Abstract
The relationship between developmental exposure to adversity and affective disorders is reviewed. Adversity discussed herein includes physical and sexual abuse, neglect, or loss of a caregiver in humans. While these stressors can occur at any point during development, the unique temporal relationship to specific depressive symptoms was the focus of discussion. Further influences of stress exposure during sensitive periods can vary by gender and duration of abuse as well. Data from animal studies are presented to provide greater translational and causal understanding of how sensitive periods, different types of psychosocial stressors, and sex interact to produce depressive-like behaviors. Findings from maternal separation, isolation rearing, chronic variable stress, and peer–peer rearing paradigms clarify interpretation about how various depressive behaviors are influenced by age of exposure. Depressive behaviors are broken down into the following categories: mood and affect, anhedonia, energy, working memory, sleep–wake, appetite changes, suicide, and general malaise. Cross-species evidence from humans, nonhuman primates, rats, and mice within each of these categories is discussed. In conclusion, sensitive periods for affective-related behaviors (anxiety, mood, and controllability) occur earlier in life, while other aspects of depression are associated with adversity later during adolescence.
Approximately 33% of the population is exposed to adversity during the course of development in the form of physical/ sexual abuse, neglect, loss of a parent or caregiver, or a natural disaster. Bullying is another form of adversity to which approximately 25% of children under the age of 11 years are exposed. Depression occurs in nearly 67% of the early life maltreatment population (Andersen & Teicher, 2008; Andersen et al., 2008; Widom, DuMont, & Czaja, 2007), compared to a lifetime prevalence rate in the general population of approximately 20% (Kessler et al., 1994). Depression emerges earlier than normal in children with a maltreatment history (Teicher, Samson, Polcari, & Andersen, 2009; Widom et al., 2007). The Treatment of SSRI-Resistant Depression in Adolescents Study has shown that cases of depression that present with a severe baseline of emotional dysregulation, a sense of hopelessness, elevated anxiety, and high levels of stress due to family conflict are less likely to achieve remission (Brent, 2009). The majority of individuals with a history of maltreatment or childhood anxiety disorder fall into this group.
The causal relationship between a history of maltreatment and subsequent depression is difficult to prove in human studies. Prospective studies (e.g., Widom et al., 2007) suggest that individuals with a developmental exposure to adversity have a higher risk of depression. Similarly, co-twin studies where women were exposed to varying degrees of childhood sexual abuse also show increased vulnerability to depression (Kendler et al., 2000; Nelson et al., 2002). Greater understanding of the relationship between maltreatment and depressive outcomes is likely to have an impact on improved treatment approaches, especially given the limited positive outcome from current pharmacological approaches.
This review paper will examine what is known about the relationship between depression and childhood exposure to maltreatment from a translational viewpoint. Various paradigms of early life stress are presented to address the maturational timing issue, followed by a breakdown of findings reported in the literature of specific symptoms of depression. This review does not include any extensive discussion of the role of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) that certainly influences depression; the reader is referred to (Eiland & Romeo, 2013) for such a discussion. Finally, ways to improve existing preventative interventions in the treatment of depression in this population are discussed.
Basic Definition and Across-Species Comparisons
The potential for any translational study to have an impact lies in its ability to facilitate valid, reliable, and direct comparisons across species. Behavioral, biochemical, or anatomical changes can provide points of shared reference. This framework is one through which cross-species comparisons are made in this paper with the goal of identifying translational points of commonality. Relationships among development, adversity, and depression will be examined with a particular focus on assessment across human, nonhuman primate, and rodents.
Utility of animal models
Animal model often refers to the use of animals to demonstrate a behavioral or biochemical phenotype that is found in humans. Given that animals will never sufficiently model the human condition and its complexities (Andersen & Thompson, 2011), the more appropriate terminology that should be used is endophenotype (e.g., Castellanos & Tannock, 2002). Endophenotypes are elemental components of a given disorder or condition that allows direct comparisons to be made across species if they have the same effect.
From a slightly different perspective, the Research Domain Criteria Project framework provides “constructs” and “domains” that are more amenable to translation. A construct is the basic unit of analysis, which includes motivation, cognition, and social behavior; the NIMH lists five domain systems: negative or positive valence, cognition, social processing, and arousal/regulatory (www.nimh.nih.gov). The next step taken in this categorization accounts for the level of analysis. Recognizing that analyses occur from behavior to genes and span from circuits to physiology, all the information necessary to produce a complete picture can occur neither within clinical studies nor in animal studies. The interdependence among different species is needed to fully understand the domains.
Another point about the use of animals needs to be addressed: evolutionary adaptation. Behaviors evolve over a very long time (tens of thousands of years) to increase the survival of the species but not necessarily the individual; other behaviors are passed across generations (Maestripieri, 2005). As environmental demands can shift relatively quickly, the species may be slow to adapt or display adaptive behaviors that do not seem to make sense in the current environment, but may allow the animal to survive a hostile world (Teicher, 2002); this model is known as adaptive calibration (McEwen, 2000). For example, corticosteroids may mediate the maturation of adult defensive systems, such that low levels of stress (e.g., novelty) will down-regulate reactivity to external stressors whereas high levels increase reactivity (Macri, Zoratto, & Laviola, 2011). Equally worth considering is at what point behavior is “abnormal” (reviewed in (Haller & Kruk, 2006). As development proceeds, adolescence is characterized by typical, yet more extreme, changes in behavior (e.g., risk taking and moodiness) and adolescence is also the period for the emergence of psychopathology (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002). Epigenetics provides a mechanism to pass environmental impact across the generations (Champagne, 2013).
A final consideration is how manipulations in animals parallel findings in humans and what that difference can tell us. While we aim to explain the human condition, animals may not experience the same psychosocial pressures or at least we do not know enough to model them. In other words, data from both sides can increase the understanding of the other.
Age-related differences in development in general
The brain continues to mature into the third decade of life. In general, synapses are formed and then competitively pruned back to optimize function for the demands of the environment (Brenhouse & Andersen, 2011). This process occurs differentially across brain regions. More fundamental brain regions undergo this process earlier, whereas the more complex brain regions, such as the cortex, prune later in adolescence and young adulthood. Different neurotransmitters play different roles during maturation, with GABA and glutamate acting as principal players (discussed below). We have previously reviewed this subject in typically developing species (Brenhouse & Andersen, 2011).
Sensitive periods of vulnerability
Sensitive periods are points in maturation when environmental input has a maximal impact on development. Sensitive periods differ from critical periods. Critical periods are well described in sensory systems (Hubel & Wiesel, 1970, 1998; Hubel, Wiesel, & LeVay, 1977), where all or none stimulation is needed for proper development. As a classic example, visual input programs the development of the ocular dominance columns such as those studied in kittens (Hubel et al., 1977; Kuppermann & Kasamatsu, 1984). Sensitive periods occur after critical periods close and fine-tune the system to match the needs of the environment (e.g., neuronal imprinting). This process can be further broken down into an experience-dependent process, which is a more individualized process, or an experience-expectant process, where the brain is primed to certain aspects of environmental information (Greenough, Black, & Wallace, 1987). Inappropriate stimulation (too much, too little, or none at all) can shift the development of a phenotype that differs from typicality.
Exposure to adverse environments during sensitive periods of development will produce neuroanatomical and behavioral consequences that may be predictable if we understood all of the factors (Andersen & Teicher, 2008; Cicchetti & Toth, 1995). Studies on the relationship among abuse, depression, and maturation support the hypothesis that clinical outcome depends on timing of the abusive episode. If an abusive episode occurs before 12 years of age, depression is a likely outcome; if the episode occurs after 12 years of age, posttraumatic stress disorder is more likely (Kaplow & Widom, 2007; Schoedl et al., 2010). A more recent study examined smaller sensitive periods to identify the strongest relationships between child abuse and depression (Dunn, McLaughlin, Slopen, Rosand, & Smoller, 2013). Data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health demonstrate a significant relationship between physical abuse that occurred between 3 and 5 years of age and depression; suicidal ideation was higher in individuals sexually abused during preschool compared with adolescence. The data on risk for depression is consistent with a first wave of depressive risk that we observed in individuals with an early abuse history (Andersen et al., 2008; Andersen & Teicher, 2008). Cortisol dysregulation is also stronger when physical or sexual abuse with depression or internalizing symptoms occurs earlier than when neglect or abuse plus depression occur together during adolescence (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Gunnar, & Toth, 2010).
Neuroanatomical studies show that abuse that occurs prior to puberty has more selective effects on the hippocampus, whereas abuse after puberty appears more selective for the prefrontal cortex in humans (Andersen & Teicher, 2008). Studies on sensitive periods in typically developing children suggest that the optimal window for environmental impact on hippocampal development is before 8 years of age (Rao et al., 2010). In bonnet monkeys that were peer raised, significant reductions in the corpus callosum and hippocampus were observed during late adolescence (Jackowski et al., 2011); these findings are consistent with those observed in humans (Andersen et al., 2008; Teicher et al., 2004). Animal research shows that different windows of early stress exposure produce unique behavioral (Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vacarro, & Andersen, 2013; Lehmann, Pryce, Bettschen, & Feldon, 1999) and neuroanatomical effects (Leussis & Andersen, 2008; Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson, & Andersen, 2012). These paradigms will be discussed in more detail below under the animal model section.
The nature of the abuse
The type of abuse (e.g., sexual, physical, or neglect) or nature of the early trauma can influence the brain region affected as well as the behavioral outcome (Tomoda et al., 2009, 2011; van Veen et al., 2013). The nature of depressive symptoms differs (described in more detail below), although exposure to different types of stressors may provide some basis for their etiological underpinnings. Depression can present as either more anxious or anhedonic in children or adolescents (Mitchell, McCauley, Burke, & Moss, 1988). Adolescents with an anxious depression report more guilt, agitation, hypersomnia, and weight changes (gain or loss; Mitchell et al., 1988). Depression with conduct disorder also appears to be another subtype (Mitchell et al., 1988). A recent study examined specific outcomes in 2,615 individuals enrolled in the Netherlands Study for Depression and Anxiety (van Veen et al., 2013). The authors found that emotional neglect had the greatest impact, affecting arousal and anhedonia dimensions. Physical and sexual abuse, as well as psychological abuse, was associated with a more anxious arousal; the former two were also associated with greater generalized arousal. Earlier studies show that physical abuse is associated with higher lifetime rates of major depression in women, but not men (MacMillan et al., 2001). Higher rates of psychopathology, including drug use, were higher in women following childhood sexual abuse than in men (MacMillan et al., 2001).
Nature of depressive symptoms
Depression is multifaceted, resulting from genetics, different environmental variables (including the timing of exposure to environmental factors), or both. A more extensive review of risk factors for depression can be found in Beardslee, Gladstone, and O’Connor (2012). Different treatment modalities support this notion, and depressive symptoms can include the following categories:
mood changes: sad, anxious, or “empty”; hopelessness, guilt, worthlessness, or helplessness; irritability; restlessness
anhedonia: a loss of interest in activities or things once pleasurable, including sex
changes in energy and feeling fatigued
working-memory problems and difficulty concentrating or making decisions
sleep–wake changes: insomnia, early-morning wakefulness, or excessive sleeping
changes in appetite
thoughts of suicide or suicide attempts 8. malaise: aches or pains, headaches, cramps, or digestive problems
Sex differences in depression
Sex differences in depression emerge during adolescence (Cyranowski, Frank, Young, & Shear, 2000) and are maintained into adulthood. Depressive symptoms are often experienced differently between men and women. Men are more likely to report symptoms 2, 3, and 5 (above), whereas women usually endorse 1, 3, and 6 (Cochran, 2000; Pollack, 1998). The initiating events that can underlie depressive symptoms may also be sexually dimorphic. Depression in female teens is more strongly associated with maternal depressive behavior, whereas males are more strongly affected by changes in supportive early care (Duggal, Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2001).
Part of these changes may also be related to different sensitive periods in brain development (for a review, see Brenhouse & Andersen, 2011). In general, males show greater overproduction and pruning in most brain regions compared to females (Giedd, Castellanos, Rajapakse, Vaituzis, & Rapoport, 1997). The process of overproduction and elimination may render males more vulnerable to environmental impacts (Andersen & Teicher, 2009). Greater decreases inhippocampus and corpus callosum size are observed in males with a history of maltreatment than in females (DeBellis & Keshavan, 2003; Teicher et al., 2004). Diurnal changes in cortisol levels also are higher in boys than in girls with a history of maltreatment (Doom, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Dackis, 2013).
Animal studies, however, have either not examined females in regard to stress and depression (Kaufman, Plotsky, Nemeroff, & Charney, 2000; Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein, & Daniels, 2008) or failed to find sex differences (discussed below). In genetic models bred to be more sensitive to depressive-like behavior, males, but not females, show greater immobility during adolescence (Mehta, Wang, & Redei, 2013). Females are more resistant to stressful events at the level of controllability that is related to escape behavior (Dalla, Edgecomb, Whetstone, & Shors, 2007; Leussis & Andersen, 2008; Leussis et al., 2012). Consistent with the human literature, sex differences only became evident when we examined different aspects of depressive-like behavior in rats. Specifically, females showed slightly higher baseline levels of helplessness that are not what one would expect based on human studies. Two possibilities exist. One hypothesis is that human males underreport their depressive symptoms. However, findings from large, community-based samples suggest that such underreporting is not the case (Wolk & Weissman, 1995) and that the reported 2:1 female to male sex difference in prevalence rate is found in the United States (Marcus et al., 2005) and around the globe (Wolk & Weissman, 1995). A second possibility is that the nature of what is measured in animals does not accurately reflect the human condition. In other words, the standard measures of depressive-like behavior in animals, the forced swim test (Detke, Rickels, & Lucki, 1995), or the active avoidance paradigm (Olton, Johnson, & Howard, 1974), miss an important aspect of depression found in females.
While depressive-like behavior in females may be elusive in animal studies, estrogen and stress are known to interact and influence affective behaviors (Dalla, Pitychoutis, Kokras, & Papadopoulou-Daifoti, 2011). In both animals and humans, low levels of estrogen are associated with greater depressive symptoms (Hajszan et al., 2010; Young & Korszun, 2010). Low or falling levels of estrogen typically occur during premenstrual dysphoria disorder and menopause, which are both associated with depressive symptoms. As estrogen modulates corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), a stress hormone, and serotonin (Bangasser & Valentino, 2012), stress-induced changes in estrogen are likely to modulate depressive behaviors. Developmentally, exposure to social stress during the juvenile period delays the onset of puberty in female non-human primates (Wilson et al., 2013). As a result, estrogen levels do not rise during the normal developmental stage. Because estrogen increases dendritic spine density (Woolley, Gould, Frankfurt, & McEwen, 1990) that is instrumental for the overproduction of synapses during adolescence, changes in estrogen in response to stress can cause lifelong susceptibility to depressive behaviors that emerge during adolescence.
Animal Models of Early Adversity: Sensitive Periods and Socially Relevant Stress
A number of animal models have been developed to facilitate understanding changes in brain development. Generally, these paradigms are based on the social stage of the animal. Early developmental manipulations involve maternal care, whereas child to adolescent stage manipulations typically involve separation from peers. The latter manipulations can be escalated further by social defeat paradigms. By adulthood, exposure to stressful manipulations will temporarily perturb a system, but do not seem to permanently alter the course of its development. For example, both early life and adult stress exposure decrease dendritic spines that last into adulthood, but adult stress decreases the spines only temporarily and they regenerate within a 3-week time point. In contrast, growing up in a complex environment increases spine density (Volkmar & Greenough, 1972).
Maternal separation paradigms
Maternal care has a significant effect on subsequent development. Our initial interest in the neurobiological consequences of childhood maltreatment emerged from preclinical studies on the effects of early experience (e.g., Brunelli, Shindledecker, & Hofer, 1989; Denenberg, Garbanati, Sherman, Yutzey, & Kaplan, 1978; Harlow & Harlow, 1965; Levine, Johnson, & Gonzalez, 1985). For rodent species, which are typically born relatively immature like humans, reliance on the dam for care is necessary for survival. Initially proposed as the “maternal mediation hypothesis” (Smotherman, Wiener, Mendoza, & Levine, 1977), the dam will adjust her care to match the environment in a way that maximizes survival. These experimental paradigms include manipulating environmental factors such as nest building material, foraging for food, and making resource availability unpredictable (primates; Rosenblum & Paully, 1984). Once these basic needs are met, the dam then focuses her time on the direct care of the offspring, including licking and grooming. If circumstances are favorable, she will often assume the more effective nursing posture of arched-back nursing (Anisman, Zaharia, Meaney, & Merali, 1998; Liu et al., 1997). Pups that are raised by dams that demonstrate high licking and grooming behavior are more resilient to stress: cortisol and ACTH levels are reduced at baseline and in response to stress challenge later in life (Liu et al., 1997).
Paradigms that reduce arched-back nursing involve disruption of maternal care by separating the pups from the dam for various lengths of time. The original model by Levine (1967) used a 24-hr separation period, while the most common duration is 3–4 hr a day between Postnatal (P) Days 2–4 (P2–P14) or P2–P20 (Leussis et al., 2012; Mourlon et al., 2010; Plotsky & Meaney, 1993; Reus et al., 2011). Variations exist within the maternal separation (MS) literature as well. We will use the designations of MS(24h) for separation for 24 hr, MS(3) for 3 hr a day between P2 and P14, or MS(4) for 4 hr a day between P2 and P20. Exceptions to the ages and/or times of separation for the different studies will be noted. In general, the MS(3) and MS(4) paradigms do not appear to differ significantly because they both include separation periods during the first week of life. The other factor that can differ among MS paradigms is whether the litter is separated as a unit from the dam or individual pups are separated from the dam and each other. While this distinction is not always clear, our unpublished observations suggest that the latter will have greater effects on the offspring.
The effectiveness of MS to produce changes in depression, fear, and anxiety depends on the timing of the paradigm as well as the species of animal. MS paradigms that include the first week but not the second week of life seem to produce differential effects. For example MS between P2 and P13 produces stronger fear responses to innate predator odors than in animals exposed to MS between P11 and P13 (Litvin et al., 2010). Similarly, exposure to saline injections at P9–P14 produces greater helplessness than injections at P2–P9 (Freund et al., 2013). The effects of MS can vary by strain of mice (Kundakovic, Lim, Gudsnuk, & Champagne, 2013) or rat (Sterley, Howells, & Russell, 2011). For example, the spontaneously hypertensive rat is resistant to anxiogenic effects of MS, but demonstrates hyperactivity that has also been reported in abuse cases (Sterley et al., 2011). More examples will be given within different sections below.
Whether all of the effects on the offspring are “mediated” by dam as originally proposed is certainly debatable. A maternal “modulation” hypothesis has been put forth that states mom can play gatekeeper in her pup’s exposure to novelty and stressors (Tang, Reeb-Sutherland, Romeo, & McEwen, 2013). This modulation is consistent with the findings of Macri, Mason, and Wurbel (2004), who describe dissociation among HPA responses, early life experiences, and fear responses.
Changes in rearing conditions
A number of paradigms can change rearing conditions, including changes in bedding (Coccurello et al., 2014), communal nursing paradigm in rats nesting (Macri, Laviola, Leussis, & Andersen, 2009) or in mice (Branchi & Alleva, 2006), or reducing nest bedding (Raineki, Cortes, Belnoue, & Sullivan, 2012). Collectively, these paradigms are designed to disrupt maternal behavior, or in the case of mice, elevate maternal behavior.
Social isolation paradigms
While removing a pup from the dam when very young is considered stressful, removal of pups from the dam when they are entering adolescence may not be as effective. Like humans, separation from peers is highly stressful. A number of social isolation paradigms are found in the literature, with the most common one being the isolation rearing paradigm. Pups are weaned from the dam and isolation housed typically from 28 to 55 days (Sahakian & Robbins, 1977; Warren & Ivinskis, 1973). During young adulthood, these subjects demonstrate a number of changes in behavior and HPA axis activity.
Other paradigms, including social defeat and chronic variable stress
The social defeat paradigm involves exposure of a subordinate rat to a dominant rat, whose dominance is established by previous bouts or age. A recent study subjected juvenile male and female rats to a dominant male adult for 10 min/ day for 10 days (Weathington, Arnold, & Cooke, 2012).
The chronic variable stress paradigm includes twice-daily exposures to a number of different stressors, including crowding, social isolation, restraint, shaker stress, swimming, change in temperature, and air quality (Jankord et al., 2011). While a number of these stressors can be used individually to produce chronic stress, the variability in their presentation typically produces different results (e.g., decreased HPA reactivity versus no change or elevation; Cruz, Marin, Leao, & Planeta, 2012). This paradigm, like the MS paradigm, has variations in duration of exposure and the number of stressors used that are likely to make cross-study comparisons difficult.
Peer–peer rearing
Initially used in the classic study of Harlow and Harlow (1965), this model has been primarily used in studies of non-human primates (Bowden & McKinney, 1972; Suomi, Harlow, & Domek, 1970). These studies have been instrumental in illustrating the importance of age, rearing conditions, and rehousing during the stressor (Gilmer & McKinney, 2003).
Important Variables That Influence the Observed Results
Sex of the subject
A number of studies show that the effects of early exposure to adversity are sex dependent. Specific examples within each sensitive period of early adversity exposure are described within the next section.
Duration of the stressor
Clinical work shows that the duration of the stressor is often associated with worse outcomes (Beitchman et al., 1992). However, disentangling the age of onset of the stressor from its duration is almost impossible, except in adult systems. Exposure to adversity early in development has a great impact not only on the foundational maturation of a number of systems, including basic behavioral processes, but also on the underlying neural structures. Alterations in behavior affecting basic self-regulatory processes that occur during the first few years of life are likely to lead to other issues later in life (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002). Similarly, exposure to early adversity is less evident during the building-up phase of hippocampal development, but without basic synaptic structure present before pruning, some minimal amount of foundation is lost and becomes evident during adolescence when excess synapses are pruned away (Andersen & Teicher, 2004).
Timing of assessment postadversity
Does the clock start at the age of initiation or the age of termination? Increasing cross-talk among disciplines (e.g., developmental psychologists, clinical outreach workers, and prevention scientists) will facilitate early identification of psychopathology, if it is going to manifest, earlier. Depression often appears as children enter adolescence. In our studies (Andersen, Tordera, Lasheras, Del Rio, & Ramirez, 2008; Teicher et al., 2009) and others (Widom et al., 2007), depression and substance-use disorder are among the most likely outcomes (Kaplow & Widom, 2007). The delay in the diagnosis for depression in our sample was ~9.2 years, unless the abuse occurred during adolescence. Childhood maltreatment accelerates the onset of depression relative to the typical age of onset in nonmaltreated individuals. Earlier studies that examined the expression of depressive symptoms within 8 weeks of an abusive episode demonstrated that adolescent maltreatment is associated with the appearance of greater depressive symptoms (reviewed by Feiring, Taska, & Lewis, 1999). However, this conclusion is likely incorrect given the extensivework in animals and humans reviewed in this paper that suggests interactive effects between maltreatment and maturation. In other words, children with a maltreatment history are strongly at risk for the development of depression too.
These data suggest that the brain must reach some maturational stage that permits the expression of the underlying pathology. A number of possibilities may explain this delay. First, the physical change in synapses must reach a certain level of loss in order for the deficit to be observed. Parkinson disease, for example, requires >90% loss of dopamine neurons before the patient experiences symptoms. Second, maturational processes need to occur to unmask the deficit. These processes may involve either the failure to overproduce synapses or the pruning of excess synapses, such as what we have observed in MS(4) subjects as they enter adulthood (e.g., (Andersen & Teicher, 2004); or age-related changes in connectivity between brain regions. A number of anxiety-driven responses that are associated with depression involve reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (Cressman et al., 2010; Cunningham, Bhattacharyya, & Benes, 2002). In rats, amygdalar–cortical projections increase in age up until P60 (late adolescence; Cunningham et al., 2002). While they appear to stabilize with age, projections from the prefrontal cortex to the basolateral amygdala prune into adulthood (Cressman et al., 2010). Connections between the prefrontal cortex and the accumbens may have similar developmental effects on the appearance of anhedonia (Brenhouse, Sonntag, & Andersen, 2008).
Increases in EEG coherence are evident in limbic regions of individuals with a history of maltreatment, including both child and adult trauma in humans (Nelson et al., 2009). Consistent with a number of the depressive features listed below, resting-state functional connectivity in areas related to episodic memory and retrieval is impaired (van der Werff et al., 2013b). In contrast, areas related to declarative memory and stronger emotional processing show stronger resting-state connectivity between the dorsal anterior cingulate and regions containing the lingual and orbital fusiform gyri in individuals who experienced maltreatment but were resilient (van der Werff et al., 2013a).
Only recently has clinical research revealed that cortisol levels or its regulation by ACTH in response to CRF challenge transition with either further maturation or time since the last episode of abuse. Children exposed to early adversity demonstrate elevated levels of cortisol initially during or immediately following the abusive episode (Bevans, Cerbone, & Overstreet, 2008; Carrion & Wong, 2012; DeBellis et al., 2005). Later, these elevations give way to blunted responses by adulthood or even shortly after the abuse ends (reviewed in Struber, Struber, & Roth, 2013). This transition makes sense, given that elevated cortisol will facilitate fight or flight. Over time, the allostatic load becomes too great for the system to bear and, eventually, cortisol regulation is blunted.
Alternatively, some behaviors may require full maturation to occur before manifesting. Animals that underwent MS for 3 hr during postnatal development showed significantly more time spent in the open arm of an elevated plus maze when assessed at P37, but less time when assessed at P54 (Feng et al., 2013).
Part of the delay in the appearance of behavioral changes is the result of delayed maturation. For example, a loss in gray matter volume in the hippocampus has been well documented in adults with a history of childhood maltreatment (Bremner et al., 1997; DeBellis et al., 1999) and in animals that were exposed to early life stress (Andersen & Teicher, 2008; Huot, Plotsky, Lenox, & McNamara, 2002). When children were examined for volume changes in the hippocampus, none were observed (DeBellis et al., 1999). Longitudinal studies now show that the loss of hippocampal volume is progressive (Tupler & DeBellis, 2006). These delayed effects are not unique to humans. Rhesus monkeys that were peer raised did not show changes in the hippocampus or corpus callosum when assessed at 24–30 months (~6–8 years human), although enlargements in the cerebellar vermis and cortical regions were found (Spinelli et al., 2009). Hippocampal and corpus callosum decreases were documented in peer-reared bonnet monkeys that were assessed at an older age (Jackowski et al., 2011). Similarly, no difference in hippocampal synaptophysin (a measure of synapses) was evident until after the onset of puberty in rats exposed to MS(4) (Andersen & Teicher, 2004). These data imply that maltreatment interacts with maturation for the full effect to manifest.
Clinical and Clinically Relevant Findings
Depression cross-species
Mood changes: Sad, anxious, or “empty”; hopelessness, guilt, worthlessness, or helplessness; irritability; restlessness
Depression often emerges during young adolescence following exposure to early life adversity (Teicher et al., 2009; Widom et al., 2007), with few depressive symptoms observed in childhood. Feelings of worthlessness and guilt have been reported in females with a maltreatment history (Schuck & Widom, 2001). Receiving welfare as a child and living with alcoholic parents increase the strength of the relationship between abuse and worthlessness. Victims of bullying report greater internalizing behaviors (miserable, fearful, worried, solitary, apathetic, and not liked), whereas the bullies rate higher externalizing scores (lies, irritability, steals, fights, not liked, disobedient, and destructive) based on the Rutter scale (Kumpulainen et al., 1998).
Anxiety and fear
Nonhuman primates that were reared by peers and not their mother demonstrated increased fear-potentiated startle as juveniles (Nelson et al., 2009). The effects of maternal separation early in life, for as little as one or two 6-day separations, persist in primates’ approach to novelty in adulthood (Hinde, Spencer-Booth, & Bruce, 1966).
Changes in fear and anxiety are evident in the animal models, but inconsistently (discussed in Stevenson, Meredith, Spicer, Mason, & Marsden, 2009). One of the plausible explanations for these inconsistencies points to the amount of learning the assessment task requires. MS subjects in general show some degree of impaired learning of fear-related tasks (Aisa et al., 2008). Regardless, a number of studies show decreased time in the open arm of the elevated plus maze, which is an index of anxiety-like behavior. MS(3) decreases open arm time in adulthood (Lee et al., 2001) in both males and females, although the effects are greater in males (Wigger & Neumann, 1999). Communal nursing in both rats (Macri et al., 2009) and mice (Branchi & Alleva, 2006) also increase anxiety-like behavior. Our study in rats demonstrated reduced maternal care the first few days of life, which may be sufficient to produce lasting changes in affect (Macri et al., 2009). These findings are consistent with other paradigms that manipulate maternal cues (Moriceau, Wilson, Levine, & Sullivan, 2006).
Isolation rearing increased anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze that was not reversed upon resocialization (Wright, Upton, & Marsden, 1991). Similarly, isolation rearing increased fear reactivityand social anxiety in earlyadulthood in male rats (Lukkes, Vuong, Scholl, Oliver, & Forster, 2009; Lukkes, Watt, Lowry, & Forster, 2009). Isolation rearing enhances anxiety-like behavior in females (Da Silva, Ferreira, Carobrez Ade, & Morato, 1996). Anxiety-like behavioralso depended on species and testing conditions; isolation rearing increased anxiety more in Fawn Hooded rats than in Wistar rats under low light conditions, but Wistars were less anxious when the lights were brighter (Hall, Huang, Fong, Pert, & Linnoila, 1998). In a different test of anxiety, defensive marble burying was reduced in both males and females that were isolated during adolescence (Arakawa, 2007).
Chronic variable stress during adolescence did not affect anxiety-like behavior (plus maze) in either males or females during adolescence (Taylor, Taylor, & Koenig, 2013). However, this same paradigm reduced freezing in females, but had no effect in males (Taylor et al., 2013).
Depressive-like behavior
Nonhuman primates that were isolated during development show depressive-like behaviors (Laudenslager, Held, Boccia, Reite, & Cohen, 1990; Suomi, Delizio, & Harlow, 1976). Depressive-like behaviors include postural changes and locomotor retardation (others are specifically discussed below). These behaviors are reversible if the monkeys are rehoused with their peers (Suomi et al., 1976).
The forced swim test (FST) has been used to test antidepressant efficacy, and by proxy, depressive-like behavior (Detke et al., 1995). Increased depressive-like symptoms occur in the MS model (Matthews, Wilkinson, & Robbins, 1996). MS(3) with the whole litter separated as a unit increased the time spent immobile in adulthood in Wistar (Aisa et al., 2008), Fischer (Ruedi-Bettschen et al., 2006), and Sprague-Dawley rats (Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein, & Daniels, 2008); Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) of rats also show greater depressive-like behavior that depends on the quality of maternal care (Friedman, Berman, & Over-street, 2006). If FSL rats were cross-fostered to the more nurturing Flinders Resistant strain, the depressive effects were less than if raised by FSL dams. Similarly, increased maternal care following brief handling made males more resistant to immobility later in life (Papaioannou, Gerozissis, Prokopiou, Bolaris, & Stylianopoulou, 2002).
The ability of the medial prefrontal cortex to exert behavioral control over a stressor plays a critical role in protecting the brain from depression (Maier & Watkins, 2005; Robbins, 2005). Loss of such control results in hopelessness, which is strongly associated with depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (Maier & Watkins, 2005). MS(4) increases depressive-like symptoms in the triadic model of learned helplessness (Freund et al., 2013; Leussis et al., 2012). Specifically, males show a greater loss of controllability over their perceived ability to escape their situation (e.g., footshock); in contrast, females show greater deficits in their motivation to escape footshock upon first exposure (Leussis et al., 2012). Sensitive periods that are sex sensitive exist before weaning and are expressed during adolescence. We found that males showed more helplessness in response to saline injections at P9–P14, than at P2–P9; conversely, females showed increased resiliency when given a daily saline injection between P2 and P9, but only a modest effect at P9–P14 (Freund et al., 2013). The effects of MS on the FST were minimal in Long–Evans rats (Mourlon et al., 2010).
When rats were housed in isolation between 30 and 35 days of age and then tested 2 days later in adolescence, the male rats demonstrated learned helplessness and the females spent more time in the closed arm of the elevated plus maze (Leussis & Andersen, 2008). Isolation rearing for longer periods (weaning to late adolescence) in rats increased immobility in the FST, although these subjects also received saline injections during preweanling stages (Kuramochi & Nakamura, 2009). The same results were observed in noninjected social isolates (Brenes, Padilla, & Fornaguera, 2009), although no enduring effect on immobility has been reported (Hong et al., 2012). These effects were lower in animals raised in an enriched environment. In contrast, no effect on swimming behavior was observed in Wistar or Fawn-Hooded rats that were isolated and tested in adulthood (Hall et al., 1998). Isolation-reared rats also show reduced exploration of novelty (File, 1978).
The use of social defeat paradigms to examine depressive-like behavior is a relatively young field. Female adolescent rats exposed to 7 days of consecutive social defeat demonstrated transient depressive behaviors in the FST (Ver Hoeve, Kelly, Luz, Ghanshani, & Bhatnagar, 2013). In contrast, exposure to a dominant male for 10 min/day for 10 days during the juvenile period produced lasting depressive-like effects in females, but not males, in adulthood (Weathington et al., 2012). Similar to the transient effects on FST behavior if social defeat occurred during adolescence, exposure to chronic variable stress during early or late adolescence failed to affect immobility in the FST; this stressor increased immobility in adult-exposed subjects only (Jankord et al., 2011).
Anhedonia: A loss of interest in activities or things once pleasurable, including sex
Only a few studies have examined anhedonia in adolescents or adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. One such study reported elevated anhedonia based on the Mood and Anxiety Symptoms Questionnaire in individuals when the abuse occurred before 14 years of age (Weathington et al., 2012).
Anhedonia in animals is measured with a variety of paradigms, including decreased consumption of sucrose solutions, decreased sexual activity, and a rightward shift in intracranial self-stimulation currents (ICSS) where more current is needed to activate reward systems (Kornetsky & Esposito, 1979). MS(3) decreased responding to novel environments (Matthews, Wilkinsin, et al., 1996). MS(3) rats also show reduced sucrose drinking compared with controls, and this reduction did not differ across males and females in both a positive and negative contrast challenge, where subjects were exposed to a low/high percentage of sucrose followed by a higher/lower one, respectively (Matthews, Hall, Wilkinson, & Robbins, 1996). Similarly, MS(3) Wistar rats also worked less for a sucrose reward in adulthood (Leventopoulos, Russig, Feldon, Pryce, & Opacka-Juffry, 2009). In a slight modification of the MS(3) paradigm, mice separated from the dam and placed on clean bedding between P1 and P14 reduced saccharin drinking in adulthood (Coccurello et al., 2014).
Sucrose drinking was increased in a positive contrast design in rats that were raised in social isolation from weaning until young adulthood (Hall, Humby, Wilkinson, & Robbins, 1997). In a separate study, these effects were not observed in males, but in female adult rats that were isolated (Hong et al., 2012). These effects may also depend on the strain of rat used. The Wistar Kyoto rat has been used in previous studies of depression (Overstreet, 2012). When this strain is exposed to social isolation between 27 and 49 days, females have reduced sucrose consumption compared to controls both immediately after the stressor in late adolescence or in adulthood; males did not show any change (Bourke & Neigh, 2011). Taken together, these data suggest that social stress manipulations on sucrose drinking depend on the age of manipulation as well as the strain of rat.
Measures of ICSS for anhedonia suggest that the MS effect is modest. Rats that underwent MS(3) between P1 and P14 failed to show an anhedonic-like shift in ICSS currents under baseline conditions (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2010; Matthews & Robbins, 2003), but required greater current following 7 days of repeated social defeat (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2010). These data suggest that MS produces a sub-threshold effect that requires additional stress for an underlying effect to emerge. In another study, MS(3) rats tested in adulthood demonstrated reduced ICSS response rates in females only with no change between MS(3) males and controls (Michaels, Easterling, & Holtzman, 2007). A subset of isolation-reared rats show equivocal changes to ICSS thresholds that are modulated by D2 receptors (Sundstrom, Hall, Stellar, & Waugh, 2002).
In general, appetitive behavior decreases following early life MS, whereas social isolations increase appetitive behavior (Harmer & Phillips, 1998). However, this relationship may also depend on when the behavior is measured. Rhesus monkeys that were peer raised, which produces stressful-like effects, demonstrated an increase insweet drinking as juveniles (Nelson et al., 2009); maternally deprived rhesus monkeys had reduced sweet drinking in adulthood (Paul, English, & Halaris, 2000).
Changes in energy and feeling fatigued
A history of childhood sexual abuse is predictive of levels of fatigue (Taylor & Jason, 2002). Abuse that occurred during childhood has a stronger association with levels of fatigue than when abuse occurred during adolescence. Childhood maltreatment, independent of type, increases risk for chronic fatigue syndrome three- to eightfold (Heim et al., 2006). These relationships are all further associated with elevated levels of depression and anxiety, and stronger if criteria for posttraumatic stress syndrome are met (Heim et al., 2009).
Working-memory problems and difficulty concentrating or making decisions
The ability to maintain negative instead of positive affective information when processing emotional material is associated with an increased risk of depression. In multiple studies of individuals with depression and a history of maltreatment, this negativity bias is a common finding (Goodman, Quas, & Ogle, 2010). Young women (mean age = 20 years) with a history of childhood maltreatment have difficulty maintaining positively valenced information relative to nonmaltreated controls (Cromheeke, Herpoel, & Mueller, 2013). In addition, longer duration of abuse is positively associated with memory impairment (Navalta, Polcairi, Webster, Boghossian, & Teicher, 2006). These results held in an adult population-based study, where greater memory impairment was associated with greater adverse childhood experiences (ACEs; Brown et al., 2007).
Working-memory impairment has been observed in both MS(3) and MS(4) subjects tested prepubertally or during adolescence, respectively (Brenhouse & Andersen, 2011; Frankola et al., 2010). MS rats showed more errors and greater latency on the win-shift paradigm during adolescence, but the effect was no longer apparent in adulthood (Brenhouse & Andersen, 2011). The effects on memory were also specific for males, because MS(3) females did not show impairment when tested prepubertally (Frankola et al., 2010). Isolation rearing impairs working and spatial memory in adult gerbils (Winterfeld, Teuchert-Noodt, & Dawirs, 1998) and in rats (Quan, Tian, Xu, Zhang, & Wang, 2010).
Relative to hooded rats raised in an enriched environment, rats raised in isolation after weaning have more errors in a radial arm maze (Juraska, Henderson, & Muller, 1984). No sex differences were evident. Similar findings were found in Wistar rats (Gorisch & Schwarting, 2006).
Sleep–wake changes: Insomnia, early-morning wakefulness, or excessive sleeping
Sleep difficulties are a common occurrence in children and adolescents with depression, such that insomnia is more prevalent than hypersomnia (Morielli, Ladan, Ducharme, & Brouillette, 1996). Posttraumatic stress disorder includes reexperiencing and nightmares as a factor in diagnosis. In adults with a history of child maltreatment, three fourths report significant sleep disturbances (Krakow et al., 2002). However, the relationship between the type of abuse and sleep disturbance may be surprising. Consistent with the notion of a maturational delay in symptoms following abuse, sleep disturbances have been reported to emerge years after the abuse (Trickett, Noll, Reiffman, & Putnam, 2001). In cases where the abuse was deemed “less serious,” the sleep disturbances were greater (Noll, Trickett, Susman, & Putnam, 2006). Such children may have been older, exposed to little violence, a shorter duration of abuse, or not closely related to the perpetrator. Other reports on sleep difficulties suggest that the type of abuse or perpetrator does not significantly matter (Rimsza, Berg, & Locke, 1988). A likely explanation is that children with “less serious abuse” may not have received sufficient treatment, possibly because they may have seemed asymptomatic (known as sleeper effects; Briere, 1992).
Early research has shown that basic sleep–wake behavior is modulated by maternal behavior and milk delivery (Hofer, 1975). MS(3) rats demonstrate greater REM sleep (~35% more than controls) and more time sleeping than controls (Kinkead, Montandon, Bairam, Lajeunesse, & Horner, 2009). These findings have been replicated (Mrdalj et al., 2013). No effect on sleep architecture was observed in Wistar rats that were exposed to prenatal stress (Dugovic, Maccari, Weibel, Turek, & Van Reeth, 1999). Social isolation later in life (P50–P60) had little effect on sleep architecture in rats, although living within an enriched environment improves sleep quality (Kiyono, Seo, & Shibagaki, 1981).
Changes in appetite
Clinical and preclinical studies show a relationship between the postnatal environment and the risk of developing obesity (Felitti, 1991; Vamosi, Heitmann, & Kyvik, 2010). The risk is higher in girls. Exposure to chronic stress may lead to “comfort” eating, where children and adolescents are likely to overeat. Elevations in cortisol can elevate insulin secretion, ultimately leading to its dysregulation; together, these factors further enhance appetite and/or weight gain (Pervanidou & Chrousos, 2011). Binge eating has been associated with greater weight gain in victims of childhood sexual abuse (Gustafson & Sarwer, 2004).
Changes in growth and eating behavior are programmed early in life (Kuhn, Pauk, & Schanberg, 1990; Plotsky & Meaney, 1993). Maternal behavior affects growth hormone, corticosterone, and other hormones involved in development in rats (Kuhn, Butler, & Schanberg, 1978) and in nonhuman primates (Kaufman et al., 2007). For example, leptin, a hormone involved in regulating energy intake and use (including thermogenesis), peaks at ~P10 in rats before declining (Blumberg, Deaver, & Kirby, 1999; Delahaye et al., 2008). Leptin can modify the effects of MS by reducing responsivity to external ACTH (Salzmann et al., 2004). Given that MS pups show reduced rates of growth before puberty (Freund et al., 2013), early life changes in leptin make MS subjects less vulnerable to gaining weight in adulthood when fed a high-fat diet (Paternain et al., 2012). However, male MS subjects fed a regular chow diet showed increased weight gain in adulthood and females did not (Matthews, Wilkinson, et al., 1996); this weight gain following a chow diet or one deficient in omega 3s, in which insulin resistance biomarkers are decreased (Bernardi et al., 2013; Delaunay et al., 1997; Lambillotte, Gilon, & Henquin, 1997; Solas et al., 2010).
Rats exposed to MS(3), and then subjected to social isolation, gained more weight than either manipulation alone (Ryu, Yoo, Kang, Lee, & Jahng, 2009). In contrast, rats exposed to a chronic variable stress paradigm during early or late adolescence or adulthood lost weight in general, as well as fat weight specifically (Jankord et al., 2011; Taylor et al., 2013); others have reported no weight loss (Cruz et al., 2012).
Thoughts of suicide, suicide attempts
Early ACEs significantly increase the risk of attempted suicide by two to five times, raising the rates from the general population prevalence of 1.1% to as high as 35% if seven or more ACEs are present (Dube et al., 2001). A recent longitudinal study in New Zealand that followed 900 individuals less than 30 years of age demonstrated a significant relationship between childhood sexual abuse and both suicidal ideation and attempts (Fergusson, McLeod, & Horwood, 2013). Of note, elevated suicidality is also evident in individuals with a history of physical abuse (Fuller-Thomson, Baker, & Brennenstuhl, 2012). Other studies have found similar results in teenagers (Klomek et al., 2013). The study of suicide, attempts, and ideation in animals at this stage is impossible.
Malaise: Aches or pains, headaches, cramps, or digestive problems
Digestion problems, including nausea, stomach-aches, and vomiting, have been associated with early abuse in boys and girls (Kugler, Bloom, Kaercher, Truax, & Storch, 2012). Digestive issues have been documented in animals that experienced MS and are reviewed elsewhere (Barreau, Ferrier, Fioramonti, & Bueno, 2007).
Treatment Implications
Exposure to adversity early in life is associated with increased susceptibility to other disorders in adulthood besides mental illness, including cancer, diabetes mellitus, and obesity (Burdge, Lillycrop, & Jackson, 2009; Felitti et al., 1998). A greater understanding about how important variables an many different levels of analysis contribute to the negative consequences of adversity exposure can be used to design interventions (Cicchetti & Gunnar, 2008). Such variables include the duration of the stressor, the immediacy or lack thereof of consequences, and the transitions that occur during the course of the resultant shifts in brain development. Discussion of these variables from a translational perspective may shed light on the gaps in our knowledge. As we (Andersen, 2005; Stanis & Andersen, in press) and others (Fishbein, 2000; Studts & van Zyl, 2013) have discussed elsewhere, interventions need to be developmentally appropriate for the subject, specific to the modality affected, and well timed to maximize their effects (Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick, & McEwen, 2013).
Maternal behavior has a significant influence on the outcome of fear, anxiety, and depressive behaviors. A recent clinical study found that reducing the child’s exposure to a depressed mother (presumably a condition that reduces her care-giving) by placing the child in daycare was associated with a significant reduction in internalizing behaviors (Herba et al., 2013). Programs that aim to increase support to a mother who is experiencing domestic abuse, which is likely to reduce her ability to care for her child and expose the child to a traumatizing event, may further provide an effective intervention to break the chain of abuse (Taft et al., 2011). The beneficial effects of brief separations during development have been documented where brief handling for 15 min daily in rats reduces stress responsiveness (Plotsky et al., 2005) and improves spatial working memory and novel object recognition in female rats (Plescia et al., 2013). Even improved environments during pregnancy may have positive benefits. Environmental enrichment during pregnancy and lactation had long-lasting effects on reactivity to acute and chronic stress in offspring that were not highly dependent on early postpartum maternal behavior (Welberg, Thrivikraman, & Plotsky, 2006). A number of intervention approaches for older children with a history of maltreatment are being developed (reviewed by Toth & Cicchetti, 2013).
Interventions would benefit greatly from improved reporting of abusive experiences. A single episode of abuse is associated with more favorable outcomes than repeated exposures (Copeland, Keeler, Angold, & Costello, 2007). Unfortunately, females are more likely to experience higher rates of revictimization of abuse, which may explain higher rates of depression in females (Koenen & Widom, 2009). Secondary to increased revictimization, a window of opportunity exists for intervention even though the individual is not symptomatic (Teicher et al., 2009). As depression is accelerated into early adolescence following maltreatment (Gladstone et al., 2004), symptoms do not emerge until this period. In our clinical sample, we found that an average of 9.2 years lapsed between the initial abusive event and depression onset (Teicher et al., 2009).
This review highlighted the historical and current understanding of the effects of social stress manipulations on behaviors related to depression. While much research has been conducted in this area, a greater understanding of the specific aspects of depression is still warranted. A number of factors need to be considered when identifying the individual’s degree of risk to develop depression (e.g., genetics, gender, age of abuse, and duration of abuse), as well as what symptoms to predict. Early life stress seems to more strongly influence anxiety, mood, and anhedonia, whereas later life stress appears to have greater effects on externalizing behaviors, which were not discussed in this review. Early interventions (Wachs et al., 2013) that consider the implications of any or all of these factors are likely to impact depression reduction.
Acknowledgments
I thank the Simches family for their continued support of my research and acknowledge funding from Grants R01 DA-015403-10 and R01 DA-026485-04. A special thank you to Carryl Navalta for helpful comments and editing.
References
- Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Rio J, Ramirez MJ. Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience. 2008;154:1218–1226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL. Stimulants and the developing brain. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 2005;26:237–243. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Teicher MH. Delayed effects of early stress on hippocampal development. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1988–1993. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Teicher MH. Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neuroscience. 2008;31:183–191. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Teicher MH. Desperately driven and no brakes: Developmental stress exposure and subsequent risk for substance abuse. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33:516–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Thompson BS. Reply to Abramowitz et al.: Animal Models of OCD. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69:e31–e32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Tomada A, Vincow ES, Valente E, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2008;20:292–301. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.20.3.292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anisman H, Zaharia MD, Meaney MJ, Merali Z. Do early-life events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 1998;16:149–164. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(98)00025-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arakawa H. Ontogeny of sex differences in defensive burying behavior in rats: Effect of social isolation. Aggression and Behavior. 2007;33:38–47. doi: 10.1002/ab.20165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bangasser DA, Valentino RJ. Sex differences in molecular and cellular substrates of stress. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology. 2012;32:709–723. doi: 10.1007/s10571-012-9824-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barreau F, Ferrier L, Fioramonti J, Bueno L. New insights in the etiology and pathophysiology of irritable bowel syndrome: Contribution of neonatal stress models. Pediatric Research. 2007;62:240–245. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e3180db2949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beardslee WR, Gladstone TR, O’Connor EE. Developmental risk of depression: Experience matters. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics North America. 2012;21:261–278. vii. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beitchman JH, Zucker KJ, Hood JE, daCosta GA, Akman D, Cassavia E. A review of the long-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1992;16:101–118. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(92)90011-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernardi JR, Ferreira CF, Senter G, Krolow R, de Aguiar BW, Portella AK, et al. Early life stress interacts with the diet deficiency of omega-3 fatty acids during the life course increasing the metabolic vulnerability in adult rats. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e62031. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevans K, Cerbone A, Overstreet S. Relations between recurrent trauma exposure and recent life stress and salivary cortisol among children. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:257–272. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Deaver K, Kirby RF. Leptin disinhibits non-shivering thermogenesis in infants after maternal separation. American Journal of Physiology. 1999;276:R606–R610. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.276.2.R606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourke CH, Neigh GN. Behavioral effects of chronic adolescent stress are sustained and sexually dimorphic. Hormones and Behavior. 2011;60:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowden DM, McKinney WT. Behavioral effects of peer separation, isolation, and reunion on adolescent male rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology. 1972;5:353–362. doi: 10.1002/dev.420050408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branchi I, Alleva E. Communal nesting, an early social enrichment, increases the adult anxiety-like response and shapes the role of social context in modulating the emotional behavior. Behavioral Brain Research. 2006;172:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse—A preliminary report. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;41:23–32. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00162-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenes JC, Padilla M, Fornaguera J. A detailed analysis of open-field habituation and behavioral and neurochemical antidepressant-like effects in postweaning enriched rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2009;197:125–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenhouse HC, Andersen SL. Developmental trajectories during adolescence in males and females: A cross-species understanding of underlying brain changes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1687–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenhouse HC, Sonntag KC, Andersen SL. Transient D1 dopamine receptor expression on prefrontal cortex projection neurons: Relationship to enhanced motivational salience of drug cues in adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:2375–2382. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5064-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brent DA. The treatment of SSRI-resistant depression in adolescents (TORDIA): In search of the best next step. Depression and Anxiety. 2009;26:871–874. doi: 10.1002/da.20617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. Medical symptoms, health risk, and history of childhood sexual abuse. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 1992;67:603–604. doi: 10.1016/s0025-6196(12)60471-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown DW, Anda RF, Edwards VJ, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Giles WH. Adverse childhood experiences and childhood autobiographical memory disturbance. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2007;31:961–969. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunelli SA, Shindledecker RD, Hofer MA. Early experience and maternal behavior in rats. Developmental Psychobiology. 1989;22:295–314. doi: 10.1002/dev.420220308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdge GC, Lillycrop KA, Jackson AA. Nutrition in early life, and risk of cancer and metabolic disease: Alternative endings in an epigenetic tale? British Journal of Nutrition. 2009;101:619–630. doi: 10.1017/S0007114508145883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrion VG, Wong SS. Can traumatic stress alter the brain? Understanding the implications of early trauma on brain development and learning. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2012;51:S23–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Review of Neuroscience. 2002;3:617–628. doi: 10.1038/nrn896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne FA. Epigenetics and developmental plasticity across species. Developmental Psychobiology. 2013;55:33–41. doi: 10.1002/dev.21036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Gunnar MR. Integrating biological measures into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:737–743. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consultation and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:6–20. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Gunnar MR, Toth SL. The differential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development. 2010;81:252–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01393.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Toth SL. A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34:541–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199505000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccurello R, Bielawski A, Zelek-Molik A, Vetulani J, Kowalska M, D’Amato FR, et al. Brief maternal separation affects brain alpha1-adrenoceptors and apoptotic signaling in adult mice. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2014;48:161–169. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran RF. Men and depression: Clinical and empirical perspectives. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Copeland WE, Keeler G, Angold A, Costello EJ. Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:577–584. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cressman VL, Balaban J, Steinfeld S, Shemyakin A, Graham P, Parisot N, et al. Prefrontal cortical inputs to the basal amygdala undergo pruning during late adolescence in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2010;518:2693–2709. doi: 10.1002/cne.22359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cromheeke S, Herpoel LA, Mueller SC. Childhood abuse is related to working memory impairment for positive emotion in female university students. Child Maltreatreatment. doi: 10.1177/1077559513511522. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruz FC, Marin MT, Leao RM, Planeta CS. Behavioral and neuroendocrine effects of the exposure to chronic restraint or variable stress in early adolescent rats. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2012;30:19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM. Amygdalo–cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: Implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;453:116–130. doi: 10.1002/cne.10376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyranowski JM, Frank E, Young E, Shear MK. Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: A theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:21–27. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalla C, Edgecomb C, Whetstone AS, Shors TJ. Females do not express learned helplessness like males do. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;3:1559–1569. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalla C, Pitychoutis PM, Kokras N, Papadopoulou-Daifoti Z. Sex differences in response to stress and expression of depressive-like behaviours in the rat. Current Topics in Behavior Neuroscience. 2011;8:97–118. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Da Silva NL, Ferreira VM, de Carobrez AP, Morato GS. Individual housing from rearing modifies the performance of young rats on the elevated plus-maze apparatus. Physiology & Behavior. 1996;60:1391–1396. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00254-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeBellis MD, Keshavan MS. Sex differences in brain maturation in maltreatment-related pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:103–117. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00013-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeBellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, Casey BJ, Gledd JN, Boring AM, et al. A.E. Bennett Research Award: Developmental traumatology: Part II. Brain development. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:1271–1284. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00045-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeBellis MD, Narasimhan A, Thatcher DL, Keshavan MS, Soloff P, Clark DB. Prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and cerebellar volumes in adolescents and young adults with adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders and comorbid mental disorders. Alcohol and Clinical Experimental Research. 2005;29:1590–1600. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000179368.87886.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delahaye F, Breton C, Risold PY, Enache M, Dutriez-Casteloot I, Laborie C, et al. Maternal perinatal undernutrition drastically reduces postnatal leptin surge and affects the development of arcuate nucleus proopiomelanocortin neurons in neonatal male rat pups. Endocrinology. 2008;149:470–475. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delaunay F, Khan A, Cintra A, Davani B, Ling ZC, Andersson A, et al. Pancreatic beta cells are important targets for the diabetogenic effects of glucocorticoids. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 1997;100:2094–2098. doi: 10.1172/JCI119743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denenberg VH, Garbanati J, Sherman DA, Yutzey DA, Kaplan R. Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science. 1978;201:1150–1152. doi: 10.1126/science.684436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Der-Avakian A, Markou A. Neonatal maternal separation exacerbates the reward-enhancing effect of acute amphetamine administration and the anhedonic effect of repeated social defeat in adult rats. Neuroscience. 2010;170:1189–1198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Detke MJ, Rickels M, Lucki I. Active behaviors in the rat forced swimming test differentially produced by serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants. Psychopharmacology. 1995;121:66–72. doi: 10.1007/BF02245592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doom JR, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Dackis MN. Child maltreatment and gender interactions as predictors of differential neuroendocrine profiles. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:1442–1454. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Chapman DP, Williamson DF, Giles WH. Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: Findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2001;286:3089–3096. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.24.3089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duggal S, Carlson EA, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. Depressive symptomatology in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:143–164. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401001109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dugovic C, Maccari S, Weibel L, Turek FW, Van Reeth O. High corticosterone levels in prenatally stressed rats predict persistent paradoxical sleep alterations. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:8656–8664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-08656.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn EC, McLaughlin KA, Slopen N, Rosand J, Smoller JW. Developmental timing of child maltreatment and symptoms of depression and suicidal ideation in young adulthood: Results from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Depression and Anxiety. 2013;30:955–964. doi: 10.1002/da.22102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiland L, Romeo RD. Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience. 2013;249:162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feiring C, Taska L, Lewis M. Age and gender differences in children’s and adolescents’ adaptation to sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1999;23:115–128. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(98)00116-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felitti VJ. Long-term medical consequences of incest, rape, and molestation. South Medical Journal. 1991;84:328–331. doi: 10.1097/00007611-199103000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventative Medicine. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng M, Sheng G, Li Z, Wang J, Ren K, Jin X, et al. Postnatal maternal separation enhances tonic GABA current of cortical layer 5 pyramidal neurons in juvenile rats and promotes genesis of GABAergic neurons in neocortical molecular layer and subventricular zone in adult rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2013;260:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fergusson DM, McLeod GF, Horwood LJ. Childhood sexual abuse and adult developmental outcomes: Findings from a 30-year longitudinal study in New Zealand. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2013;37:664–674. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE. Exploration, distraction, and habituation in rats reared in isolation. Developmental Psychobiology. 1978;11:73–81. doi: 10.1002/dev.420110111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein D. The importance of neurobiological research to the prevention of psychopathology. Prevention Science. 2000;1:89–106. doi: 10.1023/a:1010090114858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankola KA, Flora AL, Torres AK, Grissom EM, Overstreet S, Dohanich GP. Effects of early rearing conditions on cognitive performance in prepubescent male and female rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2010;94:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund N, Thompson BS, Denormandie J, Vaccarro K, Andersen SL. Windows of vulnerability: Maternal separation, age, and fluoxe-tine on adolescent depressive-like behavior in rats. Neuroscience. 2013;249:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.06.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman E, Berman M, Overstreet D. Swim test immobility in a genetic rat model of depression is modified by maternal environment: A cross-foster study. Developmental Psychobiology. 2006;48:169–177. doi: 10.1002/dev.20119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuller-Thomson E, Baker TM, Brennenstuhl S. Evidence supporting an independent association between childhood physical abuse and lifetime suicidal ideation. Suicide and Life Threatening Behaivor. 2012;42:279–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00089.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Rajapakse JC, Vaituzis AC, Rapoport JL. Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 1997;21:1185–1201. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(97)00158-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmer WS, McKinney WT. Early experience and depressive disorders: Human and non-human primate studies. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2003;75:97–113. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(03)00046-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gladstone GL, Parker GB, Mitchell PB, Malhi GS, Wilhelm K, Austin MP. Implications of childhood trauma for depressed women: An analysis of pathways from childhood sexual abuse to deliberate self-harm and revictimization. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161:1417–1425. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.8.1417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman GS, Quas JA, Ogle CM. Child maltreatment and memory. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:325–351. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorisch J, Schwarting RK. Wistar rats with high versus low rearing activity differ in radial maze performance. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2006;86:175–187. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS. Experience and brain development. Child Development. 1987;58:539–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustafson TB, Sarwer DB. Childhood sexual abuse and obesity. Obesity Reviews. 2004;5:129–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2004.00145.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajszan T, Szigeti-Buck K, Sallam NL, Bober J, Parducz A, Maclusky NJ, et al. Effects of estradiol on learned helplessness and associated remodeling of hippocampal spine synapses in female rats. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:168–174. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall FS, Huang S, Fong GW, Pert A, Linnoila M. Effects of isolation-rearing on locomotion, anxiety and responses to ethanol in Fawn Hooded and Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology. 1998;139:203–209. doi: 10.1007/s002130050705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall FS, Humby T, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. The effects of isolation-rearing on sucrose consumption in rats. Physiology & Behavior. 1997;62:291–297. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00116-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haller J, Kruk MR. Normal and abnormal aggression: Human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:292–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlow HF, Harlow MK. The effect of rearing conditions on behavior. International Journal of Psychiatry. 1965;1:43–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmer CJ, Phillips GD. Isolation rearing enhances acquisition in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. Physiology & Behavior. 1998;65:525–533. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00207-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Nater UM, Maloney E, Boneva R, Jones JF, Reeves WC. Childhood trauma and risk for chronic fatigue syndrome: Association with neuroendocrine dysfunction. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:72–80. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Wagner D, Maloney E, Papanicolaou DA, Solomon L, Jones JF, et al. Early adverse experience and risk for chronic fatigue syndrome: Results from a population-based study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:1258–1266. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herba CM, Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Liu X, Mongeau C, Seguin JR, et al. Maternal depressive symptoms and children’s emotional problems: Can early child care help children of depressed mothers? JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:830–838. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinde RA, Spencer-Booth Y, Bruce M. Effects of 6-day maternal deprivation on rhesus monkey infants. Nature. 1966;210:1021–1023. doi: 10.1038/2101021a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofer MA. Studies on how early maternal deprivation produces behavioral change in young rats. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1975;37:245–264. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197505000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong S, Flashner B, Chiu M, ver Hoeve E, Luz S, Bhatnagar S. Social isolation in adolescence alters behaviors in the forced swim and sucrose preference tests in female but not in male rats. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;105:269–275. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology. 1970;206:419–436. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Early exploration of the visual cortex. Neuron. 1998;20:401–412. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80984-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, LeVay S. Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1977;278:377–409. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1977.0050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK. Neonatal maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Research. 2002;950:52–63. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02985-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackowski A, Perera TD, Abdallah CG, Garrido G, Tang CY, Martinez J, et al. Early-life stress, corpus callosum development, hippocampal volumetrics, and anxious behavior in male nonhuman primates. Psychiatry Research. 2011;192:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jankord R, Solomon MB, Albertz J, Flak JN, Zhang R, Herman JP. Stress vulnerability during adolescent development in rats. Endocrinology. 2011;152:629–638. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-0658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juraska JM, Henderson C, Muller J. Differential rearing experience, gender, and radial maze performance. Developmental Psychobiology. 1984;17:209–215. doi: 10.1002/dev.420170302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplow JB, Widom CS. Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts long-term mental health outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:176–187. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman D, Banerji MA, Shorman I, Smith EL, Coplan JD, Rosenblum LA, et al. Early-life stress and the development of obesity and insulin resistance in juvenile bonnet macaques. Diabetes. 2007;56:1382–1386. doi: 10.2337/db06-1409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB, Charney DS. Effects of early adverse experiences on brain structure and function: Clinical implications. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:778–790. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00998-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Bulik CM, Silberg J, Hettema JM, Myers J, Prescott CA. Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use disorders in women: An epidemiological and cotwin control analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:953–959. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, et al. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the National Co-morbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:8–19. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinkead R, Montandon G, Bairam A, Lajeunesse Y, Horner R. Neonatal maternal separation disrupts regulation of sleep and breathing in adult male rats. Sleep. 2009;32:1611–1620. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.12.1611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyono S, Seo ML, Shibagaki M. Effects of rearing environments upon sleep–waking parameters in rats. Physiology & Behavior. 1981;26:391–394. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90164-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klomek AB, Kleinman M, Altschuler E, Marrocco F, Amakawa L, Gould MS. Suicidal adolescents’ experiences with bullying perpetration and victimization during high school as risk factors for later depression and suicidality. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;53:S37–S42. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenen KC, Widom CS. A prospective study of sex differences in the lifetime risk of posttraumatic stress disorder among abused and neglected children grown up. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2009;22:566–574. doi: 10.1002/jts.20478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornetsky C, Esposito RU. Euphorigenic drugs: Effects on the reward pathways of the brain. Federation Proceedings. 1979;38:2473–2476. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakow B, Melendrez D, Johnston L, Warner TD, Clark JO, Pacheco M, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, psychiatric distress, and quality of life impairment in sexual assault survivors. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders. 2002;190:442–452. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200207000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kugler BB, Bloom M, Kaercher LB, Truax TV, Storch EA. Somatic symptoms in traumatized children and adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2012;43:661–673. doi: 10.1007/s10578-012-0289-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn CM, Butler SR, Schanberg SM. Selective depression of serum growth hormone during maternal deprivation in rat pups. Science. 1978;201:1034–1036. doi: 10.1126/science.684424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn CM, Pauk J, Schanberg SM. Endocrine responses to mother–infant separation in developing rats. Developmental Psychobiology. 1990;23:395–410. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumpulainen K, Rasanen E, Henttonen I, Almqvist F, Kresanov K, Linna SL, et al. Bullying and psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-age children. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1998;22:705–717. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(98)00049-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kundakovic M, Lim S, Gudsnuk K, Champagne FA. Sex-specific and strain-dependent effects of early life adversity on behavioral and epigenetic outcomes. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2013;4:78. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuppermann BD, Kasamatsu T. Enhanced binocular interaction in the visual cortex of normal kittens subjected to intracortical norepinephrine perfusion. Brain Research. 1984;302:91–99. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)91288-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuramochi M, Nakamura S. Effects of postnatal isolation rearing and antidepressant treatment on the density of serotonergic and noradrenergic axons and depressive behavior in rats. Neuroscience. 2009;163:448–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambillotte C, Gilon P, Henquin JC. Direct glucocorticoid inhibition of insulin secretion: An in vitro study of dexamethasone effects in mouse islets. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 1997;99:414–423. doi: 10.1172/JCI119175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laudenslager ML, Held PE, Boccia ML, Reite ML, Cohen JJ. Behavioral and immunological consequences of brief mother–infant separation: A species comparison. Developmental Psychobiology. 1990;23:247–264. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee HJ, Kim JW, Yim SV, Kim MJ, Kim SA, Kim YJ, et al. Fluoxetine enhances cell proliferation and prevents apoptosis in dentate gyrus of maternally separated rats. Molecular Psychiatry. 2001;6:725–728. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann J, Pryce CR, Bettschen D, Feldon J. The maternal separation paradigm and adult emotionality and cognition in male and female Wistar rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1999;64:705–715. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00150-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leussis MP, Andersen SL. Is adolescence a sensitive period for depression? Behavioral and neuroanatomical findings from a social stress model. Synapse. 2008;62:22–30. doi: 10.1002/syn.20462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leussis MP, Freund N, Brenhouse HC, Thompson BS, Andersen SL. Depressive-like behavior in adolescents after maternal separation: Sex differences, controllability, and GABA. Developmental Neuroscience. 2012;34:210–217. doi: 10.1159/000339162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventopoulos M, Russig H, Feldon J, Pryce CR, Opacka-Juffry J. Early deprivation leads to long-term reductions in motivation for reward and 5-HT1A binding and both effects are reversed by fluoxetine. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:692–701. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine S. Maternal and environmental influences on the adrenocortical response to stress in weanling rats. Science. 1967;156:258–260. doi: 10.1126/science.156.3772.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine S, Johnson DF, Gonzalez CA. Behavioral and hormonal responses to separation in infant rhesus monkeys and mothers. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1985;99:399–410. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.99.3.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvin Y, Tovote P, Pentkowski NS, Zeyda T, King LB, Vasconcellos AJ, et al. Maternal separation modulates short-term behavioral and physiological indices of the stress response. Hormones and Behavior. 2010;58:241–249. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, et al. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Science. 1997;277:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukkes J, Vuong S, Scholl J, Oliver H, Forster G. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor antagonism within the dorsal raphe nucleus reduces social anxiety-like behavior after early-life social isolation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:9955–9960. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0854-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukkes JL, Watt MJ, Lowry CA, Forster GL. Consequences of post-weaning social isolation on anxiety behavior and related neural circuits in rodents. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;3:18. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.018.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacMillan HL, Fleming JE, Streiner DL, Lin E, Boyle MH, Jamieson E, et al. Childhood abuse and lifetime psychopathology in a community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:1878–1883. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macri S, Laviola G, Leussis MP, Andersen SL. Abnormal behavioral and neurotrophic development in the younger sibling receiving less maternal care in a communal nursing paradigm in rats. Psycho-neuroendocrinology. 2009;35:392–402. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macri S, Mason GJ, Wurbel H. Dissociation in the effects of neonatal maternal separations on maternal care and the offspring’s HPA and fear responses in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20:1017–1024. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03541.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macri S, Zoratto F, Laviola G. Early-stress regulates resilience, vulnerability and experimental validity in laboratory rodents through mother-offspring hormonal transfer. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1534–1543. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maestripieri D. Early experience affects the intergenerational transmission of infant abuse in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102:9726–9729. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504122102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier SF, Watkins LR. Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: The roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005;29:829–841. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marais L, van Rensburg SJ, van Zyl JM, Stein DJ, Daniels WM. Maternal separation of rat pups increases the risk of developing depressive-like behavior after subsequent chronic stress by altering corticosterone and neurotrophin levels in the hippocampus. Neuroscience Research. 2008;61:106–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus SM, Young EA, Kerber KB, Kornstein S, Farabaugh AH, Mitchell J, et al. Gender differences in depression: Findings from the STAR*D study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;87:141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews K, Hall FS, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. Retarded acquisition and reduced expression of conditioned locomotor activity in adult rats following repeated early maternal separation: Effects of prefeeding, d-amphetamine, dopamine antagonists and clonidine. Psychopharmacology. 1996;126:75–84. doi: 10.1007/BF02246414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews K, Robbins TW. Early experience as a determinant of adult behavioural responses to reward: The effects of repeated maternal separation in the rat. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:45–55. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews K, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. Repeated maternal separation of preweanling rats attenuates behavioral responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiology & Behavior. 1996;59:99–107. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02069-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:108–124. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00129-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta NS, Wang L, Redei EE. Sex differences in depressive, anxious behaviors and hippocampal transcript levels in a genetic rat model. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2013;12:695–704. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michaels CC, Easterling KW, Holtzman SG. Maternal separation alters ICSS responding in adult male and female rats, but morphine and naltrexone have little affect on that behavior. Brain Research Bulletin. 2007;73:310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J, McCauley E, Burke PM, Moss SJ. Phenomenology of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1988;27:12–20. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198801000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriceau S, Wilson DA, Levine S, Sullivan RM. Dual circuitry for odor-shock conditioning during infancy: Corticosterone switches between fear and attraction via amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6737–6748. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0499-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morielli A, Ladan S, Ducharme FM, Brouillette RT. Can sleep and wakefulness be distinguished in children by cardiorespiratory and videotape recordings? Chest. 1996;109:680–687. doi: 10.1378/chest.109.3.680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mourlon V, Baudin A, Blanc O, Lauber A, Giros B, Naudon L, et al. Maternal deprivation induces depressive-like behaviours only in female rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2010;213:278–287. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrdalj J, Pallesen S, Milde AM, Jellestad FK, Murison R, Ursin R, et al. Early and later life stress alters brain activity and sleep in rats. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e69923. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navalta CP, Polcari A, Webster DM, Boghossian A, Teicher MH. Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;18:45–53. doi: 10.1176/jnp.18.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson EC, Heath AC, Madden PA, Cooper ML, Dinwiddie SH, Bucholz KK, et al. Association between self-reported childhood sexual abuse and adverse psychosocial outcomes: Results from a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:139–145. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson EE, Herman KN, Barrett CE, Noble PL, Wojteczko K, Chisholm K, et al. Adverse rearing experiences enhance responding to both aversive and rewarding stimuli in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:702–704. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noll JG, Trickett PK, Susman EJ, Putnam FW. Sleep disturbances and childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31:469–480. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olton DS, Johnson CT, Howard E. Impairment of conditioned active avoidance in adult rats given corticosterone in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology. 1974;8:55–61. doi: 10.1002/dev.420080108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overstreet DH. Modeling depression in animal models. Methods in Molecular Biology. 2012;829:125–144. doi: 10.1007/978-1-61779-458-2_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papaioannou A, Gerozissis K, Prokopiou A, Bolaris S, Stylianopoulou F. Sex differences in the effects of neonatal handling on the animal’s response to stress and the vulnerability for depressive behaviour. Behavioral Brain Research. 2002;129:131–139. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00334-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paternain L, Martisova E, Milagro FI, Ramirez MJ, Martinez JA, Campion J. Postnatal maternal separation modifies the response to an obesogenic diet in adulthood in rats. Disease Models and Mechansims. 2012;5:691–697. doi: 10.1242/dmm.009043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul IA, English JA, Halaris A. Sucrose and quinine intake by maternally-deprived and control rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research. 2000;112:127–134. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00173-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pervanidou P, Chrousos GP. Stress and obesity/metabolic syndrome in childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity. 2011;6(Suppl 1):21–28. doi: 10.3109/17477166.2011.615996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plescia F, Marino RA, Navarra M, Gambino G, Brancato A, Sardo P, et al. Early handling effect on female rat spatial and non-spatial learning and memory. Behavioural Processes. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.10.011. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Research: Molecular Brain Research. 1993;18:195–200. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(93)90189-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Caldji C, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:2192–2204. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollack W. New psychotherapy for men. New York: Wiley; 1998. Mourning, melancholia and masculinity: Recognizing and treating depression in men; pp. 147–166. [Google Scholar]
- Quan MN, Tian YT, Xu KH, Zhang T, Yang Z. Postweaning social isolation influences spatial cognition, prefrontal cortical synaptic plasticity and hippocampal potassium ion channels in Wistar rats. Neuroscience. 2010;169:214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raineki C, Cortes MR, Belnoue L, Sullivan RM. Effects of early-life abuse differ across development: Infant social behavior deficits are followed by adolescent depressive-like behaviors mediated by the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7758–7765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5843-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao H, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Korczykowski M, Avants BB, et al. Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: Evidence from brain morphology in humans. Neuro-Image. 2010;49:1144–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reus GZ, Stringari RB, Ribeiro KF, Cipriano AL, Panizzutti BS, Stertz L, et al. Maternal deprivation induces depressive-like behaviour and alters neurotrophin levels in the rat brain. Neurochemical Research. 2011;36:460–466. doi: 10.1007/s11064-010-0364-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimsza ME, Berg RA, Locke C. Sexual abuse: Somatic and emotional reactions. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1988;12:201–208. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(88)90028-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW. Controlling stress: How the brain protects itself from depression. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:261–262. doi: 10.1038/nn0305-261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblum LA, Paully GS. The effects of varying environmental demands on maternal and infant behavior. Child Development. 1984;55:305–314. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruedi-Bettschen D, Zhang W, Russig H, Ferger B, Weston A, Pedersen EM, et al. Early deprivation leads to altered behavioural, autonomic and endocrine responses to environmental challenge in adult Fischer rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:2879–2893. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05158.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryu V, Yoo SB, Kang DW, Lee JH, Jahng JW. Post-weaning isolation promotes food intake and body weight gain in rats that experienced neonatal maternal separation. Brain Research. 2009;1295:127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Isolation-rearing enhances tail pinch-induced oral behavior in rats. Physiology & Behavior. 1977;18:53–58. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(77)90093-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salzmann C, Otis M, Long H, Roberge C, Gallo-Payet N, Walker CD. Inhibition of steroidogenic response to adrenocorticotropin by leptin: Implications for the adrenal response to maternal separation in neonatal rats. Endocrinology. 2004;145:1810–1822. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoedl AF, Costa MC, Mari JJ, Mello MF, Tyrka AR, Carpenter LL, et al. The clinical correlates of reported childhood sexual abuse: An association between age at trauma onset and severity of depression and PTSD in adults. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse. 2010;19:156–170. doi: 10.1080/10538711003615038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuck AM, Widom CS. Childhood victimization and alcohol symptoms in females: Causal inferences and hypothesized mediators. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2001;25:1069–1092. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(01)00257-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smotherman WP, Wiener SG, Mendoza SP, Levine S. Maternal pituitary–adrenal responsiveness as a function of differential treatment of rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology. 1977;10:113–122. doi: 10.1002/dev.420100204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solas M, Aisa B, Mugueta MC, Del Rio J, Tordera RM, Ramirez MJ. Interactions between age, stress and insulin on cognition: Implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1664–1673. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinelli S, Chefer S, Suomi SJ, Higley JD, Barr CS, Stein E. Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:658–665. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanis JT, Andersen S. Reducing substance use during adolescence: A translational framework for prevention. Psychopharmacology. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3393-1. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterley TL, Howells FM, Russell VA. Effects of early life trauma are dependent on genetic predisposition: A rat study. Behavior and Brain Function. 2011;7:11. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson CW, Meredith JP, Spicer CH, Mason R, Marsden CA. Early life programming of innate fear and fear learning in adult female rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2009;198:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Struber N, Struber D, Roth G. Impact of early adversity on glucocorticoid regulation and later mental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;38C:17–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Studts CR, van Zyl MA. Identification of developmentally appropriate screening items for disruptive behavior problems in preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013;41:851–863. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9738-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundstrom JM, Hall FS, Stellar JR, Waugh EJ. Effects of isolation-rearing on intracranial self-stimulation reward of the lateral hypothalamus: Baseline assessment and drug challenges. Life Sciences. 2002;70:2799–2810. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(02)01509-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ, Delizio R, Harlow HF. Social rehabilitation of separation-induced depressive disorders in monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1976;133:1279–1285. doi: 10.1176/ajp.133.11.1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ, Harlow HF, Domek CJ. Effect of repetitive infant–infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1970;76:161–172. doi: 10.1037/h0029809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taft AJ, Small R, Hegarty KL, Watson LF, Gold L, Lumley JA. Mothers’ AdvocateS In the Community (MOSAIC)—Non-professional mentor support to reduce intimate partner violence and depression in mothers: A cluster randomised trial in primary care. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:178. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang AC, Reeb-Sutherland BC, Romeo RD, McEwen BS. On the causes of early life experience effects: Evaluating the role of mom. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2013:S0091–S0302. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor RR, Jason LA. Chronic fatigue, abuse-related traumatization, and psychiatric disorders in a community-based sample. Society of Scientific Medicine. 2002;55:247–256. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00168-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SB, Taylor AR, Koenig JI. The interaction of disrupted Type II Neuregulin 1 and chronic adolescent stress on adult anxiety- and fear-related behaviors. Neuroscience. 2013;249:31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher MH. Scars that won’t heal: The neurobiology of child abuse. Scientific American. 2002;286:68–75. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0302-68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher MH, Dumont NL, Ito Y, Vaituzis C, Giedd JN, Andersen SL. Childhood neglect is associated with reduced corpus callosum area. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher MH, Samson JA, Polcari A, Andersen SL. Length of time between onset of childhood sexual abuse and emergence of depression in a young adult sample: A retrospective clinical report. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2009;70:684–691. doi: 10.4088/jcp.08m04235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomoda A, Sheu YS, Rabi K, Suzuki H, Navalta CP, Polcari A, et al. Exposure to parental verbal abuse is associated with increased gray matter volume in superior temporal gyrus. NeuroImage. 2011;54(Suppl 1):S280–S286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomoda A, Suzuki H, Rabi K, Sheu YS, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Reduced prefrontal cortical gray matter volume in young adults exposed to harsh corporal punishment. NeuroImage. 2009;47(Suppl 2):T66–T71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toth SL, Cicchetti D. A developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Child Maltreatreatment. 2013;18:135–139. doi: 10.1177/1077559513500380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trickett PK, Noll JG, Reiffman A, Putnam FW. Variants of intrafamilial sexual abuse experience: Implications for short- and long-term development. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:1001–1019. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tupler LA, DeBellis MD. Segmented hippocampal volume in children and adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59:523–529. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vamosi M, Heitmann BL, Kyvik KO. The relation between an adverse psychological and social environment in childhood and the development of adult obesity: A systematic literature review. Obesity Reviews. 2010;11:177–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2009.00645.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Werff SJ, Pannekoek JN, Veer IM, van Tol MJ, Aleman A, Veltman DJ, et al. Resilience to childhood maltreatment is associated with increased resting-state functional connectivity of the salience network with the lingual gyrus. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2013a;37:1021–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Werff SJ, Pannekoek JN, Veer IM, van Tol MJ, Aleman A, Veltman DJ, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in adults with childhood emotional maltreatment. Psychological Medicine. 2013b;43:1825–1836. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Veen T, Wardenaar KJ, Carlier IV, Spinhoven P, Penninx BW, Zitman FG. Are childhood and adult life adversities differentially associated with specific symptom dimensions of depression and anxiety? Testing the tripartite model. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;146:238–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ver Hoeve ES, Kelly G, Luz S, Ghanshani S, Bhatnagar S. Short-term and long-term effects of repeated social defeat during adolescence or adulthood in female rats. Neuroscience. 2013;249:63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkmar FR, Greenough WT. Rearing complexity affects branching of dendrites in the visual cortex of the rat. Science. 1972;176:1445–1447. doi: 10.1126/science.176.4042.1445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wachs TD, Georgieff M, Cusick S, McEwen BS. Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: Contributions from nutrition, neuroscience, and psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2013 doi: 10.1111/nyas.12314. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warren WG, Ivinskis A. Isolation-rearing effects on emotionality of hooded rats. Psychological Reports. 1973;32:1011–1014. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1973.32.3.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weathington JM, Arnold AR, Cooke BM. Juvenile social subjugation induces a sex-specific pattern of anxiety and depression-like behaviors in adult rats. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welberg L, Thrivikraman KV, Plotsky PM. Combined pre-and postnatal environmental enrichment programs the HPA axis differentially in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:553–564. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widom CS, DuMont K, Czaja SJ. A prospective investigation of major depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:49–56. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wigger A, Neumann ID. Periodic maternal deprivation induces gender-dependent alterations in behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to emotional stress in adult rats. Physiology & Behavior. 1999;66:293–302. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00300-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson ME, Bounar S, Godfrey J, Michopoulos V, Higgins M, Sanchez M. Social and emotional predictors of the tempo of puberty in female rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:67–83. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winterfeld KT, Teuchert-Noodt G, Dawirs RR. Social environment alters both ontogeny of dopamine innervation of the medial pre-frontal cortex and maturation of working memory in gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) Journal of Neuroscience Research. 1998;52:201–209. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4547(19980415)52:2<201::AID-JNR8>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolk S, Weissman M. American Psychiatric Press review of psychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1995. Women and depression: An update; pp. 14227–14259. [Google Scholar]
- Woolley CS, Gould E, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:4035–4039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-12-04035.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright IK, Upton N, Marsden CA. Resocialisation of isolation-reared rats does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated X-maze model of anxiety. Physiology & Behavior. 1991;50:1129–1132. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90572-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young E, Korszun A. Sex, trauma, stress hormones and depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15:23–28. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]