Abstract
Rationale
Acute antipsychotic treatment disrupts conditioned avoidance responding, and repeated treatment induces a sensitization- or tolerance-like effect. However, the neurochemical mechanisms underlying both acute and repeated antipsychotic effects remain to be determined.
Objective
The present study examined the neuroreceptor mechanisms of haloperidol, clozapine, and olanzapine effect in a rat two-way conditioned avoidance model.
Methods
Well-trained Sprague–Dawley rats were administered with haloperidol (0.05 mg/kg, sc), clozapine (10.0 mg/kg, sc), or olanzapine (1.0 mg/kg, sc) together with either saline, quinpirole (a selective dopamine D2/3 agonist, 1.0 mg/kg, sc), or 2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodo-amphetamine (DOI; a selective 5-HT2A/2C agonist, 2.5 mg/kg, sc), and their conditioned avoidance responses were tested over 3 days. After 2 days of drug-free retraining, the repeated treatment effect was assessed in a challenge test.
Results
Pretreatment of quinpirole, but not DOI, attenuated the acute haloperidol-induced disruption of avoidance responding and to a lesser extent, olanzapine-induced disruption. In contrast, pretreatment of DOI, but not quinpirole, attenuated the acute effect of clozapine. On the repeated effect, pretreatment of DOI, but not quinpirole, attenuated the potentiated disruption of haloperidol, whereas pretreatment of quinpirole attenuated the potentiated disruption of olanzapine but enhanced the tolerance-like effect of clozapine.
Conclusions
These findings suggest that acute haloperidol and olanzapine disrupt avoidance responding primarily by blocking dopamine D2 receptors, whereas acute clozapine exerts its disruptive effect primarily by blocking the 5-HT2A receptors. The repeated haloperidol effect may be mediated by 5-HT2A/2C blockade-initiated neural processes, whereas the repeated clozapine and olanzapine effect may be mediated by D2/3 blockade-initiated neural processes.
Keywords: Haloperidol; Clozapine; Olanzapine; Quinpirole; 2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodo-amphetamine; Repeated antipsychotic treatment; Conditioned avoidance response; Sensitization; Tolerance
Introduction
The conditioned avoidance response model (CAR) is a fearmotivated instrumental conditioning model which is traditionally used in behavioral pharmacology as a preclinical screen for antipsychotic activity (Bolles 1970; Levis and Brewer 2001; Rescorla and Solomon 1967). In this model, acute treatment of antipsychotic drugs selectively disrupts avoidance responding without altering unconditioned escape response (Arnt 1982; Wadenberg et al. 2001b). Recently, we have expanded the use of this model to identify the behavioral mechanisms of action of antipsychotic drugs (Li et al. 2007; Li et al. 2009a,b; Mead and Li 2009) and to examine the anxiolytic property of atypical antipsychotic drugs (Mead et al. 2008). It is suggested that understanding the nature of the avoidance deficit induced by antipsychotics may shed light on how antipsychotic drugs achieve their clinical efficacy in the treatment of psychosis (Li et al. 2009b).
Although the avoidance disruptive effect of antipsychotic treatment is robust and well-documented, the neuroreceptor basis of this action is less clear. It has also not been determined whether typical and atypical antipsychotics differ in their neurochemical mechanisms in this model (Wadenberg and Hicks 1999). For typical antipsychotics such as haloperidol (HAL), it is generally assumed (but not proven) that they disrupt avoidance behavior by blocking dopamine D2 receptors because they are primarily dopamine D2 antagonists (Dragunow et al. 1990; Seeman et al. 1976). In contrast, atypical antipsychotics (e.g., clozapine, olanzapine) have multiple-receptor binding sites (Meltzer et al. 1989; Miyamoto et al. 2005), making it more difficult to pinpoint their exact neurochemical mechanisms relevant to their CAR effects. Both clozapine (CLZ) and olanzapine (OLZ) possess a much more potent antagonism on the 5-HT2A/2C receptor in addition to relatively weak antagonism on D2 receptor (Meltzer et al. 2003). It is thus possible that their disruptive effect on CAR could be attributed to their action on D2 receptor alone (Kapur and Seeman 2001; Wadenberg et al. 2001b) or its dual action on both 5-HT2A/2C and D2 receptor (Meltzer et al. 1989) or even effects on other receptors (e.g. D1, D4, 5-HT1A).
In the present study, we took a pharmacological approach to delineate the neuroreceptor mechanisms of HAL, CLZ, and OLZ in a two-way CAR model. We examined how the avoidance disruptive effect of these drugs was affected by pretreatments of quinpirole (QUI), a selective D2/D3 dopaminergic receptor agonist and/or 2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodo-amphetamine (DOI), a selective 5-HT2A/ 2C serotonergic receptor agonist. We recently applied a similar approach in a rat maternal behavior model and found that pretreatment of QUI, but not DOI, dose-dependently reversed the HAL-induced disruptions on active maternal responses, whereas pretreatment of DOI, but not QUI, dose-dependently reversed the CLZ-induced disruptions (Zhao and Li 2009). Based on these findings and the receptor binding profiles of each antipsychotic (Miyamoto et al. 2005), we hypothesized that QUI, but not DOI, is able to attenuate the HAL-induced disruption on CAR and may also be effective in alleviating the OLZ-induced disruption to some extent. In contrast, DOI, but not QUI, is able to attenuate the CLZ-induced disruption on CAR and may also be effective in alleviating the OLZ-induced disruption to some extent. One additional experiment addressed the specificity of the reversal effects of QUI and DOI on avoidance responding and its relation to their psychomotor stimulating effects.
In our previous studies (Li et al. 2007, 2009a,b; Mead and Li 2009), we found that repeated antipsychotic treatment can produce a long-term change in its disruptive effect on avoidance responding. For example, rats previously treated with HAL and OLZ made significantly less avoidances than those who were treated with these drugs for the first time (Mead and Li 2009). This finding indicates that repeated antipsychotic treatment may induce a sensitization-like effect. However, we know almost nothing about the neuro-receptor mechanisms underlying this repeated drug effect in the avoidance conditioning model. In the present study, we also examined how this long-term repeated effect was affected by pretreatments of QUI and DOI to potentially elucidate its mechanisms.
Materials and methods
Animals
Male Sprague–Dawley rats (226–250 g upon arrival, Charles River, Portage, MI) were housed two per cage, in 48.3×26.7×20.3 cm transparent polycarbonate cages under 12-h light/dark conditions (light on between 6:30 a.m. and 6:30 p.m.). Room temperature was maintained at 22±1° with a relative humidity of 55–60%. Food and water were available ad libitum. Animals were allowed at least 1 week of habituation to the animal facility before being used in experiments. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln.
Two-way avoidance conditioning apparatus
Eight identical two-way shuttle boxes custom designed and manufactured by Med Associates (St. Albans, VT) were used. Each box was housed in a ventilated, sound-insulated isolation cubicle (96.52 cmW×35.56 cm D× 63.5 cmH). Each box was 64 cm long, 30 cm high (from grid floor), and 24 cm wide, and was divided into two equal-sized compartments by a partition with an arch style doorway (15 cm high×9 cm wide at base). A barrier (4 cm high) was placed between the two compartments, so the rats had to jump from one compartment to the other. The grid floor consisted of 40 stainless-steel rods with a diameter of 0.48 cm, spaced 1.6 cm apart center to center, through which a scrambled footshock (US, 0.8 mA, maximum duration: 5 s) was delivered by a constant current shock generator (Model ENV-410B) and scrambler (Model ENV-412). The rat location, motor activity (photobeam breaks), and crossings between compartments were monitored by a set of 16 photobeams (ENV-256-8P) affixed at the bottom of the box (3.5 cm above the grid floor). Illumination was provided by two houselights mounted at the top of each compartment. The CS (i.e., 76 dB white noise) was produced by a speaker (ENV 224 AMX) mounted on the ceiling of the cubicle, centered above the shuttle box. Background noise (approximately 74 dB) was provided by a ventilation fan affixed at the top corner of each isolation cubicle. All training and testing procedures were controlled by Med Associates programs running on a computer.
Drugs
The injection solution of HAL (5.0 mg/ml ampoules, Shanghai Xudong Haipu Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Shanghai, China) was obtained by mixing drugs with sterile water. CLZ and OLZ (gifts from NIMH drug supply program) were dissolved in 1.0% glacial acetic acid in distilled water. QUI and DOI (RBI-Sigma, Natick, MA) were dissolved in 0.9% saline. All drugs were administered subcutaneously in a volume of 1.0 ml/kg body weight. Choices of drug doses for HAL, CLZ, and OLZ were based on our previous studies showing that at the chosen doses, all three drugs produce a reliable and comparable disruption of avoidance responding (Li et al. 2004; Li et al. 2009a,b; Mead and Li 2009), and they give rise to clinical levels of striatal D2 occupancy (50–80%) at these doses (Kapur et al. 2003). The doses of QUI and DOI were chosen based on our recent maternal behavior work showing that QUI 1.0 mg/kg was effective in reversing the HAL-induced disruptions of active maternal behaviors and that DOI 2.5 mg/kg was effective in reversing the CLZ-induced disruptions of maternal behaviors (Zhao and Li 2009). Previous work also showed that DOI produces maximal behavioral effects between 2 and 3 mg/kg (Granoff and Ashby 1998; Halberstadt et al. 2009; Schreiber et al. 1995).
Experiment 1: effects of QUI pretreatment on HAL- and CLZ-induced avoidance disruption
Sixty rats were first habituated to the two-way CAR boxes for 2 days (20 min/day). Then, they were trained for conditioned avoidance responding for ten sessions over a 2-week period. Each session consisted of 30 trials. Every trial started by presenting the white noise (CS) for 10 s, followed by a continuous scrambled footshock (0.8 mA, US, maximum duration=5 s) on the grid floor. If a subject moved from one compartment into the other within the 10 s of CS presentation, it avoided the shock and this shuttling response was recorded as avoidance. If the rat remained in the same compartment for more than 10 s and made a crossing upon receiving the footshock, this response was recorded as escape. If the rat did not respond during the entire 5 s presentation of the shock, the trial was terminated and escape failure was recorded. Intertrial intervals varied randomly between 30 and 60 s. The number of avoidance responses was recorded.
At the end of the training session, 46 rats reached the training criterion (>70% avoidance in each of the last two sessions. Their averaged number of avoidances on the last training day (mean)=29.17, SE=0.18). They were first matched on avoidance performance on the last training day (i.e., predrug) to create blocks of rats (n=5–6 rats/block) that were approximately equal in performance. Within each block, they were then randomly assigned to one of five groups: VEH+VEH (n=10, saline+sterile water), VEH+ HAL (n=9), VEH+CLZ (n=9), QUI+HAL (n=9), and QUI+CLZ (n=9), and repeatedly tested daily under the CS-only (no shock, 30 trials/session) condition for three consecutive days. The CS-only condition was used to control the possible confound of number of shocks received and to exclude any possible relearning effect caused by the presence of the US. During each test, rats were first pretreated with QUI 1.0 mg/kg (sc) or saline followed by an injection of sterile water, HAL 0.05 mg/kg (sc) or CLZ 10.0 mg/kg (sc) 10 min later. Thirty minutes after the second injection, rats were placed in the CAR boxes and tested. One day after the end of the 3rd test, all rats were tested drug-free for one session under the CS-only (no shock) condition and retrained for one session under the CS–US condition to bring their avoidance back to the predrug level. A final drug challenge test was conducted 24 h after the retraining session to assess the long-term effect of repeated antipsychotic treatment on avoidance. During the test, rats in the VEH+HAL and QUI+HAL groups were injected with HAL 0.025 mg/kg, whereas rats in the VEH+CLZ and QUI+CLZ groups were injected with CLZ 5.0 mg/kg. Half of the VEH+VEH rats (n=5) were injected with HAL 0.025 mg/kg and another half (n= 5) were injected with CLZ 5.0 mg/kg. The CS-only test (no US, 30 trials) was conducted 30 min after the drug injection.
Experiment 2: effects of DOI pretreatment on HAL- and CLZ-induced avoidance disruption
The basic procedure was identical to that of experiment 1 with the exception that DOI (2.5 mg/kg, sc) pretreatment effect was examined. Sixty rats were used, of which 45 reached the training criterion (>70% avoidance in each of the last two sessions, mean=29.16, SE=0.23). Following the group assigning procedure as described in experiment 1, they were allocated to the following five groups: VEH+ VEH (saline+sterile water), VEH+HAL (0.05 mg/kg), VEH+CLZ (10.0 mg/kg), DOI+HAL, and DOI+CLZ, and were subjected to three sessions of drug testing and two sessions of drug-free testing/retraining and a final drug challenge test, following the exact same schedule as described in experiment 1. During the challenge test, four VEH+VEH rats were injected with HAL 0.025 mg/kg and five were injected with CLZ 5.0 mg/kg.
Experiment 3: effects of QUI and DOI pretreatment on HAL and CLZ suppressive effect on motor activity in the CAR
This experiment was designed to examine the specificity of the pretreatment effects of QUI and DOI on antipsychotic-induced avoidance disruption. We asked whether the reversal effects could be attributed to the drug’s effects on motor activity. The basic procedure was similar to that used in the above experiments except that on the three drug days, rats were only tested for their motor activity in the CAR boxes, not for their avoidances (i.e., no CS or US was presented). Forty-eight rats were trained in ten sessions, of which 34 reached learning criterion (mean=27.88, SE=0.41). Following the group assigning procedure as described in experiment 1, they were allocated to the following five groups: VEH +VEH (n=10, saline+sterile water), VEH+HAL (n=6), QUI+HAL (n=6), VEH+CLZ (n=6), and DOI+CLZ (n= 6). Over the next 3 days, 30 min after the second injection, they were placed in the CAR boxes and motor activity was recorded for 30 min (the approximate time required to complete one CAR session). The number of crossings between two compartments was also recorded.
Experiment 4: effects of QUI and DOI pretreatment on OLZ-induced avoidance disruption
This experiment examined the receptor mechanisms of acute and repeated effects of OLZ. The basic procedure was identical to that of experiments 1 and 2 with the exception that both QUI (1.0 mg/kg) and DOI (2.5 mg/ kg) pretreatment effects were examined against OLZ (1.0 mg/kg, sc). Sixty rats were trained in ten sessions, of which 42 reached the training criterion (mean=28.60, SE=0.31). Following the group assigning procedure as described in experiment 1, they were allocated to the following five groups: VEH+VEH (n=8, saline+sterile water), VEH+OLZ (n=9), QUI+OLZ (n=8), DOI+OLZ (n=8), and QUI+DOI+OLZ (n=9), and were subjected to the three sessions of drug testing and two sessions of drug-free testing/retraining and a final drug challenge test. During the three drug testing sessions, rats in the QUI+DOI+ OLZ group were injected with QUI and DOI first, followed by an injection of OLZ 10 min later. During the drug challenge test, all rats were injected with OLZ at 0.5 mg/kg.
Statistical analysis
The number of avoidance responses and motor activity data (number of photobeam breaks) were expressed as mean± SEM. Data on the three drug test sessions were analyzed using a split-plot analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the between-subjects factor being drug group and the within-subjects factor being test session. To identify group difference on a specific test day, one-way ANOVAs followed by post-hoc Tukey’s HSD tests (for more than three groups) were used. Independent samples t tests were used to examine two-group difference in cases where there was a prior research hypothesis.
Results
Experiment 1: effects of QUI pretreatment on HAL- and CLZ-induced avoidance disruption
One rat from the QUI+CLZ group died unexpectedly before the retraining and drug challenge test. Its data on these two sessions were not analyzed. As can be seen in Fig. 1, HAL and CLZ suppressed avoidance responding on the first drug day and maintained this suppression over the 3 days in comparison to the vehicle treatment. A split-plot ANOVA revealed a main effect of drug treatment (F(4,41)=50.271, P<0.001) and a significant treatment × session interaction (F(8,82)= 4.626, P< 0.001), but no significant main effect of session (F(2,82)= 2.350, P=0.102). One-way ANOVAs followed by post-hoc tests on each drug test day revealed that all drug groups differed significantly from the VEH+VEH group (P<0.001). Post-hoc analysis showed that on the 1st drug day, the QUI+HAL group made significantly more avoidances than the VEH+HAL group (P=0.021), whereas the QUI+CLZ group did not differ significantly from the VEH+CLZ group (P=0.991), suggesting that pretreatment of QUI attenuated the HAL-induced disruption of avoidance responding on the 1st day, but did not affect the CLZ-induced disruption. This attenuation effect was no longer present on the subsequent two drug days (all P>0.10).
To detail the time course of the attenuation effect of QUI, we examined the within-session change of avoidance responding across the three ten-trial blocks on day 1 (data not shown). QUI completely reversed the effect of HAL in the 1st block, but lost its effect in the latter blocks (1st block, VEH+VEH vs. QUI+HAL, P=0.611; QUI+HAL vs. VEH+HAL, P=0.010. Second and third blocks, VEH+ VEH vs. QUI+HAL, P>0.13), a finding consistent with QUI’s relatively short duration of action (Whitaker and Lindstrom 1987).
HAL- and CLZ-treated rats reinstated their avoidance responding in just two sessions when the drug treatments were stopped to a level that was comparable to that of vehicle treatment (Fig. 1). On the drug challenge test (Fig. 2), rats previously treated with HAL (i.e., VEH+HAL and QUI+ HAL) showed much lower avoidance responding than the vehicle rats treated with HAL for the first time. However, an opposite pattern was observed in the CLZ challenge condition: the CLZ-experienced rats (i.e., VEH+CLZ and QUI+CLZ) showed much higher avoidance responding than the vehicle rat treated with CLZ for the first time. One-way ANOVA revealed that under the HAL (0.025 mg/kg) challenge condition, there was a main effect of group (F(2,20)=3.638, P=0.045), and post-hoc tests showed that the VEH+HAL rats made significantly fewer avoidances than the VEH+ VEH ones (HAL-challenged) (P=0.039). The QUI+HAL group did not differ from the VEH+VEH (P=0.116) nor the VEH+HAL (P=0.795). Under the CLZ (5.0 mg/kg) challenge condition, there was a main effect of group (F(2,19)= 6.837, P=0.006), and post-hoc tests showed that the QUI+ CLZ group made significantly more avoidances than the VEH+VEH (CLZ-challenged) group (P=0.005), but not significantly more than the VEH+CLZ group (P=0.286).
Experiment 2: effects of DOI pretreatment on HAL- and CLZ-induced avoidance disruption
Once again, HAL and CLZ suppressed avoidance responding throughout the drug test period (Fig. 3). A split-plot ANOVA revealed a main effect of drug treatment (F(4, 40)= 48.301, P<0.001). One-way ANOVAs showed that all drug groups made significantly fewer avoidances than the VEH+VEH group on every drug test day (P<0.001). Importantly, the DOI+CLZ group made significantly more avoidance than the VEH+CLZ group (day 1, P<0.001; day 2, P=0.005; and day 3, P<0.001), whereas the DOI+ HAL group did not differ significantly from the VEH+HAL group (day 1, P=0.964; day 2, P=0.738; and day 3, P= 1.000), suggesting that pretreatment of DOI attenuated the CLZ-induced disruption of avoidance responding, but did not affect the HAL-induced disruption. This attenuation effect was persistent throughout the drug testing days.
The within-session pattern of avoidance responding on the 1st drug day revealed that DOI pretreatment attenuated the avoidance disruptive effect of CLZ (not HAL) in the 1st and 2nd 10-trial blocks (Tukey’s HSD post-hoc test: DOI+ CLZ vs. VEH+CLZ: P<0.001 and P=0.001, respectively), but lost its effect in the last block (P=0.195) (data not shown). A similar pattern was found on the 2nd and 3rd drug days with a significant reversal occurring in the 1st and 2nd blocks.
On the drug challenge test (Fig. 4), rats previously treated with HAL (i.e., the VEH+HAL rats) made fewer avoidance responses than the vehicle rats treated with HAL for the 1st time, indicating a sensitization-like HAL effect. One-way ANOVA revealed that under the HAL (0.025 mg/kg) challenge condition, there was a main effect of group (F(2,21)=5.122, P=0.017). Post-hoc tests indicated that the VEH+VEH and DOI+HAL groups did not differ from each other (P=0.904), but made significantly more avoidances than the VEH+HAL group (P= 0.046 and 0.033, respectively), indicating that pretreatment of DOI attenuated the long-term repeated HAL effect. Under the CLZ (5.0 mg/kg) challenge condition, the prior CLZ-treated rats tended to make more avoidances than the vehicle rats treated with CLZ for the 1st time, although the overall effect failed to reach a significant level (F(2,22)=2.103, P=0.148). Pretreatment of DOI failed to change the long-term repeated CLZ effect (with there being no significant difference between the VEH+ CLZ and DOI+CLZ, P>0.72).
Results from the above experiments suggest that pretreatment of DOI, but not QUI, attenuated the long-term repeated HAL effect on avoidance, whereas pretreatment of QUI, but not DOI, potentiated the long-term repeated CLZ effect. To examine whether this conclusion holds if data from both experiments were combined, we conducted two separate one-way ANOVAs for the combined HAL groups (i.e., VEH+VEH challenged with HAL, VEH+HAL, QUI+HAL, and DOI+HAL) and the combined CLZ groups (i.e., VEH+VEH challenged with CLZ, VEH+CLZ, QUI+ CLZ, and DOI+CLZ). Results were consistent with the analyses presented above (Fig. 5). Under the HAL challenge condition, the VEH+HAL group made significantly fewer avoidances than the VEH+VEH group (Tukey’s HSD, P= 0.003), and the DOI+HAL group made significantly more avoidances than the VEH+HAL group (P=0.027). Under the CLZ condition, the VEH+VEH group did not differ significantly from the VEH+CLZ group (P=0.103), but did make significantly fewer avoidances than the QUI+CLZ group (P=0.003).
Experiment 3: effects of QUI and DOI pretreatment on HAL and CLZ suppressive effect on motor activity in the CAR
Both HAL and CLZ suppressed motor activity and inter-compartment crossings of well-trained rats in the CAR boxes. Pretreatment of QUI and DOI exhibited a strong attenuation effect on HAL and CLZ, respectively (Fig. 6a, b). On the motor activity, a split-plot ANOVA revealed a main effect of group (F(4,29)=53.053, P<0.001), session (F(2,58)=13.525, P<0.001), and group×session interaction (F(8,58)=4.754, P<0.001). Individual one-way ANOVA on each drug day showed a main effect of group (P<0.001). Post-hoc tests indicated that all drug groups were less active than the VEH+VEH group on the drug test days (all P<0.001) except on day 1, when the QUI+HAL group did not differ significantly from the VEH+VEH group (P=0.338). In addition, the QUI+HAL group was significantly more active than the VEH+HAL group on day 1 (P=0.005) and day 3 (P=0.029), but not on day 2 (P=0.385). The DOI+CLZ group was significantly more active than the VEH+CLZ group only on day 2 (P=0.003), but not on days 1 (P=0.289) and 3 (P=0.177).
On the number of crossings, a split-plot ANOVA revealed a main effect of group (F(4,29)=16.797, P<0.001), session (F(2,58)=13.642, P<0.001), and group×session interaction (F(8,58)=3.738, P=0.001). Individual one-way ANOVA on each drug day showed a main effect of group (P<0.001) with the VEH+HAL and VEH+CLZ groups showing significantly fewer crossings than the VEH+VEH group on every test day (P<0.05). Also, the QUI+HAL group made significantly more crossings than the VEH+HAL group on day 1 (P=0.038), but not on day 2 (P=0.454) or three (P=0.332). The DOI+CLZ group made significantly more crossings than the VEH+CLZ group only on day 2 (P=0.003), but not on day 1 (P=0.235) and day 3 (P=0.245).
Experiment 4: effects of QUI and DOI pretreatment on OLZ-induced avoidance disruption
OLZ 1.0 mg/kg suppressed avoidance responding on the first drug day and progressively enhanced its suppression over the 3-day period (Fig. 7). A split-plot ANOVA revealed a main effect of OLZ (F(4,37)=11.154, P<0.001), a main effect of session (F(2,74)=8.558, P<0.001), but no OLZ×session interaction (F(8,74)=0.943, P=0.487). Individual one-way ANOVA showed that the drug groups made significantly fewer avoidances than the VEH+VEH group on each drug day (P=0.035–0.001) except on day 1, when the QUI+OLZ group (P=0.262) and the DOI+OLZ group (P=0.053) were not significantly different from the VEH+VEH group.
The analysis of within-session patterns of avoidance responding on drug days 1 and 2 revealed that the VEH+ OLZ group made significantly fewer avoidances than the VEH+VEH group in every block on both days (P<0.034). In contrast, the QUI+OLZ group was only significantly different from the VEH+VEH group in the last block on day 2 (P=0.033). Furthermore, the QUI+OLZ group also made more avoidances than the VEH+OLZ group in the 2nd block on day 2 (P=0.019), suggesting that pretreatment of QUI significantly attenuated the avoidance disruptive effect of OLZ.
On the drug challenge test (Fig. 8), rats previously treated with VEH+OLZ made fewer avoidances than the VEH+VEH rats. Independent samples t tests showed a significant group difference between the VEH+VEH and VEH+OLZ groups (t(15)=2.524, P=0.023), a finding consistent with our previous work (Li et al. 2009a; Mead and Li 2009). Both QUI and DOI pretreatment attenuated the long-term repeated OLZ effect. However, overall group difference was only marginally significant (overall group difference: F(4,41)=2.258, P=0.081). To further examine the pretreatment effect of QUI and DOI, we analyzed the within-session pattern of avoidance responding on the drug memory test day (data not shown). The only significant group differences were noted between the VEH+OLZ group and one of the two QUI pretreated groups in the 2nd block (one-way ANOVA: F(4,41)=3.204, P=0.023; Tukey’s HSD post hoc: P=0.042 vs. QUI+OLZ; P=0.036 vs. QUI+DOI+OLZ), but not to the DOI pretreated group (P=0.095), suggesting that QUI pretreatment is more efficacious than DOI in attenuating the long-term repeated OLZ effect.
Discussion
The present study demonstrates interesting dissociated dopamine and serotonin receptor mechanisms underlying acute and repeated effects of HAL, CLZ, and OLZ on avoidance responding—a validated behavioral measure of antipsychotic efficacy (Wadenberg and Hicks 1999). Table 1 summarizes the possible receptor mechanisms as identified in the present study.
Table 1.
Antipsychotic drugs | Haloperidol | Clozapine | Olanzapine |
---|---|---|---|
Acute effect | D2 | 5-HT2A/2C | D2 |
Repeated effect | 5-HT2A/2C | D2 | D2 and 5-HT2A/2C (to a lesser extent) |
HAL is a typical antipsychotic with strong dopamine D2 receptor antagonism and low 5-HT2A/2C antagonism (Miyamoto et al. 2005). It is not surprising to see that QUI, a dopamine D2/3 agonist, but not DOI, a mixed 5-TH2A/2C agonist, was able to reverse the avoidance disruptive effect of HAL (a simple agonist–antagonist interaction). This finding confirms that HAL disrupts avoidance responding primarily by blocking D2 receptors (Wadenberg et al. 2001b) and is consistent with previous work showing that compounds (e.g., l-dopa) that increase endogenous dopamine activity can reverse antipsychotic-induced disruption of avoidance responding (Davies and Redfern 1974; Seiden and Hanson 1964; Seiden and Peterson 1968). It is also in agreement with other findings showing that QUI and HAL counteract each other in a variety of behavioral tasks, e.g., catalepsy (Ninan and Kulkarni 1999), prepulse inhibition (Caine et al. 1995; Wan and Swerdlow 1993), and psychogenic polydipsia (Amato et al. 2008).
CLZ and OLZ are potent 5-HT2A/2C receptor antagonists and relatively weaker dopamine D2 antagonists (Meltzer et al. 2003; Meltzer et al. 1989), but to what extent each receptor action contributes to their disruptive effect on avoidance responding has not been studied. For OLZ, previous studies suggest that blockade of dopamine D2 receptors in the striatum is an integral component (Olsen et al. 2008; Wadenberg et al. 2001b). Our finding that QUI attenuated the disruptive effect of OLZ is consistent with those studies. However, whether OLZ’s antagonist action on 5-HT2A/2C receptors also contributes to its avoidance disruption is not known. Based on the evidence that adjunct treatment with selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonists to low doses of dopamine D2 blocking compounds, enhances the antipsychotic-like suppression of CAR in rats (Wadenberg et al. 1996), we can assume that OLZ’s 5-HT2A receptor antagonism may contribute to its disruption on CAR. Given that 5-HT2C agonism is known to exert an antipsychotic-like suppression on CAR (Wadenberg and Hicks 1999), we speculate that OLZ’s antagonism on 5-HT2C receptors may actually lessen its disruptive effect on CAR via D2 and 5-HT2A antagonism mechanisms. Therefore, the lack of DOI reversal effect on OLZ could be explained by DOI’s agonist action on 5-HT2C which may have masked its reversal effect on OLZ via 5-HT2A receptors. Thus, in the case of OLZ, it appears that its avoidance disruptive effect is mainly mediated by its D2 and 5-HT2A antagonism. Its 5-HT2C antagonism may possibly lessen its avoidance effect. This notion is also consistent with other evidence showing that 5-HT2C antagonism is functionally opposed to 5-HT2A antagonism with regard to antipsychotic action (Meltzer 2002). More work employing more selective 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C is needed validate this conclusion.
Based on their receptor binding profiles (Miyamoto et al. 2005) as well as findings from our maternal behavior work (Zhao and Li 2009), we hypothesized that pretreatment of DOI, but not QUI would attenuate acute CLZ-induced disruption on avoidance responding. Results confirm this hypothesis. The lack of an effect of QUI on the acute effect of CLZ is similar to what we observed in our maternal behavior study (Zhao and Li 2009), and suggests that its D2 antagonism may not play an important role in its acute effect on avoidance responding. This conclusion is consistent with the observation that although CLZ at 10.0 mg/kg causes 50% inhibition of avoidance responding, it only produces 25% dopamine D2 receptor occupancy (Olsen et al. 2008), far less than what is required to achieve its clinical effect (Kapur et al. 2000). Others also reported that DOI at 10 mg/ kg reversed the avoidance disruptive effect of 10 mg/kg CLZ (Browning et al. 2005), suggesting the reversal effect of DOI on CLZ is quite robust and is a generalized effect. Because CLZ has dual action on both 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors, and DOI is nonselective for 5-HT2A versus 5-HT2C receptors, it is impossible to determine which exact receptor action is responsible for DOI’s reversal effect and CLZ’s avoidance disruptive effect. There are good reasons to speculate that CLZ may disrupt avoidance responding by primarily antagonizing 5-HT2A receptor, as opposed to 5-HT2C. Firstly, 5-HT2C receptor agonists (e.g., WAY-163909, mCPP, D-LSD) are shown to possess a property of suppressing avoidance responding (Grauer et al. 2009; Wadenberg and Hicks 1999). If DOI works by antagonizing CLZ-induced blockade of 5-HT2C receptors, we would expect pretreatment of DOI to potentiate the disruptive of CLZ, as opposed to reverse it. Secondly, selective 5-HT2C receptor agonists are generally ineffective in counteracting CLZ. For example, Ro 60–0175 (3 mg/kg), a selective 5-HT2C receptor agonist is incapable of attenuating CLZ (20 mg/kg)—induced dopamine release in rat medial prefrontal cortex, whereas DOI (2.5 mg/kg) is capable of achieving such an effect (Ichikawa et al. 2001). Thirdly, many behavioral and molecular effects of DOI are found to be mediated by its antagonism on 5-HT2A receptors, not on 5-HT2C receptors. For example, DOI-induced effects on locomotor activity, drug discrimination, head-twitch response, and prepulse inhibition (PPI) of acoustic startle are antagonized by 5-HT2A-selective antagonist (e.g., M100907) but not by selective 5-HT2C/2B antagonists (e.g. SB 200,646A, SB 206,553, and SER-082) (Halberstadt et al. 2009; Schreiber et al. 1995; Sipes and Geyer 1995, 1997; Smith et al. 2003), and repeated DOI administration induces significant changes in binding in 5-HT2A receptors but not 5-HT2C receptors (Smith et al. 1999).
At the behavioral level, we observed that the patterns of reversal effects of QUI and DOI are fairly consistent with their reversal effects on HAL- and CLZ-induced motor suppression (Fig. 6). It is thus tempting to suggest that QUI and DOI attenuated HAL- and CLZ-induced avoidance disruption by increasing motor function. This view may be too simplistic because the increased motor activity and number of inter-compartment crossings may reflect other psychological processes, such as an increase in arousal and attention, or an increase in motivation in responding to stimuli. The motor function view is also difficult in explaining why QUI failed to attenuate the effect of CLZ and DOI failed to attenuate the effects of HAL, as well as why the combination of QUI and DOI also failed to attenuate the effect of OLZ. It is also incompatible with the findings that DOI at this dose range decreases, not increases, motor activity in rats (Elliott et al. 1990; Granoff and Ashby 1998; Hawkins et al. 2008; Hillegaart et al. 1996; Krebs-Thomson and Geyer 1996) and that pretreatment of DOI can reduce neuroleptic-induced catalepsy (presumably increasing motor function) (Wadenberg and Ahlenius 1995). The additional finding that QUI at 1.0 mg/ kg had little effect on avoidance responding by itself, whereas DOI at 2.5 mg/kg actually suppressed avoidance (see Table 2) is also inconsistent with the general motor function view.
Table 2.
Predrug | Testa | |
---|---|---|
QUI-1.0 mg/kg (n=12) | 27.64±0.897 | 22.64±3.082 |
DOI-2.5 mg/kg (n=15) | 28.07±0.679 | 8.73±2.205* |
Rats from experiment 3 were first retrained in a 30-trial CAR session to reacquire avoidance responding. Only those that reached the retraining criterion (>70% avoidance responses) in that retraining session were used in this follow-up test. Rats tested with QUI-1.0 mg/kg consisted of rats that were treated with either vehicle, HAL or QUI+ HAL in experiment 3. Rats tested with DOI-2.5 mg/kg consisted of rats that were treated with vehicle, CLZ or DOI+CLZ in experiment 3
P<0.001 significantly different from the predrug condition
QUI (1.0 mg/kg, s) and DOI (2.5 mg/kg, s) were administered 40 min before a 30-trial CAR test
In our previous studies and the present one, we have observed that rats that have been previously treated with HAL and OLZ make significantly fewer avoidance responses than rats treated with these drugs for the first time in a later drug challenge test (Li et al. 2007, 2009a; Mead and Li 2009). Interestingly, CLZ appeared to produce an opposite effect: rats previously treated with CLZ tended to make more avoidances than CLZ-naïve rats. This tolerance-like effect of CLZ is not new (Goudie et al. 2007a, b; Villanueva and Porter 1993), as Sanger (1985) also reported that tolerance developed rapidly to the avoidance disruptive effect of CLZ with repeated administration over 4 days. The new findings are on the distinct receptor mechanisms behind the sensitization and tolerancelike effects. Our results indicate that the repeated effect of HAL may be mediated by its action on 5-HT2A/2C receptor system, whereas the repeated effect of OLZ and CLZ may be mediated by their action on D2/3 receptor system. Given the fact that HAL is also a 5-HT2A receptor inverse agonist (Weiner et al. 2001), and repeated HAL treatment causes a reduction in 5-HT2A receptor mRNA expression in various limbic regions (Buckland et al. 1997), it is possible that HAL causes a sensitization-like effect in this model by down-regulating 5-HT2A receptor. DOI may decrease this long-term impact of HAL by counteracting its effect on 5-HT2A receptor. This idea is also consistent with the well-known augmentation effect of 5-HT2A antagonism on HAL. For example, 5-HT2A-selective antagonist M100907 is shown to potentiate HAL-induced dopamine release in the medial prefrontal cortex (Bonaccorso et al. 2002), to reduce the reward-attenuating effect of HAL (Benaliouad et al. 2007), and to potentiate the avoidance disruptive effect of HAL (Wadenberg et al. 2001a). The repeated effect of CLZ and OLZ via D2/3 receptor systems could be understood in the context of their known long-term effect on D2/3 receptors (Atkins et al. 1999; Kapur et al. 2003; Moran-Gates et al. 2006). One important task for future research is to figure out how antipsychotics induce brain changes through these and other receptor systems (e.g., 5-HT1A, D1, D4, etc.) and how important different receptor mechanisms are to the clinical antipsychotic action of a drug.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported in part by the NARSAD Young Investigator Award (2007–2009), and the NIMH (R21MH079894) grant to Professor Ming Li. We thank Mr. Wei He and Ms. Natashia Swalve for their technical and editorial help. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their extensive and constructive comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript.
Contributor Information
Ming Li, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 238 Burnett Hall, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA.
Tao Sun, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 238 Burnett Hall, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA; Jiangsu Key Laboratory of Neurodegeneration, Department of Pharmacology, Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, People’s Republic of China.
Chen Zhang, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 238 Burnett Hall, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA; Shanghai Mental Health Center, Shanghai Jiaotong University, 600 South Wanping, Xuhui District, Shanghai, People’s Republic of China.
Gang Hu, Jiangsu Key Laboratory of Neurodegeneration, Department of Pharmacology, Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, People’s Republic of China.
References
- Amato D, Stasi MA, Borsini F, Nencini P. Haloperidol both prevents and reverses quinpirole-induced nonregulatory water intake, a putative animal model of psychogenic polydipsia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;200:157–165. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1229-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnt J. Pharmacological specificity of conditioned avoidance response inhibition in rats: inhibition by neuroleptics and correlation to dopamine receptor blockade. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol (Copenh) 1982;51:321–329. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0773.1982.tb01032.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkins JB, Chlan-Fourney J, Nye HE, Hiroi N, Carlezon WA, Jr, Nestler EJ. Region-specific induction of deltaFosB by repeated administration of typical versus atypical antipsychotic drugs. Synapse. 1999;33:118–128. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199908)33:2<118::AID-SYN2>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benaliouad F, Kapur S, Rompre PP. Blockade of 5-HT2a receptors reduces haloperidol-induced attenuation of reward. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:551–561. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolles RC. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol Rev. 1970;77:32–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bonaccorso S, Meltzer HY, Li Z, Dai J, Alboszta AR, Ichikawa J. SR46349-B, a 5-HT(2A/2C) receptor antagonist, potentiates haloperidol-induced dopamine release in rat medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:430–441. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00311-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning JL, Patel T, Brandt PC, Young KA, Holcomb LA, Hicks PB. Clozapine and the mitogen-activated protein kinase signal transduction pathway: implications for antipsychotic actions. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:617–623. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckland PR, D’Souza U, Maher NA, McGuffin P. The effects of antipsychotic drugs on the mRNA levels of serotonin 5HT2A and 5HT2C receptors. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1997;48:45–52. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(97)00076-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caine SB, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Effects of D3/D2 dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1995;12:139–145. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(94)00071-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies JA, Redfern PH. The interaction of dopamine-antagonists and amantadine hydrochloride on the acquisition of the conditioned avoidance response in rats. Neuropharmacology. 1974;13:941–948. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(74)90085-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragunow M, Robertson GS, Faull RL, Robertson HA, Jansen K. D2 dopamine receptor antagonists induce fos and related proteins in rat striatal neurons. Neuroscience. 1990;37:287–294. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(90)90399-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott PJ, Walsh DM, Close SP, Higgins GA, Hayes AG. Behavioural effects of serotonin agonists and antagonists in the rat and marmoset. Neuropharmacology. 1990;29:949–956. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(90)90146-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudie AJ, Cole JC, Sumnall HR. Olanzapine and JL13 induce cross-tolerance to the clozapine discriminative stimulus in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2007a;18:9–17. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328014138d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudie AJ, Cooper GD, Cole JC, Sumnall HR. Cyproheptadine resembles clozapine in vivo following both acute and chronic administration in rats. J Psychopharmacol. 2007b;21:179–190. doi: 10.1177/0269881107067076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granoff MI, Ashby CR., Jr The effect of the repeated administration of the compound 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on the response of rats to the 5-HT2A, C receptor agonist (+/−)-1-(2, 5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane (DOI) Neuropsychobiology. 1998;37:36–40. doi: 10.1159/000026474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grauer SM, Graf R, Navarra R, Sung A, Logue SF, Stack G, Huselton C, Liu Z, Comery TA, Marquis KL, Rosenzweig-Lipson S. WAY-163909, a 5-HT2C agonist, enhances the preclinical potency of current antipsychotics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;204:37–48. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1433-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halberstadt AL, van der Heijden I, Ruderman MA, Risbrough VB, Gingrich JA, Geyer MA, Powell SB. 5-HT(2A) and 5-HT (2C) receptors exert opposing effects on locomotor activity in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1958–1967. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins MF, Uzelac SM, Hearn JK, Baumeister AA. Effects of selective serotonin2 ligands on behaviors evoked by stress in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:632–639. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillegaart V, Estival A, Ahlenius S. Evidence for specific involvement of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A/C receptors in the expression of patterns of spontaneous motor activity of the rat. Eur J Pharmacol. 1996;295:155–161. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(95)00666-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ichikawa J, Dai J, Meltzer HY. DOI, a 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist, attenuates clozapine-induced cortical dopamine release. Brain Res. 2001;907:151–155. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02596-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S, Seeman P. Does fast dissociation from the dopamine d (2) receptor explain the action of atypical antipsychotics?: a new hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:360–369. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S, Wadenberg ML, Remington G. Are animal studies of antipsychotics appropriately dosed? Lessons from the bedside to the bench. Can J Psychiatry. 2000;45:241–246. doi: 10.1177/070674370004500302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S, VanderSpek SC, Brownlee BA, Nobrega JN. Antipsychotic dosing in preclinical models is often unrepresentative of the clinical condition: a suggested solution based on in vivo occupancy. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;305:625–631. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.046987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs-Thomson K, Geyer MA. The role of 5-HT(1A) receptors in the locomotor-suppressant effects of LSD: WAY-100635 studies of 8-OH-DPAT, DOI and LSD in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 1996;7:551–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levis DJ, Brewer KE. The neurotic paradox: attempts by two-factor fear theory and alternative avoidance models to resolve the issues associated with sustained avoidance responding in extinction. In: Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook of contemporary learning theories. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2001. pp. 561–597. [Google Scholar]
- Li M, Davidson P, Budin R, Kapur S, Fleming AS. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs on maternal behavior in postpartum female rats. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:69–80. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li M, Fletcher PJ, Kapur S. Time course of the antipsychotic effect and the underlying behavioral mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:263–272. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li M, He W, Mead A. An investigation of the behavioral mechanisms of antipsychotic action using a drug-drug conditioning paradigm. Behav Pharmacol. 2009a;20:184–194. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32832a8f66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li M, He W, Mead A. Olanzapine and risperidone disrupt conditioned avoidance responding in phencyclidine-pretreated or amphetamine-pretreated rats by selectively weakening motivational salience of conditioned stimulus. Behav Pharmacol. 2009b;20:84–98. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283243008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mead A, Li M. Avoidance-suppressing effect of antipsychotic drugs is progressively potentiated after repeated administration: an interoceptive drug state mechanism. J Psychopharmacol. 2009;24:1045–1053. doi: 10.1177/0269881109102546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mead A, Li M, Kapur S. Clozapine and olanzapine exhibit an intrinsic anxiolytic property in two conditioned fear paradigms: contrast with haloperidol and chlordiazepoxide. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:551–562. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer H. Mechanisms of action of atypical antipsychotic drugs. In: Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology the fifth generation of progress: an official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer HY, Matsubara S, Lee JC. The ratios of serotonin2 and dopamine2 affinities differentiate atypical and typical antipsychotic drugs. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1989;25:390–392. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer HY, Li Z, Kaneda Y, Ichikawa J. Serotonin receptors: their key role in drugs to treat schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003;27:1159–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyamoto S, Duncan GE, Marx CE, Lieberman JA. Treatments for schizophrenia: a critical review of pharmacology and mechanisms of action of antipsychotic drugs. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:79–104. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran-Gates T, Gan L, Park YS, Zhang K, Baldessarini RJ, Tarazi FI. Repeated antipsychotic drug exposure in developing rats: dopamine receptor effects. Synapse. 2006;59:92–100. doi: 10.1002/syn.20220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ninan I, Kulkarni SK. Quinpirole, 8-OH-DPAT and ketanserin modulate catalepsy induced by high doses of atypical antipsychotics. Meth Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 1999;21:603–608. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen CK, Brennum LT, Kreilgaard M. Using pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modelling as a tool for prediction of therapeutic effective plasma levels of antipsychotics. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;584:318–327. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Solomon RL. Two-process learning theory: relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychol Rev. 1967;74:151–182. doi: 10.1037/h0024475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanger DJ. The effects of clozapine on shuttle-box avoidance responding in rats: comparisons with haloperidol and chlordiazepoxide. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1985;23:231–236. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(85)90562-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiber R, Brocco M, Audinot V, Gobert A, Veiga S, Millan MJ. (1-(2, 5-dimethoxy-4 iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane)-induced head-twitches in the rat are mediated by 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) 2A receptors: modulation by novel 5-HT2A/2C antagonists, D1 antagonists and 5-HT1A agonists. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1995;273:101–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeman P, Lee T, Chau-Wong M, Wong K. Antipsychotic drug doses and neuroleptic/dopamine receptors. Nature. 1976;261:717–719. doi: 10.1038/261717a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seiden LS, Hanson LC. Reversal of the reserpine-induced suppression of the conditioned avoidance response in the cat by l-DOPA. Psychopharmacologia. 1964;6:239–244. doi: 10.1007/BF00413153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seiden LS, Peterson DD. Reversal of the reserpine-induced suppression of the conditioned avoidance response by l-dopa: correlation of behavioral and biochemical differences in two strains of mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1968;159:422–428. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sipes TE, Geyer MA. DOI disruption of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat is mediated by 5-HT(2A) and not by 5-HT(2C) receptors. Behav Pharmacol. 1995;6:839–842. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sipes TE, Geyer MA. DOI disrupts prepulse inhibition of startle in rats via 5-HT2A receptors in the ventral pallidum. Brain Res. 1997;761:97–104. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00316-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith RL, Barrett RJ, Sanders-Bush E. Mechanism of tolerance development to 2, 5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine in rats: downregulation of the 5-HT2A, but not 5-HT2C, receptor. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;144:248–254. doi: 10.1007/s002130051000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith RL, Barrett RJ, Sanders-Bush E. Discriminative stimulus properties of 1-(2, 5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane [(+/−)DOI] in C57BL/6 J mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;166:61–68. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1252-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villanueva HF, Porter JH. Differential tolerance to the behavioral effects of chronic pimozide and clozapine on multiple random interval responding in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 1993;4:201–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadenberg ML, Ahlenius S. Antagonism by the 5-HT2A/C receptor agonist DOI of raclopride-induced catalepsy in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol. 1995;294:247–251. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(95)00535-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadenberg ML, Hicks PB. The conditioned avoidance response test re-evaluated: is it a sensitive test for the detection of potentially atypical antipsychotics? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:851–862. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00037-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadenberg ML, Salmi P, Jimenez P, Svensson T, Ahlenius S. Enhancement of antipsychotic-like properties of the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist, raclopride, by the additional treatment with the 5-HT2 receptor blocking agent, ritanserin, in the rat. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1996;6:305–310. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(96)00035-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadenberg MG, Browning JL, Young KA, Hicks PB. Antagonism at 5-HT(2A) receptors potentiates the effect of haloperidol in a conditioned avoidance response task in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001a;68:363–370. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00483-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wadenberg ML, Soliman A, VanderSpek SC, Kapur S. Dopamine D(2) receptor occupancy is a common mechanism underlying animal models of antipsychotics and their clinical effects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001b;25:633–641. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00261-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wan FJ, Swerdlow NR. Intra-accumbens infusion of quinpirole impairs sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1993;113:103–109. doi: 10.1007/BF02244341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner DM, Burstein ES, Nash N, Croston GE, Currier EA, Vanover KE, Harvey SC, Donohue E, Hansen HC, Andersson CM, Spalding TA, Gibson DF, Krebs-Thomson K, Powell SB, Geyer MA, Hacksell U, Brann MR. 5-hydroxytryptamine2A receptor inverse agonists as antipsychotics. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;299:268–276. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitaker NG, Lindstrom TD. Disposition and biotransformation of quinpirole, a new D-2 dopamine agonist antihypertensive agent, in mice, rats, dogs, and monkeys. Drug Metab Dispos. 1987;15:107–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao C, Li M. The receptor mechanisms underlying the disruptive effects of haloperidol and clozapine on rat maternal behavior: a double dissociation between dopamine D(2) and 5-HT(2A/2C) receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93:433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]