptomatology, suicidal ideation, and emotion dysregulation than patients with NSSI − BPD [9, 10]. Regarding functions of NSSI, adolescents with NSSI + BPD endorsed higher self-punishment, anti-suicide, and anti-dissociation functions of NSSI than adolescents with NSSI − BPD [11].

Among different personality concepts, Cloninger´s [12, 13] biopsychosocial personality model seems to be able to describe healthy as well as pathological temperament and character traits, and to differentiate between patients with and without personality disorders [14, 15]. The extended model [13] includes four temperament dimensions (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, persistence) and three character dimensions (self-directedness, cooperativeness, self-transcendence), see Table 1. Low levels of self-directedness and cooperativeness are characteristics for personality disorders [16].

Table 1.

Temperament and character dimensions

Dimension High level Low level
Temperament
Novelty seeking Curious, impulsive, sensation seeking Indifferent, thoughtful, modest
Harm avoidance Worried, pessimistic, frightened, shy Relaxed, optimistic, fearless, confident, talkative
Reward dependence Sensitive, warm, dependent Cold, secluded, independent
Persistence Hard-working, ambitious, perfectionist Inactive, lethargic, pragmatic
Character
Self-directedness Mature, effective, responsible, determined, high self-acceptance Immature, unreliable, indecisive, low self-acceptance
Cooperativeness Social tolerant, empathic, helpful Social intolerant, critical, cold, not helpful, destructive
Self-transcendence Experienced, patient, creative, self-forgetting, connected to the universe, spiritual Uncomprehending, proud, unimaginative, lack of humility

Patients with BPD often show a temperament profile consisting of both high harm avoidance and novelty seeking [14, 1618]. According to Cloninger, Praybeck, Svrakic, and Wetzel [19], a personality pattern consisting of high novelty seeking and high harm avoidance represents an approach-avoidance conflict that may cause affective instability, a core feature of BPD. Studies of adolescents with NSSI − BPD are needed to investigate the link between NSSI and the described personality pattern, especially high novelty seeking and harm avoidance. Indeed, higher levels of novelty seeking were found in adolescents with NSSI compared to adolescents without NSSI [20]. Furthermore, adolescents with depressive disorder and self-harm behavior reported more harm avoidance than those without self-harm [21].

Low self-directedness is related to self-injurious behavior in adolescents [20, 21], BPD in adolescents [18] and BPD in adults [14]. Higher levels of cooperativeness were found in female adolescents with self-harm behavior (self-injuring behavior including suicidal behavior) compared to those without self-harm behavior [22], whereas adults with BPD showed lower levels of cooperativeness than adult controls [14]. Ohmann et al. [22] offer the explanation that higher cooperativeness levels in adolescents with self-harm behavior may be related to pronounced helplessness. High self-transcendence is linked to NSSI in adolescents [20] and to BPD in adults [14]. Low reward dependence is linked to internalizing symptoms like depression and anxiety [23], but no association has been found between reward dependence and NSSI [20], nor between reward dependence and self-harm behavior [21, 22]. Kaess et al. [18] found lower reward dependence in adolescents with BPD than in clinical and healthy controls. Further, persistence is linked neither to BPD [14, 18] nor to NSSI [20] or self-harm behavior.

In summary, for BPD, most studies support the personality pattern suggested by Cloninger et al. [16, 19], consisting of high novelty seeking and harm avoidance as well as low levels of self-directedness and cooperativeness [14, 18]. Adolescents with NSSI show a similar personality pattern to adolescents with BPD, however most studies have not controlled for comorbid BPD [e.g. 20, 21]. Studies using the big five model found similar personality traits related to self-injurious behavior, namely high neuroticism (comparable to harm avoidance), low agreeableness (comparable to cooperativeness), and low conscientiousness (comparable to self-directedness and persistence) [24, 25]. One part of novelty seeking, impulsivity, might explain the difficulties self-injurers have with resisting the urge to injure themselves [26]. NSSI itself is often an impulsive act, as most of the individuals with NSSI think about the act for less than five minutes before committing it [27]. Indeed, on self-report measures individuals with NSSI indicated higher impulsivity than individuals without NSSI [26, 28, 29], and patients with repetitive self-harm reported even higher impulsivity than patients with onetime self-harm behavior [30]. However, previous research has found low convergence between self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity [for a meta-analysis see [31].

Response inhibition, one aspect of impulsivity, can be measured with a Go/NoGo task. Janis and Nock [29] compared self-reported impulsivity with experimentally assessed impulsivity in adolescents with NSSI. While participants with NSSI scored higher on self-reported impulsivity, they did not differ from the mixed clinical and nonclinical comparison groups without NSSI on behavioral measures. This result has been replicated in studies of adults with NSSI [26, 32]. The difference between self-reported and experimentally assessed impulsivity may be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs. While self-report questionnaires measure general response tendencies (traits), behavioral tasks may in fact measure spontaneous reactions that are influenced by current cognitive processes [32]. Therefore, it seems important not only to investigate impulsivity with self-report measures, but also with behavioral tasks.

In summary, previous research is consistent with the notion that certain temperament traits underlie features of BPD symptoms. However, it remains unclear, if the same pattern can be found in a sample of adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD. None of the presented studies assessed self-injuring behavior according to the DSM-5 criteria [e.g. 2022]; whereas Hefti et al. [20] investigated a school sample, Joyce et al. [21] investigated depressed adolescents with and without self-harm behavior, and Ohmann et al. [22] investigated adolescents presenting at in- and outpatient clinics. Thus, the samples were heterogeneous. To our knowledge, no study has investigated Cloninger’s temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI disorder with and without BPD. Cloninger’s personality traits might be especially suitable for the distinction between adolescents with and without BPD because of its dimensional structure. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate impulsivity (self-report and a behavioral measures), temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI disorder (according to DSM-5), and differences in personality dimensions according to Cloninger et al. [13] between adolescents with NSSI with and without comorbid BPD.

We hypothesized that there are dimensional differences in temperament and character traits between four groups of adolescents. Specifically, we addressed the following research questions.

  1. Do adolescents with NSSI disorder show a different personality pattern in comparison to the clinical control (CC) and the nonclinical control (NC) groups? Taking the results of previous studies into account, we hypothesized that adolescents with NSSI disorder would show higher values on novelty seeking, self-transcendence, and harm avoidance as well as lower values on self-directedness compared to the NC and the CC groups.

  2. Do adolescents with NSSI + BPD show a distinct personality pattern in comparison to adolescents with NSSI − BPD? To our knowledge, no other studies exist, and therefore this analysis was exploratory.

  3. Do adolescents with NSSI − BPD report more impulsivity than the NC and the CC groups? Is this difference evident in an emotional Go/NoGo task? Because of the heterogeneous results of previous studies, this analysis was also exploratory.

Methods

Procedure

All participants and their parents were informed about the study and gave their written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The local ethics committee approved the study. First, the clinical interviews were conducted and questionnaires distributed, and then the Go/NoGo task was administered.

Measures

Diagnostic assessments

To examine the participants’ current or past DSM-IV-TR diagnoses for Axis I disorders, we conducted two structured interviews with each adolescent. The Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents (Kinder-DIPS) [33] assesses the most frequent mental disorders in childhood and adolescence. Questions for substance use disorders were asked from the adult DIPS [34]. The Kinder-DIPS has good validity and reliability for Axis I disorders (child version, kappa = 0.48–0.88) [35]. NSSI was assessed according to the DSM-5 research criteria, with questions reformulated as criteria. Interrater reliability estimates for the diagnosis of NSSI were very good (kappa = 0.90). Before conducting the interviews, Master’s students in clinical child psychology underwent systematic training.

Participants were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II disorders (SCID-II) [36], to assess for personality disorders. The SCID-II has been found to be suitable for use among adolescents [37]. Interrater reliability for BPD in our sample was very good (kappa = 1.00).

The Borderline Symptom List 95 (BSL-95) [38] was used as an additional instrument to measure the degree of borderline symptomatology. The items are based on the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV. The self-report questionnaire shows good psychometric properties [39].

The Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI) [40] is a self-report measure assessing the seven temperament and character traits based on Cloninger’s [13] biopsychosocial model of personality. The scales have good levels of internal consistency, with Cronbach´s α ranging from 0.79 to 0.85 [40]. The internal consistencies within the present sample ranged from α = 0.76 to 0.82.

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) [41], German version [42] is a valid and reliable self-report questionnaire to assess impulsivity with three subscales: Attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity. The internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.81.

The Youth Self Report (YSR) [43, 44] measures a broad range of psychopathology. The problem behavior section of the YSR consists of the following primary subscales: withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior. Two second-order scales reflecting internalizing and externalizing problems and a total problems score can be calculated. Internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.94 for the total score, α = 0.94 for the internalizing score, and α = 0.79 for the externalizing score.

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [45] consists of 21 items and assesses depressive symptoms. The internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.95.

Non-emotional and emotional Go/NoGo task

Participants were instructed to press a button as fast as possible if a Go stimulus appears on the screen and to suppress reactions to NoGo stimuli. Participants had a practice run with six trials, followed by the non-emotional Go/NoGo task. Afterwards participants completed an emotional Go/NoGo task with four combinations of angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions with 12 trials for each combination. For all runs, targets occurred on 50% of the trials. The order of the four emotional runs and the trials within each run were randomized across participants.

Facial stimuli consisted of colored angry, happy, and neutral expressions from 18 individuals (9 females) taken from the NimStimFace Stimulus set [46]. Non-emotional stimuli (“+” and “×”) were presented for 200 ms and emotional stimuli for 500 ms, after a 500 ms fixation cross. The longer presentation time for emotional stimuli was due to the higher complexity of faces compared to crosses, similar to Hare et al. [47]. The inter-stimulus interval was 1.5 s, in which a reaction was still possible. Stimuli were presented with E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA), and omission (no reaction to Go) and commission (reaction to NoGo) errors as well as reaction times were recorded simultaneously. Omission errors indicate inattention [48], commission errors insufficient response inhibition [49], and reaction time to Go stimuli as a measure of response bias, with faster reactions indicating a response or attention bias toward the shown emotion [50].

Data analyses

Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were used to compare the groups (NC, CC, NSSI − BPD, NSSI + BPD) on dependent variables such as impulsivity and psychopathology. One-way between groups analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) calculated to further analyze significant group differences of MANOVAs. As we were interested in specific group differences, we set up orthogonal comparisons for psychopathology, personality, and self-reported impulsivity. The first comparison contrasted the NC group with the clinical groups (CC, NSSI, NSSI + BPD), the second contrasted the CC group with the two NSSI groups (NSSI − BPD and NSSI + BPD), and the third contrasted the two NSSI groups (NSSI − BPD and NSSI + BPD). Due to the small sample size, the analyses proceeded using bootstrapping with 2000 resamples. To correct for multiple testing, p values were adjusted according to the Bonferroni-Holm procedure. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 24.

For the Go/NoGo task, a similar analytic strategy was used. First, outliers (z-values > 3) were excluded, then the sensitivity index d’ (z(Reaction rate to Go) – z(Reaction rate to NoGo) was calculated, as a measure of discrimination, with lower values representing an inability to distinguish between stimuli and lower performance levels [52]. To examine group differences, the non-emotional Go/NoGo task was evaluated with a one-way ANOVA, and the emotional Go/NoGo tasks were analyzed separately for emotional Go (neutral NoGo) and for neutral Go (emotional NoGo) with MANOVAs. These analyses were calculated for the sensitivity index d’, errors of commission and omission, as well as for the reaction time on Go trials. If the Levene test indicated that the variance homogeneity of an outcome was violated, we transformed it for the analysis (log10 or sqrt) and if indicated, Greenhouse Geisser corrected values were used. Significance levels were set at α = 0.05.

Results

Participants

Participants were 167 female adolescents, aged 12–19 years (M = 15.94, SD = 1.47), recruited from different inpatient child and adolescent psychiatric units in Switzerland and Germany. Participants included 57 adolescents fulfilling the DSM-5 research criteria for NSSI disorder (NSSI) but not for BPD, 14 adolescents with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD), 32 adolescents with a DSM-IV [51] diagnosis other than current or past NSSI (clinical controls, CC), and 64 nonclinical adolescents who had no current or past experience of mental disorders (nonclinical controls, NC). Participants were similar with respect to age, Welch’s F (3, 47.19) = 0.41. Regarding nationalities, most of the participants were Swiss and German, except for two Italians, one Thai and one Pole. The three most frequent mental disorders in all groups were: major depression (37.50% in CC, 70.18% in NSSI, 78.6% in NSSI + BPD), social phobia (34.38% in CC, 36.84% in NSSI, 42.9% in NSSI + BPD), and specific phobia (28.13% in CC, 19.30% in NSSI, 35.70% in NSSI + BPD). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was a common comorbid disorder in NSSI (14.04%) and NSSI + BPD (50%), with an additional two participants from the CC group also presenting with PTSD (6.25%). Groups differed significantly regarding the diagnoses depression, χ 2 (2, N = 103) = 11.87, p < 0.01, and PTSD, p < 0.01, according to a two-sided Fisher’s exact test. There were no significant differences regarding any other DSM-IV disorders assessed with clinical interviews. Further comorbid diagnoses of the clinical groups were dysthymia, oppositional defiant disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, bulimia nervosa, anorexia nervosa, obsessive–compulsive disorder, agoraphobia, panic disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Groups differed significantly regarding the number of diagnoses, F (2, 100) = 30.37, p < 0.01, with patients in the NSSI + BPD group meeting significantly more diagnoses than the other groups (M = 5.43, SD = 1.83), and the NSSI − BPD group meeting significantly more diagnoses (M = 3.39, SD = 1.36) than the CC group (M = 2.03, SD = 1.00). In addition to the number of diagnoses, significant group differences emerged for psychopathology, for both internalizing and externalizing problems (according to the Youth Self Report). NSSI + BPD scored highest, followed by NSSI, CC and NC, see Table 2. Regarding borderline symptomatology, adolescents with NSSI − BPD differed significantly from adolescents with NSSI + BPD on the subscales self-destruction and hostility. Furthermore, NSSI − BPD scored above the cut off on the subscale for social isolation.

Table 2.

Mean (standard deviations) of characteristics of non-clinical adolescents (NC), clinical controls without NSSI (CC), adolescents with NSSI disorder (NSSI), and adolescents with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD), as well as ANOVA with orthogonal contrasts and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) between non-clinical and clinical groups (NC vs. rest), clinical controls and NSSI (CC vs. NSSI and NSSI + BPD), and NSSI disorder vs. Borderline personality disorder (NSSI vs. NSSI + BPD)

Characteristic NC CC NSSI NSSI + BPD NC vs. rest Cohen’s CC vs. Cohen’s NSSI vs. Cohen’s
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) d NSSI total d NSSI + BPD d
YSR (n = 57) (n = 28) (n = 47) (n = 11) t (139) t (139) t (139)
Total 57.60 (18.70) 81.80 (21.60) 105.38 (29.97) 134.28 (22.40) 12.56** 2.22 7.04** 1.55 4.03** 1.02
YSR exta 9.79 (6.56) 12.38 (6.45) 17.47 (9.15) 30.76 (7.82) 6.77** 1.43 4.58** 1.51 3.50** 1.52
YSR int 9.83 (6.46) 23.68 (9.56) 32.49 (9.53) 41.18 (8.68) 14.66** 2.76 6.22** 1.44 3.10** 0.94
BDIb 7.02 (7.20) 21.89 (12.68) 33.40 (12.17) 43.20 (13.29) 13.17** 2.39 4.70** 1.31 1.82* 0.81
BSL-95 (n = 57) (n = 25) (n = 38) (n = 9) t (125) t (125) t (125)
Totala 47.67 (28.69) 117.31 (68.98) 182.84 (68.26) 240.55 (70.52) 11.31** 2.42 4.01** 1.38 1.46* 0.86
Dysphoriac 20.56 (8.98) 26.40 (10.23) 30.53 (6.72) 33.04 (6.93) −2.27 1.13 −.546 0.67 −.110 0.38
t (94.85) t (34.51) t (16.74)
Self-perceptiona 3.63 (4.37) 15.13 (14.98) 28.10 (17.06) 41.85 (20.82) 11.65** 1.85 4.30** 1.14 2.12 0.79
t (96.16) t (34.61) t (26.40)
Affect regulationb 6.04 (6.33) 19.48 (11.99) 28.66 (11.29) 36.67 (7.12) 13.42** 2.59 4.54** 1.31 2.95* 0.77
Self-destructionb 1.46 (2.38) 9.20 (7.91) 25.66 (11.55) 34.37 (7.88) 16.28** 3.12 8.17** 2.31 2.21** 0.81
Social isolationb 4.09 (4.97) 12.58 (9.65) 21.87 (12.66) 29.33 (10.46) 10.38** 1.96 4.31** 1.21 1.81* 0.62
Hostilityb 2.34 (3.14) 4.64 (4.41) 8.82 (5.92) 14.89 (5.82) 8.36** 1.58 4.69** 1.35 2.74** 1.05
Intrusionsa 1.34 (2.11) 6.32 (6.37) 12.13 (7.50) 20.33 (12.08) 10.51** 1.77 4.58** 1.15 1.65 0.99
JTCI (n = 51) (n = 26) (n = 46) (n = 11) t (130) t (130) t (130)
Novelty seeking (T)a 47.29 (8.20) 43.00 (8.62) 48.20 (11.61) 56.00 (8.31) 0.66 0.20 3.42** 0,96 2.39** 0.72
Harm avoidance (T) 49.33 (10.18) 59.38 (8.59) 61.35 (11.10) 69.64 (8.51) 7.32** 1.47 2.34** 0,66 2.44** 0.79
Reward dependence (T) 57.06 (8.37) 52.04 (9.20) 49.96 (10.77) 45.91 (12.03) −4.18** 0.79 −1.64 0,39 −1.24 0.37
Persistence (T) 50.22 (10.21) 53.73 (9.93) 45.09 (11.74) 35.27 (9.70) −2.71** 0.54 −4.92** 1.31 −2.74** 0.88
Self-directedness (C) 52.22 (10.41) 43.88 (10.45) 33.22 (11.70) 26.73 (9.81) −8.51** 1.68 −4.97** 1.32 −1.78 0.58
Cooperativeness (C) 53.75 (8.89) 56.88 (9.21) 54.93 (11.77) 46.27 (9.70) −0.54 0.11 −2.41* 0.62 −2.56** 0.78
Self-transcendence (C) 49.43 (9.58) 53.92 (10.68) 50.02 (9.12) 50.82 (11.81) 1.15 0.21 −1.38 0.34 0.24 0.08
BIS (n = 28) (n = 21) (n = 29) (n = 8) t (82) t (82) t (82)
Impulsivity (BIS) 20.76 (3.15) 20.06 (3.47) 22.97 (3.94) 26.85 (2.78) 2.99** 0.77 4.70** 1.45 2.78** 1.07
Attentional 15.61 (4.01) 14.90 (3.16) 18.25 (4.10) 20.88 (1.89) 2.67** 0.72 4.24** 1.55 1.77* 0.72
Non-planning 25.52 (4.33) 24.59 (5.13) 27.47 (5.76) 34.63 (5.07) 2.72** 0.68 4.27** 1.24 3.51** 1.31
Motor 21.16 (3.96) 20.70 (3.97) 23.21 (6.90) 25.04 (4.04) 1.46 0.39 2.24* 0.70 0.89 0.29

YSR Youth self report (ext = externalizing, int = internalizing); BDI Beck Depression Inventory-II; JTCI Junior Temperament and Character Inventory; BIS Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

Bootstrapped and Bonferroni-Holm corrected p values * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

a log transformation, b root transformation, c reciprocal transformation

Junior Temperament and Character Inventory

As reported in Table 2, significant group differences were shown on the temperament scales novelty seeking, F(3, 130) = 4.32, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.09, harm avoidance, F(3, 130) = 18.80, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.30, reward dependence, F(3, 130) = 6.47, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.13, and persistence F(3, 130) = 9.57, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.18, as well as on the character scales self-directedness, F(3, 130) = 32.71, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.43, and cooperativeness, F(3, 130) = 2.99, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.06. There was no significant group difference regarding self-transcendence, F(3, 130) = 1.28, p = 0.28, η2 = 0.03. Compared to clinical controls, adolescents with NSSI scored higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness. The harm avoidance score was over the cut off while the other scores were within the normal range. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD showed even higher scores for novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower scores for persistence and cooperativeness than adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD scored above the cut off on harm avoidance and below the cut off on persistence and self-directedness.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

Regarding the MANOVA for the BIS subscales, the group main effect was significant, F(3, 82) = 9.21, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.25. There was no significant Group x Impulsivity interaction, F(6, 164) = 1.36, p = 0.23, η2 = 0.05, indicating that the group differences are the same for all three subscales of the BIS. As shown in Table 2, the subsequent one-way ANOVA yielded significant group differences regarding impulsivity for the total scale, F(3, 130) = 9.21, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.25, as well as for the subscales attentional, F (3, 130) = 7.47, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.21, and non-planning impulsivity, F(3, 130) = 8.32, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.23, but not for the subscale motor impulsivity, F(3, 130) = 2.13, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.07.

Go/NoGo-Task

Regarding the non-emotional task, there was no significant group effect for participants’ sensitivity index, F(3, 151) = 0.93, p = 0.43, commission errors, F(3, 151) = 0.43, p = 0.73, omission errors, F(3, 154) = 1.22, p = 0.31, or reaction time, F(3, 147) = 2.06, p = 0.11. The ANOVAs for the emotional task, when emotional faces were Go trials, revealed no significant main effects or interactions except for commission errors. There was a significant main effect for facial emotion, F(1, 148) = 29.83, p < 0.01, indicating a higher commission error rate for angry faces than for happy faces. Regarding omission errors, the main effect for facial emotion reached significance, F(1, 155) = 65.50, p < 0.01, indicating a higher omission error rate for angry faces than for happy faces. For reaction time (Go), the main effect for facial emotion was significant, F(1, 154) = 20.95, p < 0.01, indicating a faster reaction to happy compared to angry faces. The ANOVAs conducted for the emotional task, when neutral faces were Go trials revealed no significant effects for the sensitivity index, commission and omission error rates. For reaction time as an outcome, only one significant main effect was found: facial emotion, F(1, 146) = 11.94, p < 0.01, indicating a faster reaction to neutral faces, when happy faces served as NoGo compared to angry faces. The means and standard deviations are displayed in Table 3.

Table 3.

Sensitivity index d’, commission and omission errors of the Go/NoGo, as well as reaction times for go trials of non-clinical adolescents (NC), clinical controls without NSSI (CC), adolescents with NSSI disorder (NSSI), and adolescents with NSSI and borderline personality disorder (NSSI + BPD)

Condition NC CC NSSI NSSI + BPD
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
d’
X 0.16 (1.16) 0.31 (1.07) −0.01 (1.30) −0.27 (1.29)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo) 0.12 (1.66) −0.18 (1.59) 0.02 (1.38) −0.72 (1.46)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo) −0.04 (1.47) 0.42 (0.87) 0.08 (1.37) −0.86 (1.50)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo) 0.05 (1.12) 0.19 (1.19) −0.10 (1.33) −0.40 (1.50)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo) 0.34 (1.44) 0.36 (0.82) 0.06 (1.46) −0.62 (1.20)
Commission
X 1.95 (4.55) 2.00 (5.19) 2.02 (4.57) 3.57 (7.45)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo) 15.42 (14.80) 15.42 (11.22) 18.63 (16.92) 21.15 (16.44)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo) 8.67 (11.43) 6.67 (10.24) 8.82 (11.80) 13.39 (11.46)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo) 5.83 (9.34) 4.03 (9.89) 6.37 (9.37) 4.46 (9.31)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo) 5.42 (10.88) 3.23 (6.43) 5.19 (9.31) 6.25 (9.49)
Omission
X 14.34 (13.24) 12.26 (13.09) 17.21 (15.13) 18.57 (10.46)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo) 7.38 (12.37) 10.48 (12.95) 6.37 (6.76) 11.61 (10.36)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo) 0.82 (3.12) 0.00 (0.00) 0.47 (2.40) 1.79 (4.54)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo) 2.29 (6.71) 2.92 (5.38) 3.54 (9.61) 8.65 (9.39)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo) 4.30 (16.44) 6.05 (18.78) 6.60 (18.61) 12.50 (18.99)
RT Go
X 373.62 (42.10) 378.22 (41.96) 361.03 (40.66) 353.66 (29.87)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo) 514.52 (86.87) 529.93 (109.17) 509.37 (83.11) 421.31 (119.90)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo) 483.46 (72.24) 492.22 (81.30) 478.21 (78.84) 487.61 (96.52)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo) 503.67 (86.93) 522.27 (89.08) 516.01 (82.00) 517.93 (100.72)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo) 533.06 (87.16) 546.78 (106.83) 527.60 (95.38) 551.99 (89.60)

d’ sensitivity index; Commission Commission error; Omission Omission error; RT Go reaction time for the go condition

There were no significant group effects

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate temperament and character traits on the basis of Cloninger's [12, 13] personality model, with a special focus on impulsivity in adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD (NSSI − BPD), adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD (NSSI + BPD), a clinical control group, and a nonclinical control group. As expected, the groups showed distinct personality profiles. The JTCI scales as well as most YSR scales indicate a staircase-like appearance ranging from nonclinical adolescents to adolescents with NSSI + BPD. Adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD scored higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on self-directedness, persistence and cooperativeness than clinical controls. In adolescents with NSSI + BPD this personality pattern was even more pronounced than in adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Thus, we were able to replicate the personality pattern consisting of high harm avoidance and novelty seeking in adolescents with NSSI + BPD, similar to Cloninger [16] and Kaess et al. [18]. The approach-avoidance conflict generated from this pattern might be a reason for the emotional instability patients with BPD experience [19]. In addition, we extended these findings to adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD. In these patients, the personality pattern described above was less pronounced. Nevertheless, the harm avoidance score above cut off indicates that adolescents with NSSI − BPD are more careful, fearful, insecure, and negativistic than the adolescents from the CC and the NC groups. Adolescents with NSSI − BPD differed from adolescents with NSSI + BPD regarding psychopathology and partially in borderline symptomatology but nevertheless showed a similar personality pattern to adolescents with NSSI + BPD. This result underlines the need for a dimensional personality assessment to better understand adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Further research should focus on maladaptive personality traits that do not constitute a formal personality disorder and on the validation of the dimensional personality model suggested in section III of the DSM-5.

Results of the present study replicated a profile of lower levels of self-directedness in adolescents with NSSI (−BPD and +BPD) than adolescents without NSSI, similar to Hefti et al. [20] and Joyce et al. [21]. In contrast to Ohmann et al. [22], we found lower levels of cooperativeness in adolescents with NSSI compared to adolescents without NSSI, however this result is similar to the low level of cooperativeness found in adolescents with BPD [53]. Lower cooperativeness may cause more interpersonal conflict and distress through socially intolerant, critical, and destructive conflict behavior. In fact, previous research indicates that adolescents with NSSI frequently report problems in social interactions [54] that can trigger NSSI [55]. Compared to the CC group, the level of persistence in adolescents with NSSI was low but still in the normal range. Previous studies have shown that adolescents with NSSI give up faster when pursuing goals, while adolescents without NSSI are more diligent and persevering [40]. All groups were similar regarding self-transcendence, therefore, we could not find supporting evidence for a higher self-transcendence as previously reported in adolescents with NSSI [20] and adults with BPD [14]. This may be explained by differences in the study populations (school sample vs. clinical sample, female vs. male adolescents, adolescents vs. adults and NSSI vs. BPD).

To summarize, there was a significant difference in temperament and character traits between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD, despite the small NSSI + BPD sample size (n = 14). Compared to the other groups, the NSSI − BPD group displayed higher standard deviations on the subscales of the JTCI, indicating the heterogeneity of this group. Considerable diagnostic heterogeneity among adolescents with NSSI has been described in earlier studies [2].

Adolescents with NSSI disorder (−BPD and +BPD) showed more novelty seeking than the CC group as well as higher scores on all subscales of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (attentional, non-planning, and motor impulsivity). However, this difference was not evident in the Go/NoGo task with neither a group effect, nor an emotion effect emerging. Happy faces were associated with faster reactions and a lower error rate compared to angry faces, indicating that happy faces are easier to discern than angry faces. Our results are in line with several other studies that indicated more self-reported impulsivity in adolescents [26, 29] and adults with NSSI [32], but failed to show this difference on behavioral measures. This leaves the question open, as to whether adolescents with NSSI perceive themselves as more impulsive than they actually are. However, this discrepancy between self-report and behavioral measures is not only observed in adolescents with NSSI, but also represents a general difficulty in the measurement of impulsivity that may be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs [32]. It remains to be investigated, if the difference between self-reported and experimentally assessed impulsivity can be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs, or if adolescents with NSSI are able to suppress their impulsivity for an experimental task. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD reported even more impulsivity than adolescents with NSSI − BPD, especially more non-planning impulsivity (lack of future orientation and foresight). Highly impulsive individuals may be especially motivated to act rashly in the context of negative emotions because long-term benefits become less important compared to short-term gains of emotion regulation, e.g. The Theory of Urgency [56], also see [57]. Therefore, individuals with high levels of non-planning impulsivity may be highly motivated to obtain the immediate benefits of NSSI (e.g., relief of negative emotions) with less concern for the long-term consequences of NSSI. There was no significant difference between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and with NSSI − BPD in the Go/NoGo task.

The results of the present study should be interpreted in the context of some limitations. The design of the study was cross-sectional. Therefore, the current study cannot explain whether certain temperament and character traits might favor the development of NSSI. This should be investigated in future prospective longitudinal studies. Nevertheless, results indicate an association between temperament and character traits and NSSI disorder. Due to the small sample sizes of adolescents with BPD, comorbidity with other personality disorders could not be included in the analyses. The recommendation of the DSM-5 is to apply a diagnosis of a personality disorder in children and adolescents when maladaptive personality traits appear to be pervasive, persistent, unlikely to be limited to a particular developmental stage or another mental disorder, and after one year of persistent symptoms. Given the mean age of the participants under 16 years of age, we were careful applying a diagnosis of a personality disorder. However, despite the small NSSI + BPD sample size, significant differences emerged between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD. The high prevalence of NSSI in inpatient samples (50%) [9] represented a challenge for the recruitment of a clinical inpatient sample without NSSI. Our sample consisted of female adolescents admitted to a psychiatric unit and therefore generalizations to male outpatients must be made with caution. Regarding the Go/NoGo task, the low error rate indicates that the response pressure was too low. Therefore, future studies should use a higher ratio of Go stimuli to NoGo stimuli.

A strength of this study was the use of the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for NSSI disorder in a clinical sample. In addition, a clinical control group of adolescents with other mental disorders without NSSI was included. This allowed us to identify temperament and character traits specific to NSSI disorder with and without BPD. To our knowledge, this is the first study comparing temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD in an inpatient setting. In addition to self-report measures, impulsivity was assessed using an experimental task.

Conclusions

Given the differences in temperament and character traits between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD, a personality assessment using the JTCI [40] might be useful for the diagnostic distinction between adolescents with NSSI with and without BPD. A clear distinction of these two groups might be helpful when choosing a specific treatment for adolescents engaging in NSSI. As specific treatment programs for adolescents with NSSI are still in development, practitioners mostly use treatment programs for BPD [58]. The development of specific treatment programs for adolescents with NSSI may not only optimize treatment, but also allow an early intervention, preventing chronic conditions [59]. Future studies should investigate temperament and character traits of adolescents with NSSI in the long-term as well as the effects of psychotherapy on character and temperament development.

Authors’ contributions

TT and CR completed the data analyses and made substantial contributions to the interpretation of the data, the drafting, and the revision of the manuscript. TI and MS contributed to the ideas, the acquisition and interpretation of the data, the drafting and the revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank the participants in this study as well as the research assistants and graduate students on the project at the University of Basel for their assistance in data collection and management. The authors thank the following clinics for recruitment: Zentrum für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und -psychotherapie Clienia Littenheid AG, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrischer Dienst Koenigsfelden, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Kriens, St. Elisabethen-Krankenhaus Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Loerrach, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Chur, Universitaere Psychiatrische Kliniken Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Basel, Universitaetsklinik fuer Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Bern, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrische Klinik Solothurn, and Klinik Sonnenhof Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrisches Zentrum Ganterschwil.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have competing interests.

Availability of data and material

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Consent for publication

All participants and parents gave their written consent.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The local ethics committee (Ethikkommission Beider Basel, EKBB) approved the study.

Funding

This study is supported by grant project 100014_135205 awarded to Tina In-Albon in collaboration with Marc Schmid by the Swiss National Science Foundation.

Abbreviations

NSSI

nonsuicidal self-injury

BPD

Borderline personality disorder

NSSI − BPD

adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD

NSSI + BPD

adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD

CC

clinical controls

NC

nonclinical controls

DSM-5

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed

PTSD

posttraumatic stress disorder

Kinder-DIPS

Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents

SCID-II

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II disorders

BSL-95

Borderline Symptom List 95

JTCI

Junior Temperament and Character Inventory

BIS

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

YSR

Youth Self Report

BDI-II

Beck Depression Inventory-II

ANOVA

analyses of variance

MANOVA

multivariate analyses of variance

Footnotes

Taru Tschan and Claudia Peter-Ruf contributed equally to this work

Contributor Information

Taru Tschan, Phone: +49(0)6341 28035641, Email: tschan@uni-landau.de.

Claudia Peter-Ruf, Email: claudia.peter.ruf@gmail.com.

Marc Schmid, Phone: +41612658974, Email: marc.schmid@upkbs.ch.

Tina In-Albon, Phone: +49(0)634128035639, Email: in-albon@uni-landau.de.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Auerbach RP, Kim JC, Chango JM, Spiro WJ, Cha C, Gold J, et al. Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: examining the role of child abuse, comorbidity, and disinhibition. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:579–584. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Plener PL, Kapusta ND, Kölch MG, Kaess M, Brunner R. Non-suicidal self-injury as autonomous diagnosis-implications for research and clinic of the DSM-5 proposal to establish the diagnosis of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in adolescents. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2012;40:113–120. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zetterqvist M, Lundh LG, Dahlström Ö, Svedin CG. Prevalence and function of non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) in a community sample of adolescents, using suggested DSM-5 criteria for a potential NSSI disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:759–773. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9712-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.In-Albon T, Ruf C, Schmid M. Proposed diagnostic criteria for the DSM-5 of nonsuicidal self-injury in female adolescents: diagnostic and clinical correlates. Psychiatry J. 2013 doi: 10.1155/2013/159208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Victor SE, Klonsky ED. Daily emotion in non-suicidal self-injury. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70:364–375. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Andrews T, Martin G, Hasking P, Page A. Predictors of onset for non-suicidal self-injury within a school-based sample of adolescents. Prev Sci. 2014;15:850–859. doi: 10.1007/s11121-013-0412-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zlotnick C, Mattia JI, Zimmerman M. Clinical correlates of self-mutilation in a sample of general psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187:296–301. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199905000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glenn CR, Klonsky ED. Nonsuicidal self-injury disorder: an empirical investigation in adolescent psychiatric patients. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013;42:496–507. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.794699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Turner BJ, Dixon-Gordon KL, Austin SB, Rodriguez MA, Rosenthal MZ, Chapman AL. Non-suicidal self-injury with and without borderline personality disorder: differences in self-injury and diagnostic comorbidity. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bracken-Minor KL, McDevitt-Murphy ME. Differences in features of non-suicidal self-injury according to borderline personality disorder screening status. Arch Suicide Res. 2014;18:88–103. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2013.809040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:573–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Barnow DS, Rüge J, Spitzer C, Freyberger HJ. Temperament und Charakter bei Personen mit Borderline-Persönlichkeitsstörung. Nervenarzt. 2005;76:839–848. doi: 10.1007/s00115-004-1810-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schmeck K, Schlüter-Müller S, Foelsch PA, Doering S. The role of identity in the DSM-5 classification of personality disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2013;7:27. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-7-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cloninger CR. A practical way to diagnosis personality disorder: a proposal. J Pers Disord. 2000;14:99–108. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2000.14.2.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Joyce PR, Mulder RT, Luty SE, McKenzie JM, Sullivan PF, Cloninger RC. Borderline personality disorder in major depression: symptomatology, temperament, character, differential drug response, and 6-month outcome. Compr Psychiat. 2003;44:35–43. doi: 10.1053/comp.2003.50001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kaess M, Resch F, Parzer P, von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna IA, Henze R, Brunner R. Temperamental patterns in female adolescents with Borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201:109–115. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31827f6480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cloninger CR, Praybeck T, Svrakic DM, Wetzel R. The Temperament and Character Inventory: A guide to its development and use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality. St Louis: Washington University; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hefti S, In-Albon T, Schmeck K, Schmid M. Temperaments-und Charaktereigenschaften und selbstverletzendes Verhalten bei Jugendlichen. Nervenheilkunde. 2013;32:45–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Joyce PR, Light KJ, Rowe SL, Cloninger CR, Kennedy MA. Self-mutilation and suicide attempts: relationships to bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, temperament and character. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44:250–257. doi: 10.3109/00048670903487159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ohmann S, Schuch B, König M, Blaas S, Fliri C, Popow C. Self-injurious behavior in adolescent girls. Psychopathology. 2008;41:226–235. doi: 10.1159/000125556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kim SJ, Lee SJ, Yune SK, Sung YH, Bae SC, Chung A, et al. The relationship between the biogenetic temperament and character and psychopathology in adolescents. Psychopathology. 2006;39:80–86. doi: 10.1159/000090597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.MacLaren VV, Best LA. Nonsuicidal self-injury, potentially addictive behaviors, and the five factor model in undergraduates. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49:521–525. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mullins-Sweatt SN, Lengel GJ, Grant DM. Non-suicidal self-injury: the contribution of general personality functioning. Personal Ment Health. 2013;7:56–68. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Glenn CR, Klonsky DE. A multimethod analysis of impulsivity in nonsuicidal self-injury. Personal Disord. 2010;1:67–75. doi: 10.1037/a0017427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nock MK, Prinstein MJ. Contextual features and behavioral functions of self-mutilation among adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:140–146. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Claes L, Muehlenkamp J. The relationship between the UPPS-P impulsivity dimensions and nonsuicidal self-injury characteristics in male and female high-school students. Psychiatry J. 2013 doi: 10.1155/2013/654847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Janis IB, Nock MK. Are self-injurers impulsive? Results from two behavioral laboratory studies. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169:261–267. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Evans J, Platts H, Liebenau A. Impulsiveness and deliberate self-harm: a comparison of “first-timers” and “repeaters”. Acta Psychiat Scand. 1996;93:378–380. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10663.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:965–982. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McCloskey MS, Look AE, Chen EY, Pajoumand G, Berman ME. Nonsuicidal self-injury: relationship to behavioral and self-rating measures of impulsivity and self-aggression. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2012;42:197–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00082.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J. Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Heidelberg: Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schneider S, Margraf J. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen. 4. Heidelberg: Springer; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Neuschwander M, In-Albon T, Adornetto C, Roth B, Schneider S. Interrater-Reliabilität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS) Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2013;41:319–334. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917//a000247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen HUSKIDII. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Salbach-Andrae H, Bürger A, Klinkowski N, Lenz K, Pfeiffer E, Fydrich T, et al. Diagnostik von Persönlichkeitsstörungen im Jugendalter nach SKID-II. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2008;36:117–125. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917.36.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Sender I, Gratwohl T, Stieglitz RD. Entwicklung der borderline-symptom-liste. PsychotherPsych Med. 2001;51:201–211. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-13281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Chapman AL, Kühler T, Stieglitz RD. Psychometric properties of the borderline symptom list (BSL) Psychopathology. 2007;40:126–132. doi: 10.1159/000098493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Goth K, Schmeck K. Das Junior Temperament und Charakter Inventar (JTCI) Manual. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Barratt ES. Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 1959;9:191–198. doi: 10.2466/pms.1959.9.3.191. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hartmann AS, Rief W, Hilbert A. Psychometric properties of the German version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, version 11 (BIS-11) for adolescents. Percept Mot Skills. 2011;112:353–368. doi: 10.2466/08.09.10.PMS.112.2.353-368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Döpfner M, Melchers P, Fegert J, Lehmkuhl G, Lehmkuhl U, Schmeck K, et al. Deutschsprachige Konsensus-Versionen der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 4–18), der Teacher Report Form (TRF) und der Youth Self Report Form (YSR) Kindh Entwickl. 1994;3:54–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C. Beck Depressions-Inventar II (BDI-II) Frankfurt am Main: Harcourt Test Services; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:927–934. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Trommer BL, Hoeppner JB, Lorber R, Armstrong KJ. The Go-No-Go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol. 1988;24:610–614. doi: 10.1002/ana.410240504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schulz KP, Fan J, Magidina O, Marks DJ, Hahn B, Halperin JM. Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition?: convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ladouceur CD, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Ryan ND, Casey BJ. Processing emotional facial expressions influences performance on a Go/NoGo task in pediatric anxiety and depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:1107–1115. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01640.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Pacheco-Unguetti AP, Acosta A, Lupiáñez J, Román N, Derakshan N. Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. Q J Exp Psychol. 2012;65:646–660. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.637114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brown SA. Personality and non-suicidal deliberate self-harm: trait differences among a non-clinical population. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169:28–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Adrian M, Zeman J, Erdley C, Lisa L, Sim L. Emotional dysregulation and interpersonal difficulties as risk factors for nonsuicidal self-injury in adolescent girls. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2011;39:389–400. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9465-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Tschan T, Schmid M, In-Albon T. Parenting behavior in families of female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury in comparison to a clinical and a nonclinical control group. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s13034-015-0051-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Cyders MA, Smith GT. Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. Psychol Bull. 2008;134:807. doi: 10.1037/a0013341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:53. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mehlum L, Tørmoen AJ, Ramberg M, Haga E, Diep LM, Laberg S, et al. Dialectical behavior therapy for adolescents with repeated suicidal and self-harming behavior: a randomized trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:1082–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Bradford Reich D, Silk KR. Prediction of the 10-year course of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:827–832. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES