Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Jan 23.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Sci. 2010 Mar 9;21(4):511–517. doi: 10.1177/0956797610364762

Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior

Matthew K Nock 1, Jennifer M Park 2, Christine T Finn 2, Tara L Deliberto 1, Halina J Dour 1, Mahzarin R Banaji 1
PMCID: PMC5258199  NIHMSID: NIHMS763294  PMID: 20424092

Abstract

Suicide is a leading cause of death worldwide, challenging all theories that assume a universal drive for self-preservation. It is difficult to predict and prevent because people who consider killing themselves often are unwilling or incapable of reporting their intention. Advances in the measurement of implicit cognition provide an opportunity to test whether automatic associations of self with death can provide a behavioral marker of suicide risk. We measured implicit associations about death/suicide in 157 people presenting for treatment at a psychiatric emergency department while they awaited medical attention. Results confirmed that suicide attempters hold a significantly stronger implicit association between death/suicide and self than do psychiatrically distressed nonattempters. Moreover, the implicit association of death/suicide with self was associated with an approximately six-fold increase in the odds of making a suicide attempt in the next 6 months, exceeding the predictive validity of known risk factors (e.g., depression or suicide attempt history) and both patients’ and clinicians’ predictions. These results provide the first evidence of a behavioral marker for suicidal behavior and suggest that measures of implicit cognition may be useful for detecting and predicting sensitive clinical behaviors that are unlikely to be reported.

Keywords: suicide, suicide attempt, prediction, IAT


Suicide is a leading cause of death worldwide and is among the most perplexing of all human behaviors in that it fundamentally challenges the belief that all organisms are motivated by a drive for self-preservation. Although scholars and scientists have attempted to understand and measure the “suicidal mind” for centuries (Shneidman, 1998, 2004), a major barrier has been the near-universal reliance on self-report. This approach is limited by the fact that people often do not know their own minds (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Wilson, 2009) and is especially problematic in measuring suicidal thoughts because people often are motivated to deny or conceal such thoughts to avoid intervention or hospitalization. For instance, one recent study found that 78% of patients who die by suicide explicitly deny suicidal thoughts in their last verbal communications before killing themselves (Busch, Fawcett, & Jacobs, 2003). Another demonstrated that the risk of suicide is significantly elevated immediately after people are released from hospital care (Qin & Nordentoft, 2005), presumably following their verbal report that they are no longer considering killing themselves.

In an attempt to improve the understanding and prevention of suicide, scientists have searched for objective markers for suicide risk (i.e., measurable characteristics that indicate the presence of an underlying disease process or elevated risk of this negative outcome). Most such work has examined potential biological markers (Mann et al., 2006). However, this approach has been limited in that the factors identified are not specific to suicidal behavior (Caspi et al., 2003) and because many of these results have failed to replicate (Risch et al., 2009). The complementary approach of identifying behavioral markers holds great promise given the possible specificity and the ease of use of behavioral tests. To date, however, the identification of behavioral markers for psychopathology and suicide has received surprisingly little empirical attention.

We tested whether those who had made a decision to kill themselves would reveal stronger implicit cognition associating self with death/suicide and whether the strength of such an association would predict actual suicide attempts. We developed and evaluated a version of the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) that measures the association of death/suicide with self. Variations of this task have been used in previous research (Nock & Banaji, 2007a, 2007b); however, earlier studies were limited to predicting only past episodes of self-injurious/suicidal behavior and future suicide ideation in a laboratory setting. The current study was designed to provide a significant advance over prior research by examining the usefulness of a test of this putative behavioral marker administered to adults presenting to a psychiatric emergency department—some of whom had just made a suicide attempt. Given the alarmingly high risk of future suicide attempts among this population (Qin & Nordentoft, 2005), we also followed participants over the next 6 months to test whether an implicit association of death/suicide with self also predicted future suicide attempts. Finally, we tested whether this measure added incrementally to prediction above and beyond the use of known risk factors—the strongest possible test of any new marker. Evidence for such prediction would extend research on self-destructive behaviors, would add to recent evidence that the IAT can aid in the prediction of socially sensitive behaviors (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009), and would illustrate the usefulness of psychological science in the improvement of clinical prediction and decision making (Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000).

METHOD

Participants

Participants were 157 adults presenting to the psychiatric emergency department of a large metropolitan hospital, drawn from a larger sample of 198 patients, 41 of whom were excluded from the analyses because (a) they were discharged from the emergency department before completing study measures (e.g., transported via ambulance to another hospital; n = 28); (b) they showed evidence of cognitive impairment (e.g., severe psychotic symptoms or somnolence from medication effects; n = 12); or (c) a computer malfunction occurred (n = 1). Those included did not differ from those excluded in terms of age, sex, ethnicity, or psychiatric diagnoses (ps = .24–.91). Participants’ characteristics are presented in Table 1. This sample size provided adequate statistical power to detect the medium-to-large effect sizes expected (α = .05, 1−β = .79 and .99, for medium and large effects, respectively).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Sample

Variable No suicide attempt in past week
(n = 114)
Suicide attempt in past week
(n = 43)
Statistical test Effect size
Mean age in years 35.1 (11.8) 36.6 (12.6) t(155) = –0.68 d = 0.11
Sex (%) χ2(1) = 0.89 Φ = .08
 Female 36.0 44.2
 Male 64.0 55.8
Race (%) χ2(4) = 5.13 Φ = .18
 White 82.5 76.7
 Black   7.9 18.6
 Hispanic   6.1   2.3
 Asian   1.8   2.3
 Other   1.8   0.0
Mental disorders present (%)
 Any depressive disorder 68.4 88.4 χ2(1) = 8.67* Φ = .24
 Any psychotic disorder   6.1   0.0 χ2(1) = 2.76 Φ = .13
 Any anxiety disorder 23.7 25.6 χ2(1) = 0.44 Φ = .05
 Any impulse-control disorder   1.8   0.0 χ2(1) = 0.76 Φ = .07
 Any eating disorder   5.3   0.0 χ2(1) = 2.35 Φ = .12
 Any substance use disorder 23.7 27.9 χ2(1) = 0.30 Φ = .04
 Any alcohol use disorder 23.7 34.9 χ2(1) = 2.00 Φ = .11
 Any other disorder   6.1   4.7 χ2(1) = 0.13 Φ = .03
 Mean total number of disorders   1.6 (1.0)   1.8 (0.7) t(155) = –1.70 d = 0.27
Prior suicide attempt (%) χ2(1) = 13.3** Φ = .29
 No prior attempt 57.0 30.2
 One prior attempt 19.3 18.6
 Multiple prior attempts 23.7 51.2
**

p < .01.

Procedure

Consistent with standard clinical procedures, upon presenting to the emergency department, all patients with a mental health complaint were evaluated by a member of the psychiatric clinical staff and typically remained in the emergency department for 1 to 4+ hrs while they underwent further evaluation, received medical/psychiatric treatment, or awaited transfer/discharge. During this time, a member of our research team approached patients meeting study inclusion/exclusion criteria, described the study, and obtained informed consent. The inclusion criterion was adult status (at least 18 years of age); the exclusion criterion was the presence of any factor that impaired the ability to comprehend and effectively participate in the study, including inability to speak English, gross cognitive impairment, or extremely agitated or violent behavior. These criteria were determined primarily by the clinician’s examination. As an additional measure of cognitive impairment, we included several true/false questions about the study at the end of the consent form, which participants had to answer correctly to be invited to participate. Consenting patients completed all study measures in the emergency department while seated either in their hospital bed, in a small office in the emergency department, or in the emergency department waiting area.

Measurement

Death/suicide Implicit Association Test (IAT)

The IAT is a brief computer-administered test that uses people’s reaction times when classifying semantic stimuli to measure the automatic mental associations they hold about various topics, in this case, life and death/suicide (see https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/ for demonstration tests). The death/suicide IAT was administered and scored in keeping with standard IAT procedures (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). In the IAT version reported here, participants classified stimuli representing the constructs of “death” (i.e., die, dead, deceased, lifeless, and suicide) and “life” (i.e., alive, survive, live, thrive, and breathing) and the attributes of “me” (i.e., I, myself, my, mine, and self) and “not me” (i.e., they, them, their, theirs, and other). Response latencies for all trials were recorded and analyzed using the standard IAT scoring algorithm (Greenwald et al., 2003). The relative strength of each participant’s association between “death” and “me” was indexed by calculating a D score for each participant, where positive D scores represent a stronger association between death and self (i.e., faster responding on the “death”/“me” blocks relative to the “life”/“me” blocks) and negative scores represent a stronger association between life and self.

Demographic and psychiatric factors

Known demographic and psychiatric risk factors for suicide attempts were assessed to test the incremental predictive validity of the IAT. Each participant’s age, sex, and race/ethnicity, as well as his or her principal psychiatric diagnosis, were recorded from the medical record during his or her emergency department visit.

History of suicidal behaviors

Current and past history of suicidal behavior was assessed to determine group status at baseline and to examine the incremental predictive validity of the IAT, given that prior suicidal thoughts and attempts are among the strongest predictors of subsequent suicide attempts (Nock, Borges, et al., 2008). Presence of a suicide attempt was assessed using the Self-Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Interview (SITBI), a structured interview with good reliability and validity (Nock, Holmberg, Photos, & Michel, 2007). Consistent with current evidence-based assessment practices, the SITBI assesses the presence of suicide attempt (i.e., “an actual attempt to kill yourself in which you had at least some intent to die”) and distinguishes such behavior from suicide gestures (i.e., “doing something to lead someone to believe you wanted to kill yourself when you really had no intention of doing so”) and nonsuicidal self-injury (i.e., “purposely hurting yourself without wanting to die”). Patients’ severity of suicide ideation while in the emergency department was assessed with the Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation (Beck & Steer, 1991).

Clinician and patient predictions

The assessment of suicide risk in clinical settings relies largely on clinical prediction, which incorporates the clinician’s intuition, or gut feeling, based on her or his clinical interview with the patient and evaluation of all available information. Clinician prediction was assessed with the question, “Based on your clinical judgment and all that you know of this patient, if untreated, what is the likelihood that this patient will make a suicide attempt in the next 6 months? (0–10, with 0 being no likelihood and 10 being very high likelihood),” administered via a brief questionnaire completed by each patient’s primary clinician (e.g., attending psychiatrist). Risk assessment also incorporates the patient’s own prediction of the likelihood of a future suicide attempt. Patient prediction was assessed with the question, “On this scale of 0 to 4, what is the likelihood that you will make a suicide attempt in the future?” We used clinicians’ and patients’ subjective, single-item estimates rather than administering a multi-item interview or rating scale because we wanted to compare the predictive ability of the IAT to what is commonly used in emergency department settings rather than what is possible using methods from other research studies. In addition, because prior studies have demonstrated differential prediction of suicide attempts using clinician versus patient report (Joiner, Rudd, & Rajab, 1999), these were included as distinct predictors in the current study.

Follow-up assessment

The presence of a suicide attempt during the 6-month post–emergency department period was assessed in all participants using two methods: a telephone interview during which we readministered the SITBI and an examination of the hospital medical record of each participant to determine whether he or she had returned to the hospital due to a suicide attempt during this 6-month period—a commonly used approach in follow-up studies of suicide attempters (McAuliffe, Corcoran, Hickey, & McLeavey, 2008; Tiihonen et al., 2006). A suicide attempt was considered to have occurred during the follow-up period if there was evidence for an attempt from either of these two sources, which showed a high level of agreement (κ = .84).

Data analysis

Performance on the IAT was compared, using a t test for independent samples, between those who did and those who did not make a suicide attempt immediately before presenting to the emergency department. Next, we tested whether the IAT added incrementally to the prediction of suicide attempt status at baseline beyond the effect of other predictors. In keeping with recommendations on the statistical prediction of suicidal behavior (Cohen, 1986), we used hierarchical logistic regression analyses in which significant correlates of suicide attempters were entered in the first step and performance on the IAT was entered in the second step. The same analytic procedure was followed in prospectively predicting suicide attempts during follow-up, with the addition of a step controlling for clinician/patient prediction and severity of suicide ideation while in the emergency department. The prediction of 6-month suicide attempts focused specifically on patients with a lifetime history of suicide attempt at baseline, given that this is a group known to be at significantly elevated risk of suicidal behavior (Nock, Borges, et al., 2008), and so we wanted to test whether the IAT could predict suicide attempts among this high-risk group. Because all participants in this model had made a suicide attempt, the variable for history of prior suicide attempts in this analysis indicated whether each person had a history of multiple suicide attempts (coded 0 or 1), a factor known to further increase the risk of subsequent attempts (Rudd, Joiner, & Rajab, 1996).

RESULTS

Results revealed that patients presenting to the emergency department after a suicide attempt had a significantly stronger implicit association between death/suicide and self than those presenting with other psychiatric emergencies, t(155) = 2.46, p < .05. This difference was not explained by demographic or clinical differences, because the groups differed on only two other factors (i.e., presence of a current depressive disorder and history of prior suicide attempts; see Table 1) and the IAT continued to predict suicide attempts even after controlling for these factors, χ2(1, n=157) = 4.12, p < .05 (see Table 2). Most important, this effect was specific to suicidal self-injury, as those who made a suicide attempt had a significantly stronger implicit association with death/suicide than did those who engaged in self-injurious behavior with no intent to die (i.e., suicide gesture or nonsuicidal self-injury), t(59) = 2.84, p < .05 and the latter group did not differ from noninjurious patients, t(122) = 1.69, n.s..

Table 2.

Hierarchical Logistic Regression Analysis Predicting Suicide Attempt Status at Presentation to the Emergency Department (n=157)

Variable b SE Wald Statistic Odds ratio (95% confidence interval)
Step 1a
 Any depressive disorder 1.35 0.58 5.55 3.87 (1.26–11.94)*
 Prior suicide attempt
  No prior attempt 9.63
  One prior attempt 0.82 0.52 2.50 2.27 (0.82–6.27)
  Multiple prior attempts 1.35 0.44 9.60 3.84 (1.64–9.01)*
Step 2b
 IAT 1.85 0.94 3.93 6.38 (1.02–39.93)*

Notes: IAT = Implicit Association Test.

a

χ2(3, n=157) = 20.20, p < .01, R2 = .18.

b

χ2(1, n=157) = 4.12, p < .05, R2 = .21.

*

p < .05.

Next, we tested whether implicit associations with death/suicide prospectively predicted the occurrence of suicide attempts. Fourteen participants made a suicide attempt during the follow-up period. Performance on the IAT prospectively predicted the occurrence of suicide attempts and did so beyond the influence of other clinical predictors, χ2(1, n=157) = 4.71, p < .05; see Table 3).

Table 3.

Hierarchical Logistic Regression Analysis Predicting Suicide Attempt During 6-Month Follow-Up Period Among Baseline Attempters (n=91)

Variable b SE Wald Statistic Odds ratio (95% confidence interval) χ2 R2
Step 1 χ2(2) = 5.46 0.10
 Any depressive disorder   0.91 1.10 0.70 2.50 (0.29–21.35)
 Multiple suicide attempts   1.42 0.80 3.13 4.14 (0.86–19.96)
Step 2 χ2(3) = 11.00* 0.29
 Scale for Suicide Ideation −0.01 0.04 0.08 0.99 (0.92–1.06)
 Clinician prediction   0.15 0.15 1.09 1.16 (0.88–1.55)
 Patient prediction   0.76 0.27 7.91 2.13 (1.26–3.61)**
Step 3a χ2(1) = 4.71* 0.36
 IAT (continuous)   3.42 1.66 4.25 30.68 (1.18–795.12)*
Step 3b χ2(1) = 5.86* 0.38
 IAT (dichotomous)   1.77 0.76 5.39 5.88 (1.32–26.26)*

Notes: IAT = Implicit Association Test.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01.

In a final analysis, we dichotomized scores on the IAT indicating whether each person’s score represented an association between death/suicide and self (D score > 0) versus life and self (D score < 0), to test this theoretically and clinically meaningful cut point. Patients whose performance revealed a stronger association between death/suicide and self were significantly more likely to make a suicide attempt after leaving the emergency department (31.8%) than were those with a stronger association between life and self (10.1%), χ2(1, n=91) = 6.02, p < .05. This cut point yielded adequate sensitivity (.50) and strong specificity (.81; see Table 4); significantly predicted future suicide attempts beyond the other clinical predictors; and provided a more stable estimate of the association between implicit cognition and odds of a subsequent suicide attempt (see Table 3, Step 3b). Specifically, the presence of an implicit association with death/suicide was associated with an approximately six-fold increase in the odds of making a suicide attempt in the next 6 months.

Table 4.

Classification Statistics for the IAT in Prospectively Predicting Suicide Attempt (n=91)

D score Suicide attempt at follow-up Sensitivity Specificity Positive predictive value Negative predictive value Likelihood ratio
Yes No

>0 7 15 .50 .81 .32 .90 2.63
<0 7 62 (7/14) (62/77) (7/22) (62/69) (50/0.19)

Notes: IAT = Implicit Association Test. Scores on the IAT were dichotomized to indicate either an association between death/suicide and self (D score > 0) or an association between life and self (D score < 0). Sensitivity = proportion of actual suicide attempts correctly identified by the test. Specificity = proportion of non-suicide attempts correctly identified by the test. Positive predictive value = proportion of those with a positive test who were correctly classified as a suicide attempter. Negative predictive value = proportion of those with a negative test who were correctly classified as a nonattempter.

DISCUSSION

The decision to end one’s own life is perhaps the most important determination a person can make; however, suicidal thoughts often are held privately and are not detectable by others or even by oneself, creating a deep epistemological quandary. This study addresses this long-standing scientific and clinical dilemma by identifying a behavioral marker—an implicit association between death/suicide and self—that distinguishes suicide attempters from other psychiatrically distressed patients, predicts future suicide attempts, and provides superior prediction compared with currently used methods.

These findings are important for both the scientific understanding and clinical prediction of suicidal behavior. Current theories of suicide suggest that people kill themselves to escape intolerable circumstances, such as those resulting from negative life circumstances and the experience of mental disorders (Hawton & van Heeringen, 2009; Nock, Hwang, Sampson, & Kessler, 2009). However, most people experiencing these things never attempt to kill themselves, and these risk factors do not explain why some people cope with difficult circumstances through adaptive methods (e.g., seeking treatment) but others choose suicidal behavior as a means of escape. Our findings suggest that a person’s implicit cognition may guide which behavior she or he chooses, to cope with extreme distress. More specifically, an implicit association with death/suicide may represent one of the final steps in the pathway to suicide that is activated when a person is deciding how to respond to extreme distress (Nock, 2009). Notably, however, this study provides no evidence that such implicit cognitions are causally related to suicide attempts, or even precede them. An alternative account would be that an implicit association between death/suicide and self is a consequence of prior suicidal behaviors. We statistically controlled for past history of suicide attempts in our analyses to address this issue but cannot confidently rule it out entirely. Future studies aimed at changing implicit cognition and observing the effect on future suicidal behavior are needed to answer questions about the potentially causal relation with suicide attempts.

The lack of an association between clinician prediction and subsequent suicidal behavior is unfortunate but consistent with research on the limited value of human judgment in clinical decision-making processes (Dawes, Faust, & Meehl, 1989; Swets et al., 2000). That the measure used here was able to predict suicide attempts beyond the effect of known risk factors (e.g., depression) is particularly noteworthy and suggests that the assessment of implicit cognition may prove valuable for improving prediction in clinical settings. The fact that scores above the zero point on the IAT prospectively predict suicide attempts supports the consequential validity of this measure (Greenwald, Nosek, & Sriram, 2006) and provides evidence for a cut point that ultimately may facilitate the use of the IAT in the clinical decision-making process. Most risk factors for suicidal behavior have high sensitivity but poor specificity (e.g., mental disorders); therefore, the high specificity of the IAT renders it especially useful when combined with measures of these other constructs in the prediction of suicidal behavior.

These encouraging results must be interpreted in the light of several limitations, which point toward important directions for future research. First, patients were recruited from only one emergency department in the northeastern United States. Replication of this study in other sites is required to test the generality of the observed results. Second, although the prediction of suicide attempts among this high-risk sample represents a significant advance over what was previously possible, future studies are needed to test the ability of this approach to also predict first-onset suicide attempts, high-lethality suicide attempts, and suicide death. Such studies require larger samples and longer follow-up periods than those used in the current study but represent important next steps in this line of research. Third, the stimuli used in the IAT tested here focused mostly on death; future versions targeting suicide-related cognitions more narrowly may provide even better prediction and require testing in subsequent studies. Fourth, although we demonstrated that the assessment of implicit cognition can improve on assessment methods currently used by clinicians, we did not compare this improvement with that provided by other scientifically based approaches (e.g., biological measures or structured risk-assessment measures; Mann et al., 2006; Nock, Wedig, Janis, & Deliberto, 2008). Future research is needed not only to further develop the understanding of behavioral markers for the risk of suicidal behavior but for combining such information with that from other data sources (e.g., biological or historical) to advance the understanding, prediction, and prevention of suicidal behavior.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH076047) and the Norlein Foundation. The authors are grateful to Christine Cha, Andres De Los Reyes, and Christopher Dial for their helpful comments on this article.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interests with respect to their authorship and/or the publication of this article.

References

  1. Beck AT, Steer RA. Manual for the Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Busch KA, Fawcett J, Jacobs DG. Clinical correlates of inpatient suicide. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2003;64:14–19. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v64n0105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen J. Statistical approaches to suicidal risk factor analysis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1986;487:34–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1986.tb27883.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dawes RM, Faust D, Meehl PE. Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science. 1989;243:1668–1674. doi: 10.1126/science.2648573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1464–1480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Sriram N. Consequential validity of the implicit association test: Comment on Blanton and Jaccard (2006) American Psychologist. 2006;61:56–61. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;97:17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hawton K, van Heeringen K. Suicide. Lancet. 2009;373:1372–1381. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60372-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Joiner TE, Jr, Rudd MD, Rajab MH. Agreement between self- and clinician-rated suicidal symptoms in a clinical sample of young adults: Explaining discrepancies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67:171–176. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.2.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Mann JJ, Currier D, Stanley B, Oquendo MA, Amsel LV, Ellis SP. Can biological tests assist prediction of suicide in mood disorders? International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;9:465–474. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. McAuliffe C, Corcoran P, Hickey P, McLeavey BC. Optional thinking ability among hospital-treated deliberate self-harm patients: A 1-year follow-up study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008;47:43–58. doi: 10.1348/014466507X230958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review. 1977;84:231–259. [Google Scholar]
  15. Nock MK. Why do people hurt themselves? New insights into the nature and function of self-injury. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18:78–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01613.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Nock MK, Banaji MR. Assessment of self-injurious thoughts using a behavioral test. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007a;164:820–823. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.5.820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Nock MK, Banaji MR. Prediction of suicide ideation and attempts among adolescents using a brief performance-based test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007b;75:707–715. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Cha CB, Kessler RC, Lee S. Suicide and suicidal behaviors. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2008;30:133–154. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Nock MK, Holmberg EB, Photos VI, Michel BD. Self-Injurious thoughts and behaviors interview: Development, reliability, and validity in an adolescent sample. Psychological Assessment. 2007;19:309–317. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Nock MK, Hwang I, Sampson N, Kessler RC. Mental disorders, comorbidity, and suicidal behaviors: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Molecular Psychiatry. 2009 doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.29. advance online publication, 31 March 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Nock MK, Wedig MM, Janis IB, Deliberto TL. Self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. In: Hunsely J, Mash E, editors. A guide to assessments that work. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. pp. 158–177. [Google Scholar]
  22. Qin P, Nordentoft M. Suicide risk in relation to psychiatric hospitalization: Evidence based on longitudinal registers. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:427–432. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, et al. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301:2462–2471. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rudd MD, Joiner T, Rajab MH. Relationships among suicide ideators, attempters, and multiple attempters in a young-adult sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:541–550. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.4.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Shneidman ES. The suicidal mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  26. Shneidman ES. Autopsy of a suicidal mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  27. Swets JA, Dawes RM, Monahan J. Psychological science can improve diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2000;1:1–26. doi: 10.1111/1529-1006.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Tiihonen J, Lonnqvist J, Wahlbeck K, Klaukka T, Tanskanen A, Haukka J. Antidepressants and the risk of suicide, attempted suicide, and overall mortality in a nationwide cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:1358–1367. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Wilson TD. Know thyself. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:384–389. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01143.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES