Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Prev Sci. 2017 Feb;18(2):131–140. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0698-4

A Web-Based Intervention to Reduce Indoor Tanning Motivations In Adolescents: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Joel Hillhouse 1, Rob Turrisi 2, Nichole M Scaglione 2, Michael J Cleveland 3, Katie Baker 1, L Carter Florence 1
PMCID: PMC5247320  NIHMSID: NIHMS812251  PMID: 27549602

Abstract

Youthful indoor tanning as few as ten sessions can increase the risk of melanoma by 2 to 4 times with each additional session adding another 2% to the risk. Recent research estimates that indoor tanning can be linked to approximately 450,000 cases of skin cancer annually in the United States, Europe, and Australia. Despite these risks, indoor tanning remains popular with adolescents. This study tested the efficacy of a web-based skin cancer prevention intervention designed to reduce indoor tanning motivations in adolescent females. A nationally representative sample of 443 female teens were enrolled from an online panel into a two-arm, parallel group design, randomized controlled trial. Treatment participants received an appearance-focused intervention grounded in established health behavior change models. Controls viewed a teen alcohol prevention website. Outcome variables included willingness and intentions to indoor tan, willingness to sunless tan and measures of indoor tanning attitudes and beliefs. The intervention decreased willingness and intentions to indoor tan and increased sunless tanning willingness relative to controls. We also examined indirect mechanisms of change through intervening variables (e.g., indoor tanning attitudes, norms, positive and negative expectancies) using the product of coefficients approach. The web-based intervention demonstrated efficacy in changing adolescent indoor tanning motivations and improving their orientation toward healthier alternatives. Results from the intervening variable analyses give guidance to future adolescent skin cancer prevention interventions.

Keywords: melanoma prevention, web-based intervention, adolescents, indoor tanning, national sample


Melanoma is one of the most common cancers found in young women in the United States (National Cancer Institute, 2015). Indoor tanning is an important melanoma risk factor, linked to approximately 450,000 cases of skin cancer annually in the United States, Europe, and Australia (Wehner et al, 2014). Youthful indoor tanning as few as ten sessions can increase melanoma risk by 2 to 4 times (Cust et al., 2011) with each additional tanning session adding another 2% to the risk (Boniol, Autier, Boyle, & Gandini, 2012). Consequently, reducing indoor tanning has emerged as a central goal in the Surgeon General's Call to Action to Prevent Skin Cancer (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2014).

Despite these significant risks, indoor tanning is still popular among high school and young adult females in the US (Guy, Tai, & Richardson, 2011). Approximately one-third of US female adolescents report either engagement or future interest in indoor tanning (Guy et al., 2015). Epidemiological data indicate that indoor tanning increases at 16 years old and then again at 18 years old as adolescents gain increasing mobility, finances and freedom (Wehner et al., 2014). These trends are troubling given early adoption of indoor tanning has been associated with increased odds of becoming a frequent habitual indoor tanner later in life (Baker, Hillhouse, & Liu, 2010). Decreasing indoor tanning access and motivation in teens will be critical for achieving the Surgeon General's goals of reducing melanoma incidence.

To combat the risk from indoor tanning, anti-tanning policies directed at restricting minor access to tanning businesses have gained momentum (Guy et al., 2014; Pan & Geller, 2015). These regulations are critical for reducing use of indoor tanning facilities by non-adults. However, the impact of even the most stringent legislation ceases at age 18 years old, precisely when indoor tanning use tends to increase. Moreover, there are off-the-grid, non-salon tanning locations that are either not regulated (e.g., homes, gyms, etc.) or where regulations are poorly enforced (Hillhouse, Stapleton, Florence, & Pagoto, 2015; Pagoto et al., 2015). The indoor tanning policy initiatives need to be accompanied by primary and secondary prevention efforts directed at adolescent tanning motivations if they are to reduce indoor tanning behavior across the lifespan.

Previous intervention studies demonstrated successful indoor tanning reduction in adults (Gibbons, Gerrard, Lane, Mahler, & Kulik, 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse, Turrisi, Stapleton, & Robinson, 2008; Stapleton et al., 2015; Stapleton, Turrisi, Hillhouse, Robinson, & Abar, 2010). Several of these studies (Gibbons, Gerrard, Lane, Mahler, & Kulik, 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Stapleton et al., 2015; Stapleton, Turrisi, Hillhouse, Robinson, & Abar, 2010) utilized an appearance-focused approach grounded in established health behavior change models (e.g., theory of reasoned action; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; behavioral alternatives; Jaccard, 1981; prototype-willingness; Gibbons, Gerrard, & Lane, 2009).

The behavioral alternatives model (BAM; Jaccard, 1981; 2012; Jaccard & Wood, 1986) recognizes important goals can usually be satisfied by an array of potential behaviors. For example, imagine a typical adolescent woman faced with the decision of whether or not to indoor tan before the prom. Her decision is influenced by how she evaluates indoor tanning, but also by how she appraises alternative methods available to enhance her appearance. For example, she might consider using sunless tanning products. Sunless tanning products, often termed self-tanners, are lotions, creams and sprays that can make the skin look tan without ultraviolet radiation. In general, she will choose the alternative that will accomplish her goals (e.g., improved appearance for prom) in a way that is most appealing to her.

An intervention guided by the behavioral alternatives approach offers healthier alternative behaviors to achieve goals previously obtained through the unhealthy behavior. Offering a healthy alternative to achieve goals reduces the person's motivation to go back to the health risky activity once the intervention ends thereby improving treatment sustainability. The behavioral alternatives model has proven successful in understanding health-risk behaviors including adult indoor tanning (Danoff-Burg & Mosher, 2006; Hillhouse et al, 1999; 2008), sunbathing and sunscreen use in adolescents (Turrisi et al, 1998; 1999), organ donation (Radecki & Jaccard, 1997), and college alcohol drinking (Turrisi et al, 2010).

Behavioral willingness is another construct to consider when attempting to modify adolescent behavior motivations. Gibbons and Gerrard (1995) developed this construct to capture the phenomena whereby adolescents will frequently indicate no intention to participate in risky behaviors yet do so when given the chance. Willingness refers to a receptivity to behavioral opportunity and reflects what teens might be willing to do under certain social conditions. Behavioral willingness and intentions are highly correlated. However, willingness consistently captures about 10% of unique adolescent health risk behavior variance (Gibbons, Gerrard, & Lane, 2009), and willingness is typically a stronger predictor than intentions for younger teens (Gerrard, Gibbons, Houlihan, Stock, & Pomery, 2008). As teens grow older and gain more experience, behavioral intentions eventually pass willingness in the prediction of most behaviors (Pomery, Gibbons, Reis-Bergan, & Gerrard, 2009). For these reasons, we examined the effects of the intervention on both indoor tanning intentions and willingness in this study.

A review of the literature revealed only one pilot intervention (Lazovich et al., 2013) and one school-based intervention (Aarestrup et al., 2014) focused on adolescent indoor tanning. The current study adds to this literature by examining the efficacy of a web-based intervention designed to reduce tanning motivations in female adolescents. We examined the direct effects of the intervention on short-term (6 month) indoor tanning intentions and willingness in a national sample of adolescent females using a randomized controlled trial. We also examined how the intervention potentially impacted the tanning motivation outcomes through intervening variables derived from the theoretical model (see Figure 1) which guided the intervention.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A theoretical model of the appearance intervention influence on indoor tanning intentions and willingness.

Study Hypotheses

Hypothesis 1

The female adolescents in the intervention condition will demonstrate significant reductions in indoor tanning intentions and willingness and increases in sunless tanning willingness compared to the female adolescents in the placebo control condition.

Hypothesis 2

The changes in indoor tanning intentions and willingness will be associated with changes in the psychosocial intervening variables from the theoretical model (e.g., indoor tanning attitudes, normative beliefs, positive and negative expectancies).

Methods

Study Design

The study used a two-arm, parallel group design, randomized controlled trial in female teens (see Figure 2 for a study design participant flow diagram). Intervention participants viewed a website designed to reduce indoor tanning motivations and increase sunless tanning willingness. Control participants were treated identically to intervention participants with the exception that they viewed a teen-oriented alcohol prevention website, Above the Influence (Partnership for Drug-Free Kids, 2015). Participants from two cohorts completed baseline assessments in May of 2011 and 2012. There were 443 participants recruited, of which 388 completed both the baseline assessment and the six-month follow-up (87.6% retention rate).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Study design participant flow diagram.

Participants

We recruited an adolescent female sample drawn from the nationally representative Knowledge Networks KnowledgePanel®. KnowledgePanel®, a commercial online panel for measurement of public opinion, attitudes and behaviors, uses probability-based sampling with an address-based sampling methodology for selecting panel members (GfK, 2015). Therefore, the panel provides a high level of representativeness for whatever samples are selected. Parents from the panel who had a 12 to 18-year-old daughter were sent an invitation to participate. After obtaining consent from the parent and teen, a screening survey designed to determine eligibility was administered. A total of 1,337 parent-teen dyads provided consent for the screener measure. Of these, 571 parent-teen dyads met the eligibility requirements (i.e., female daughters between 12 and 18 years old who report previous indoor tanning use or strong intentions or willingness to indoor tan in the future). These dyads were invited to participate in the intervention. The invitation was accepted by 443 dyads (77.6% participation rate) who were randomized into intervention (n = 214) and control (n = 229) groups. We offered participants the opportunity to earn up to $120 compensation for completing all study requirements. The university's Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.

Indoor Tanning Prevention Intervention

The intervention was based on the theoretical model in Figure 1 (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008). We pilot tested this theoretical model with a separate teen sample to identify variables to target in the intervention. The target variables identified included positive tanning expectancies (e.g., I need a tan to look good, tanning is a good way to relax), negative tanning expectancies (e.g., skin cancer risk of indoor tanning, skin damage risk of indoor tanning) and social/cultural norms (e.g., peer indoor tanning popularity and acceptability, social group pressure to tan).

The research team developed modules for each topic in the theoretical model. A professional digital marketing company, Marketing Strategies, Inc., converted each module into a website which was pretested with adolescents and then further refined using an interactive process involving the investigative team, the web development team and adolescent tanner beta testers. The final website contained four main sections (Fashion and Beauty; Celebrity Watch; Peer Relationships; Resources) with 15 subsections (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Online intervention sections, subsections and key variables targeted

Main Sections Subsections Key Variables Targeted
Fashion and Beauty Importance of healthy skin for appearance 1, 3, 6
Tanning effects on skin appearance and health 1. 3, 4, 5, 6
Skin damage harm avoidance strategies 1, 4, 5, 6
Teen narratives: Indoor tanning caused melanoma and appearance damage 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6
Celebrity Watch Importance of healthy skin for celebrities appearance 1, 3, 6, 7
Celebrity avoidance of UV tanning and exposure 1, 3, 6, 7
Celebrity use of sunless tanning 7, 10
Peer Relationships Actual versus perceived indoor tanning use by peers 8, 9
How peer pressure leads to unwanted behavior including tanning 9
Tips on handling peer pressure 9
Specific tips on handling tanning peer pressure 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 9
Specific information on pressure to tan for prom and how to avoid this 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10
Resources Links to fashion and beauty sites that provide tanning alternatives information 1, 2, 3, 7, 9
Links to sunless tanning information 10
Links to indoor tanning health information (i.e., ACS, NCI, etc) 3, 4, 5

Targeted Variables

1

Need tan to look good.

2

No tan is unattractive and unpopular.

3

Look healthier with tan.

4

Skin cancer risk.

5

Other health problem risk.

6

Skin damage risk.

7

Celebrity tanning.

8

Peer indoor tanning popularity and acceptability.

9

Social group pressure to tan,

10

Sunless tanning as a tanning alternative

Intervention Integrity

We evaluated intervention integrity for the website by examining data analytics that tracked the number of pages visited, and the amount of time spent on each page. We told participants their role in the research was to evaluate and provide feedback on health websites designed for teen audiences. The website analytics revealed that intervention participants spent an average of 23 minutes reviewing the website (range 0 to 66 minutes; sd =18.8 minutes). Approximately 89% visited at least 50% of the website pages, with 73% visiting 100% of the website pages. We did not have access to the data analytics for the control website. To obtain an estimate of control participation, at follow-up, we asked control participants whether they had visited the Above the Influence website. One hundred ninety-three of the 206 control participants (93.7%) who completed follow-up reported visiting the control website at least once.

Measures

Demographic characteristics

Participants completed questions that included background and demographic data (i.e., age, ethnicity, education, skin type, previous tanning behavior, etc.). Skin type was assessed using the Fitzpatrick scale (Fitzpatrick, 1988).

Tanning motivation outcomes

Indoor tanning intentions

At baseline and follow up, participants reported their intentions to indoor tan on a 3-item scale (e.g., “Please tell us how strongly you INTEND to indoor tan in the next year.”) with response options ranging from definitely do not intend (1) to definitely do intend (7). The items were summed to create an index of intention to indoor tan (αbaseline = 0.95; αfollow-up = 0.97). The scale has a long history of use in the literature where it has demonstrated strong associations with future indoor tanning behavior (Hillhouse, Turrisi, Holwiski, & McVeigh, 1999; Noar et al., 2015; Stapleton et al., 2010)

Indoor tanning willingness

Assessment of behavioral willingness was adapted from items used by Gibbons, Gerrard, Blanton, & Russell (1998) adding a time frame. For indoor tanning willingness participants were instructed to “Imagine that in the next 12 months you had the opportunity to indoor tan.” Indoor tanning willingness used a 3-item scale (e.g., “Please tell us how WILLING you would be to indoor tan in the next year.”) with response options ranging from definitely not willing (1) to definitely willing (7). Items were summed to create an index of indoor tanning willingness (αbaseline = 0.96; αfollow-up = 0.98).

Sunless tanning willingness

Sunless tanning willingness was assessed with a single item, with options ranging from definitely not willing (1) to definitely willing (7). Participants were instructed to “Imagine that in the next 12 months you had the opportunity to sunless tan. Please tell us how WILLING you would be to sunless tan in the next year.” Responses were normally distributed with a baseline mean of 3.96 (SD = 2.15) and a follow-up mean of 3.60 (SD = 2.19).

Psychosocial intervening variables

Indoor tanning attitudes

Indoor tanning attitudes were measured with a 3-item short-form of an indoor tanning attitude scale (e.g., “I feel favorable about indoor tanning.”) with a long history in the literature (Hillhouse, Adler, Drinnon, & Turrisi, 1997; Stapleton et al., 2015). The short-form correlates 0.99 with the original 5-item measure. Participants indicated the extent to which they agreed (1 = Strongly Disagree; 5 = Strongly Agree) with the items, which were summed to create an index of positive indoor tanning attitudes (αbaseline = 0.94; αfollow-up = 0.96).

Normative beliefs

We assessed normative beliefs with two constructs from previous studies, injunctive and descriptive norms (Stapleton, Turrisi, & Hillhouse, 2008).

Injunctive norms

These are the perceptions of peer approval of indoor tanning, and were assessed with three items (e.g., “Girls at my school approve of me indoor tanning.”). These items were summed to create an injunctive norms index (αbaseline = 0.89; αfollow-up = 0.92).

Descriptive norms

These are the perceptions of the popularity of a behavior among one's peers, and were assessed with three items (e.g., “Tanning seems popular in girls my age.”), which were summed to form a descriptive norms index (αbaseline = 0.92; αfollow-up = 0.93). Response options for both scales ranged from Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (5).

Positive and negative expectancies

The positive expectancies scale was a 7-item measure of the beliefs about the attractiveness and relaxation benefits associated with indoor tanning. For example, participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they felt tanning made them feel more attractive (e.g., “I look more attractive when I have a nice tan.”) and was relaxing (e.g., “Indoor tanning is a stress-free way to relax.”). We measured negative expectancies using five items assessing perceived susceptibility to skin damage and skin cancer from tanning (e.g., “If I indoor tan regularly, I will increase my risk for skin cancer.”). For all expectancy measures, participants were instructed to “Imagine that you indoor tan regularly,” and response options ranged from Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (5). Items were summed to create separate scales of positive (αbaseline = 0.90; αfollow-up = 0.92) and negative (αbaseline = 0.92; αfollow-up = 0.95) expectancies. All these items have been previously used in the literature (Hillhouse et al., 2008; Turrisi et al., 2004).

Analytic Strategy

Baseline and attrition analyses

We conducted descriptive analyses of baseline demographic data across all subjects. In addition, independent group t-tests were used to compare baseline variables between treatment and control participants and between the two cohorts to check for cohort effects. To understand potential bias due to attrition, we used independent group t-tests to compare means of all study variables at baseline between study participants who completed the follow-up and those who did not. In addition we used independent group t-tests to look at differences between treatment and control participants who did not complete the follow-up assessment.

Direct intervention effects

Our analyses used an intent-to-treat approach. First, we used independent samples t-tests to examine baseline differences between treatment and control conditions on the outcome (indoor tanning intentions and willingness, sunless tanning willingness) and intervening (i.e., indoor tanning attitudes, indoor tanning normative beliefs, indoor tanning positive and negative expectancies) variables to confirm randomization was achieved. Next, we used multivariate linear regression to test the effect of the intervention on the post-test outcome variables (e.g., post-test indoor tanning intentions), controlling for baseline measures of the specific outcome measure (e.g., baseline indoor tanning intentions) and cohort membership. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's f2 (Selya, Rose, Dierker, Hedeker & Mermelstein, 2012)

Indirect intervention effects

We examined indirect mechanisms of change through intervening variables using the product of coefficients approach (McKinnon et al., 2002). Figure 3 presents the path diagram for the intervening variable model. This approach obtains an estimate of the indirect effect quantified as the product of α and β (Sobel, 1982). Separate models were estimated for each hypothesized intervening variable (attitudes, normative beliefs, and expectancies) assessed at post-test. To account for autoregressive effects and minimize biased estimates of the indirect effects (Mitchell & Maxwell, 2013), these models included the baseline measure of the specific intervening variable and the outcome variable. Cohort membership was included as a control variable. All models also included a direct path (c′) from intervention group to the specific outcome variable at post-test. Because the standard error of the αβ product term is not likely to be normally distributed (McKinnon et al., 2002), we used a bootstrapping method (with n = 5,000 bootstrap resamples) to test the significance of the indirect effect. Bootstrapping is a non-parametric procedure that produces an approximation of the sample distribution of the indirect effects and can be used to generate confidence intervals around point estimates in the model. In the present study, bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals of αβ were obtained. As recommended, estimates of indirect effects were considered significant if zero was not included within the confidence intervals (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Path diagram for the intervening variable model.

Missing values

Analyses were conducted using Mplus 6.1 (Muthen & Muthen, 2010), accounting for missing values using full information maximum likelihood (FIML). FIML addresses missing data by estimating parameter estimates and standard errors using all available information from partially missing cases and produces less biased and more efficient results than listwise deletion (Graham, 2009). Overall, we had 30.8% cases missing some data. However, most of these only had missing data on a small number of variables for a total of 3.3% missing data overall. FIML cannot account for missingness for cases when both the predictor and outcome are missing, leading to exclusion of between 2 and 9 cases across the various models.

Results

Baseline and Attrition Analyses

Average age of participants in the sample was 15.2 years old (sd = 1.91), and 39% reported previous use of indoor tanning. Analyses of baseline differences in outcome and intervening variables between intervention and control conditions revealed one significant difference for injunctive norms, t(261) = 1.95, p = .05. Control participants reported higher indoor tanning injunctive norms (M = 11.90, SD = 2.56) than intervention participants (M = 11.24, SD = 2.89). There were no other significant differences between the two groups on any of the variables at baseline, confirming randomization was achieved. There were also no differences between the two cohorts.

Analyses of baseline differences between participants who completed follow-up versus those who did not revealed no significant differences. Furthermore, examination of baseline differences in intervention participants who did not complete follow-up versus control participants who did not complete follow-up revealed no significant differences.

Direct Intervention Effects

Table 2 presents the model results predicting post-test indoor tanning intentions and willingness, and sunless tanning willingness for the intent-to-treat analyses. These residualized change models controlled for baseline measures of the specific outcome measure (i.e., baseline indoor tanning intentions and willingness, sunless tanning willingness). For the intent-to-treat analyses, participants in the intervention group reported significantly lower tanning intentions (b = −1.54, p < 0.01) and willingness to indoor tan (b = −1.34, p < 0.01) at follow-up, compared to control group participants. The intervention also had a marginally significant effect on predicting changes in sunless tanning willingness. Controlling for baseline levels, members of the intervention group reported higher levels of sunless tanning willingness at the post-test assessment, relative to members of the control condition (b = 0.37, p < 0.10). Intervention effect sizes were 0.29, 0.23, and 0.19 for indoor tanning intentions and willingness, and sunless tanning willingness, respectively.

Table 2.

Intent-to-Treat linear regression models predicting intervention effects.

Indoor Tanning Intentions (N = 443) Indoor Tanning Willingness (N = 443) Sunless Tanning Willingness (N = 443)
b se t b se t b se t
Baseline 0.47*** 0.05 9.99 0.51*** 0.05 10.33 0.41*** 0.05 8.31
Cohort −0.67 0.56 −1.20 −0.14 0.61 −0.23 −0.18 0.20 −0.91
Condition (Treatment Group = 1) −1.54** 0.55 −2.81 −1.34* 0.61 −2.21 0.37+ 0.20 1.83
Effect size 0.29 0.23 0.19

Note: Baseline refers to baseline (W1) score of each outcome

+

p < 0.10

*

p < 0.05

**

p < .01

***

p < 0.001

Effect sizes calculated using Cohen's f2

Indirect Intervention Effects

Intervention effects on the intervening variables (α paths)

After controlling for baseline variable levels, examination of α paths revealed significant intervention effects in their expected directions on attitudes (b = −0.86, p < 0.05), injunctive norms (b = −0.77, p < 0.05) and negative expectancies (b = 1.04, p < 0.05). Thus, being in the intervention group was associated with less positive attitudes toward indoor tanning, lower perceptions of peer approval, and increased negative expectancies regarding skin damage and cancer that could result from indoor tanning. The effects of the intervention on post-test assessments of descriptive norms and positive expectancies were not significant. Path coefficients can be found in Table 3.

Table 3.

Linear regression models testing indirect effects of intervention group on post-test outcomes.

α Path (Group → Mediator) β Path (Mediator → Outcome) C' Path (Group → Outcome) Indirect Effect (αβ)
b se t b se t b se t EST se t
Attitudes
Positive Attitudes −0.86* 0.35 −2.46 1.11*** 0.06 19.68 −0.44 0.38 −1.16 −0.96* 0.39 −2.44
1.37*** 0.04 32.41 −0.22 0.36 −0.61 −1.18* 0.48 −2.45
Normative Beliefs
Injunctive Norms −0.77* 0.31 −2.50 0.86*** 0.07 12.20 −0.94+ 0.49 −1.93 −0.66* 0.27 −2.44
0.98*** 0.09 11.25 −0.82 0.53 −1.54 −0.75* 0.30 2.49
Descriptive Norms −0.06 0.27 −0.23 0.37*** 0.09 4.17 −1.36* 0.55 −2.51 −0.02 0.10 −0.22
0.49*** 0.10 5.13 −1.28* 0.60 −2.13 −0.03 0.13 −0.22
Expectancy Beliefs
Positive Expectancies −1.02 0.64 −1.59 0.47*** 0.03 13.74 −0.85+ 0.46 −1.85 −0.48 0.31 −1.57
0.59*** 0.03 18.41 −0.68 0.48 −1.41 −0.60 0.38 −1.58
Negative Expectancies 1.04* 0.47 2.18 −0.42*** 0.07 −6.25 −0.99+ 0.52 −1.89 −0.43* 0.21 −2.02
−0.48*** 0.07 −6.68 −0.86 0.58 −1.49 −0.50* 0.24 −2.07

Note: EST = Sobel's

+

p < 0.10

*

p < 0.05

** p < .01

***

p < 0.001

In the β Path column, the first row under each variable is intentions to indoor tan, followed by willingness

Effects of intervening variables on the outcomes (β paths)

All examined β paths were significant in their expected directions (see Table 3). Attitudes, injunctive and descriptive norms, and positive outcome expectancies were positively associated with intentions and willingness to indoor tan. Negative expectancies were negatively associated with intentions and willingness to indoor tan. Thus, the more susceptible an individual felt she was to skin damage and cancer, the lower her intentions and willingness to indoor tan.

Indirect effects (αβ)

Using a bootstrapped 95% confidence interval based on 5,000 samples, the indirect path coefficient (αβ) from intervention group to indoor tanning intentions was significant for three of the proposed intervening variables: positive attitudes (Sobel's z = −0.96, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −1.72; −0.17), injunctive norms (Sobel's z = −0.66, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −1.22; −0.16), and negative expectancies (Sobel's z = −0.43, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −0.91; −0.07). Similarly, the indirect path coefficient (αβ) from intervention group to indoor tanning willingness was significant for positive attitudes (Sobel's z = −1.18, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −2.42; −0.21), injunctive norms (b = −0.75, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −1.37; −0.19), and negative expectancies (Sobel's z = −0.50, p < 0.05, 95% CI: −1.04; −0.08). The 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effects of the other intervening variables all contained zero and were thus judged to be non-significant.

Discussion

This study demonstrates an appearance-based skin cancer prevention approach previously successful in adults can be efficacious with adolescents. We enrolled a national sample of female adolescents who indicated strong motivations toward future indoor tanning into a randomized controlled trial. The teens who received the intervention had reduced indoor tanning intentions and willingness and increased willingness to try sunless tanning relative to control teens who viewed a teen-oriented alcohol prevention website (Partnership for Drug-Free Kids, 2015).

Reducing teen access to and motivations for indoor tanning will be critical to reducing future indoor tanning behavior. Decreasing indoor tanning behavior will be essential in achieving the Surgeon General's goals for reducing melanoma. Impacting tanning in teens is of particular importance. Previous research indicates that the younger someone initiates indoor tanning, the more likely they are to become a frequent habitual tanner and to develop melanoma (Baker et al., 2010; Cust, et al., 2011). Reducing both access, through legislation, and motivations, through prevention interventions such as this one will ensure that teens don't initiate indoor tanning early. Reducing future motivations to tan will also help counteract the sharp rise in indoor tanning that occurs when adolescents obtain legal adulthood and are beyond the reach of current legislation. The tanning industry is poised to take advantage of the current limits of indoor tanning legislation with a strong presence around US college and university campuses and other places young adults are found (Boyers et al., 2014; Pagoto et al., 2015).

The intervention was grounded in an established health behavior change model, the behavioral alternatives model (Jaccard, 1981) and included the construct of willingness (Gibbons, Gerrard, & Lane, 2009). Health risk behaviors like indoor tanning are typically engaged in to fulfill important personal goals such as improving appearance (Aarts, 2007). By providing a healthier alternative means to achieve these personal goals, the behavioral alternatives model increases the likelihood adolescents will be motivated to change their behavior. Since the original goal continues to be met by the alternative, the adolescent should also be less likely to return to the unhealthy behavior. Therefore, adopting the healthy alternative should increase the sustainability of the intervention.

This intervention presented sunless tanning as an alternative to indoor tanning. The results indicated that, after the intervention, treatment participants seemed more willing to try sunless tanning than control participants. Sunless tanning was presented as a reasonable alternative since it shares many of the advantages of indoor tanning (e.g., ease, doesn't require a change in fashion style, etc.) without the harmful UV radiation. Besides, many of the salons that teens use already offer sunless services. There is also some evidence that sunless tanning may serve as a transition from indoor tanning to ceasing tanning altogether (Mahoney et al., 2012; Quinn et al., 2015; Russo, Van Acker, Vander Wal, & Sinha, 2012).

The guiding model predicts that the intervention will exert its impact on indoor tanning motivations indirectly through the website modules’ effects on indoor tanning beliefs and attitudes. As predicted, intervention participants relative to controls reported less favorable attitudes toward indoor tanning, lower perceptions of peer approval of indoor tanning, and greater negative expectancies of indoor tanning outcomes. Of these, changes in indoor tanning attitudes, and perceived peer approval of indoor tanning demonstrated indirect mechanisms of change for the impact of the intervention on indoor tanning willingness and intentions. Therefore, the intervention worked as expected, and similarly to previous efforts with young adults (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008).

This study reinforces previous evidence that focusing on the effects of behavior salient to the target audience, such as appearance damage in young women, can lead to engagement and changes in future behavioral motivations. Appearance issues are of particular importance to female teens, with the likelihood that they are even more central for those interested in indoor tanning. Focusing on appearance problems associated with tanning behavior provides an excellent means of getting and maintaining teens’ attention.

There is a small but growing literature focused on prevention of indoor tanning motivations and behavior in adolescent populations. For example, pilot work by Lazovich et al. (2013) focused on intervening with both parents and teens, which had positive effects on mothers’ knowledge and teens’ future intentions. While only a pilot study, this parent-based approach is promising. Aarestrup et al. (2014), using a school-based approach and a health-oriented e-magazine, found reductions in teen behavior but not future motivations. The current results extend these findings by using a randomized controlled trial to demonstrate the utility of a theoretically-grounded appearance-focused approach in adolescents.

The study does have several limitations that need to be considered. First, participants only came to the intervention because they were part of a study that directed them there. We do not know how it would be received or consumed in real world settings. Additionally, the website which was cutting edge when the study was initiated, has now been supplanted by newer social media technologies which are more heavily utilized by adolescents. Also, the intervention only assessed the effects of one possible alternative, sunless tanning. Many other alternatives exist with the possibility that tailoring the alternative to the individual's primary goals of tanning will improve efficacy with some individuals. Lastly, this study focused exclusively on indoor tanning. Outdoor tanning could possibly be substituted for indoor tanning in these teens. Future work needs to broaden the focus to tanning and sun exposure in general.

Despite these limitations, this study has implications for future research, practice and policy. The success of the approach in this efficacy study indicates it should be refined for wider dissemination. For example, translating the messages into content appropriate for social media, which is very popular with adolescents, could increase the ability to successfully impact future skin cancer morbidity and mortality. Also, recent work suggests teen tanners can be categorized into patterns that have different implications for initiation, behavior, and intervention (Hillhouse, Turrisi et al., 2016). Finding ways to match these prevention messages to specific indoor tanning subpopulations could prove to have beneficial effects in this population. It is clear that policy initiatives restricting minor access to tanning beds are vital for reducing melanoma risk. It is equally clear that interventions designed to impact tanning motivations such as in this study will also be critical in achieving this goal.

Acknowledgments

Research reported in this manuscript was supported by the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health under award number R01CA134891. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aarestrup C, Bonnesen CT, Thygesen LC, Krarup AF, Waagstein AB, Jensen PD, Bentzen J. The effect of a school-based intervention on sunbed use in Danish pupils at continuation schools: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;54:214–220. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.011. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aarts H. Health and goal-directed behavior: The nonconscious regulation and motivation of goals and their pursuit. Health Psychology Review. 2007;1:53–82. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker MK, Hillhouse J, Liu XF. The effect of initial indoor tanning with mother on current tanning patterns. Archives of Dermatology. 2010;146:1427–1428. doi: 10.1001/archdermatol.2010.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boniol M, Autier P, Boyle P, Gandini S. Cutaneous melanoma attributable to sunbed use: systematic review and meta-analysis. British Medical Journal. 2012;345(jul24 2):e4757–e4757. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e4757. doi:10.1136/bmj.e4757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boyers L, Karimkhani C, Crane LA, Asdigian N, Hollonds A, Dellavalle RP. Buying indoor tanning with university debit cards. JAMA Dermatol. 2014;71(1):199–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2014.02.041. doi:10.1016/j.jaad.2014.02.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cust AE, Armstrong BK, Goumas C, Jenkins MA, Schmid H, Hopper JL, Mann GJ. Sunbed use during adolescence and early adulthood is associated with increased risk of early-onset melanoma. International Journal of Cancer. 2011;128:2425–2435. doi: 10.1002/ijc.25576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Danoff-Burg S, Mosher C. Predictors of Tanning Salon Use: Behavioral Alternatives for Enhancing Appearance, Relaxing and Socializing. J Health Psychol. 2006;11(3):511–518. doi: 10.1177/1359105306063325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Addison-Wesley; Reading, MA: 1975. [Google Scholar]
  9. Fitzpatrick TB. The validity and practicality of sun-reactive skin types I through VI. Archives of Dermatology. 1988;124:869–871. doi: 10.1001/archderm.124.6.869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gerrard M, Gibbons F, Houlihan A, Stock M, Pomery E. A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model. Developmental Review. 2008;28:29–61. [Google Scholar]
  11. GfK. GfK KnowledgePanel Designed Summary. 2015 Retrieved from www.knowledgenetworks.com/ganp/docs/KnowledgePanel(R)-Design-Summary.pdf.
  12. Gibbons FX, Gerrard M. Predicting young adults’ health risk behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1995;69:505–517. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.3.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Blanton H, Russell DW. Reasoned action and social reaction: Willingness and intention as independent predictors of health risk. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1998;74:1164–1180. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Lane DJ. Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness. Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2009. A Social Reaction Model of Adolescent Health Risk. pp. 107–136. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Lane DJ, Mahler HIM, Kulik JA. Using UV photography to reduce use of tanning booths: A test of cognitive mediation. Health Psychology. 2005;24:358–363. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.358. doi:Doi 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Graham JW. Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:549–576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Guy GP, Jr., Berkowitz Z, Everett Jones S, Holman DM, Garnett E, Watson M. Trends in indoor tanning among US high school students, 2009-2013. JAMA Dermatology. 2015;151:448–450. doi: 10.1001/jamadermatol.2014.4677. doi:10.1001/jamadermatol.2014.4677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Guy GP, Jr., Berkowitz Z, Jones SE, Olsen EO, Miyamoto JN, Michael SL, Saraiya M. State indoor tanning laws and adolescent indoor tanning. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104:e69–74. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301850. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2013.301850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Guy GP, Jr., Tai E, Richardson LC. Use of indoor tanning devices by high school students in the United States, 2009. Preventing Chronic Disease. 2011;8:A116. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hillhouse J, Adler C, Drinnon J, Turrisi R. Application of Azjen's Theory of Planned Behavior to Predict Sunbathing, Tanning Salon Use, and Sunscreen Use Intentions and Behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1997;20:365–378. doi: 10.1023/a:1025517130513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hillhouse J, Stapleton J, Florence C, Pagoto S. Prevalence And Correlates Of Indoor Tanning In Non-Salon Locations Among A National Sample Of Young Women. JAMA-Dermatology. 2015;151:1134–1136. doi: 10.1001/jamadermatol.2015.1323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hillhouse J, Turrisi R. Examination of the efficacy of an appearance-focused intervention to reduce UV exposure. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2002;25:395–409. doi: 10.1023/a:1015870516460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hillhouse J, Turrisi R, Cleveland MJ, Scaglione NM, Baker K, Florence LC. Theory-driven longitudinal study exploring indoor tanning initiation in teens using a person-centered approach. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2016;50(1):48–57. doi: 10.1007/s12160-015-9731-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hillhouse J, Turrisi R, Holwiski F, McVeigh S. An examination of psychological variables relevant to artificial tanning tendencies. Journal of Health Psychology. 1999;4:507–516. doi: 10.1177/135910539900400405. doi:10.1177/135910539900400405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hillhouse J, Turrisi R, Stapleton J, Robinson J. A randomized controlled trial of an appearance-focused intervention to prevent skin cancer. Cancer. 2008;113:3257–3266. doi: 10.1002/cncr.23922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jaccard J. Attitudes and behavior: Implications for attitudes toward behavioral alternatives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1981;17:286–307. [Google Scholar]
  27. Jaccard J. The Reasoned Action Model: Directions for future research. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 2012;64:50–80. [Google Scholar]
  28. Jaccard J, Wood G. An idiothetic analysis of consumer decision making. In: Brinberg D, Lutz R, editors. Perspectives on methodology in consumer research. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lazovich D, Choi K, Rolnick C, Jackson JM, Forster J, Southwell B. An intervention to decrease adolescent indoor tanning: a multi-method pilot study. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;52(5 Suppl):S76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.08.009. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mahoney A, Swetter SM, Biello KB, Resnick EA, Feuerstein I, Geller AC. Attitudes toward indoor tanning among users of sunless tanning products. Archives of Dermatology. 2012;148:124–126. doi: 10.1001/archderm.148.1.123. doi:10.1001/archderm.148.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mitchell MA, Maxwell SE. A Comparison of the cross-sectional and sequential designs when assessing longitudinal mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2013;48(3):301–339. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2013.784696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide. 6th ed. Muthén & Muthén; Los Angeles, CA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  34. National Cancer Institute SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2012, National Cancer Institute based on November 2014 SEER data submission. 2015 Retrieved from http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2012/
  35. Noar SM, Myrick JG, Zeitany A, Kelley D, Morales-Pico B, Thomas NE. Testing a social cognitive theory-based model of indoor tanning: implications for skin cancer prevention messages. Health Communication. 2015;30:164–174. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2014.974125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pagoto SL, Lemon SC, Oleski JL, Scully JM, Olendzki GF, Evans MM, Hillhouse J. Availability of tanning beds on US college campuses. JAMA Dermatology. 2015;151:59–63. doi: 10.1001/jamadermatol.2014.3590. doi:10.1001/jamadermatol.2014.3590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pan M, Geller L. Update on indoor tanning legislation in the United States. Clinics in Dermatology. 2015;33:387–392. doi: 10.1016/j.clindermatol.2014.12.016. doi:10.1016/j.clindermatol.2014.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Partnership for Drug-Free Kids. Above the Influence. 2015 Retrieved from http://abovetheinfluence.com/
  39. Pomery EA, Gibbons FX, Reis-Bergan M, Gerrard M. From willingness to intention: Experience moderates the shift from reactive to reasoned behavior. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2009;35:894–908. doi: 10.1177/0146167209335166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;403:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Quinn M, Alamian A, Hillhouse J, Scott C, Turrisi R, Baker K. Prevalence and correlates of indoor tanning and sunless tanning product use among female teens in the United States. Preventive Medicine Reports. 2015;2:40–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2014.12.004. doi:10.1016/j.pmedr.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Radecki CM, Jaccard J. Psychological aspects of organ donation: a critical review and synthesis of individual and next-of-kin donation decisions. Health Psychol. 1997;16(2):183–195. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.16.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Russo VA, Van Acker MM, Vander Wal JS, Sinha AA. Patterns of use of sunless tanning product alternatives to indoor tanning among female college students. Archives of Dermatology. 2012;148:855–857. doi: 10.1001/archdermatol.2012.491. doi:10.1001/archdermatol.2012.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A practical guide to calculating Cohen's f(2), a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sobel Michael E. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology. 1982;13:290–312. [Google Scholar]
  46. Stapleton JL, Manne SL, Darabos K, Greene K, Ray AE, Turner AL, Coups EJ. Randomized controlled trial of a web-based indoor tanning intervention: Acceptability and preliminary outcomes. Health Psychology. 2015;34(Suppl):1278–1285. doi: 10.1037/hea0000254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Stapleton J, Turrisi R, Hillhouse J. Peer crowd identification and indoor artificial UV tanning behavioral tendencies. Journal of Health Psychology. 2008;13:940–945. doi: 10.1177/1359105308095068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stapleton J, Turrisi R, Hillhouse J, Robinson JK, Abar B. A comparison of the efficacy of an appearance-focused skin cancer intervention within indoor tanner subgroups identified by latent profile analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2010;33:181–190. doi: 10.1007/s10865-009-9246-z. doi:10.1007/s10865-009-9246-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Turrisi R, Abar C, Mallett KA, Jaccard J. An examination of the mediational effects of cognitive and attitudinal factors of a parent intervention to reduce college drinking. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2010;40(10):2500–2526. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00668.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Turrisi R, Hillhouse J, Gebert C, Grimes J. Examination of cognitive variables relevant to sunscreen use. J Behav Med. 1999;22(5):493–509. doi: 10.1023/a:1018609524523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Turrisi R, Hillhouse J, Heavin S, Robinson J, Adams M, Berry J. Examination of the short-term efficacy of a parent-based intervention to prevent skin cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2004;27:393–412. doi: 10.1023/b:jobm.0000042412.53765.06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. US Department of Health and Human Services The Surgeon General's Call to Action to Prevent Skin Cancer. 2014 Retrieved from Washington, DC: http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/calls/prevent-skin-cancer/
  53. Wehner MR, Chren MM, Nameth D, Choudhry A, Gaskins M, Nead KT, Linos E. International prevalence of indoor tanning: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Dermatology. 2014;150:390–400. doi: 10.1001/jamadermatol.2013.6896. doi:10.1001/jamadermatol.2013.6896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES