Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Jan 24;7:41070. doi: 10.1038/srep41070

Body size and vocalization in primates and carnivores

D L Bowling 1,a, M Garcia 1,2, J C Dunn 3,4, R Ruprecht 1, A Stewart 5, K-H Frommolt 6, W T Fitch 1
PMCID: PMC5259760  PMID: 28117380

Abstract

A fundamental assumption in bioacoustics is that large animals tend to produce vocalizations with lower frequencies than small animals. This inverse relationship between body size and vocalization frequencies is widely considered to be foundational in animal communication, with prominent theories arguing that it played a critical role in the evolution of vocal communication, in both production and perception. A major shortcoming of these theories is that they lack a solid empirical foundation: rigorous comparisons between body size and vocalization frequencies remain scarce, particularly among mammals. We address this issue here in a study of body size and vocalization frequencies conducted across 91 mammalian species, covering most of the size range in the orders Primates (n = 50; ~0.11–120 Kg) and Carnivora (n = 41; ~0.14–250 Kg). We employed a novel procedure designed to capture spectral variability and standardize frequency measurement of vocalization data across species. The results unequivocally demonstrate strong inverse relationships between body size and vocalization frequencies in primates and carnivores, filling a long-standing gap in mammalian bioacoustics and providing an empirical foundation for theories on the adaptive function of call frequency in animal communication.


Pioneering work on the biology of vocal communication focused on comparative anatomy and physiology, proposing a primary role for the larynx in protecting the respiratory system and suggesting how this arrangement inevitably led to the production of sounds imbued with valuable information about the biological status of the vocalizer1,2,3. More recent work has added a focus on the acoustics of vocalizations themselves, leading to further hypotheses about the communicative potential of specific acoustic properties4,5. One basic aspect of the modern approach is size-frequency allometry: the study of the relationship between the size of an animal’s body and the frequency content of its vocalizations. Morton (1977) was among the first to formally propose a link between these domains6. In his widely cited motivation-structural rules hypothesis, he proposed that vocalization frequency was shaped by its capacity to communicate biologically critical information about body size6,7. The key assumption underlying this idea is that larger animals inevitably produce vocalizations with lower frequency content than smaller animals. This negative size-frequency allometry provided the physical foundation upon which early biological associations were built, ultimately channeling the evolution of vocalization under pressure from natural selection and resulting in a widely conserved size-frequency code in animal communication.

General support for Morton’s assumption comes from our modern understanding of the physics of vocal production (Fig. 1). At the broadest level, vocalizations can be described as the result of tissue vibrations generated by the passage of air through a constriction in an animal’s vocal tract. In most tetrapods (excluding birds) the principal oscillator is the vocal folds within the larynx. During phonation, air from the lungs passes between the vocal folds, setting them into oscillation and producing a rich spectrum of vibrations that provide the acoustic power for vocalization. When the pattern of vocal fold vibration is approximately periodic, the resulting spectrum is harmonic and we perceive a voice pitch related to its fundamental frequency (‘F0’), which corresponds to the rate of one open/close cycle of vocal fold motion. These laryngeal “source” vibrations are propagated into the air in the supralaryngeal vocal tract, whose resonances act as acoustic “filters”, imposing a second distinct set of resonant frequencies called formants on the laryngeal spectrum8,9.

Figure 1. Vocal production in mammals.

Figure 1

(A) Cut-away diagram of a macaque monkey showing the vocal production apparatus. The larynx is highlighted in red, the supralaryngeal vocal tract is highlighted in blue. (B) Spectrograms depicting key frequency features of animal vocalization. Both spectrograms show the same vocalization (a snow leopard contact call). In the top panel, longer temporal analysis windows (0.25 seconds) emphasize the fundamental frequency (‘F0’; bracketed in red) and its harmonics (horizontal black lines). The dominant frequency (‘DF’; bracketed in yellow) is also labeled. In the bottom panel, shorter temporal analysis windows (0.006 seconds) emphasize the first four formants (‘F1-4’; bracketed in blue).

Two facts about this combination of source and filter constrain the frequency content of vocalization. First, the lowest F0 at which the vocal folds can vibrate is fundamentally limited by their length: longer folds vibrate at lower F0s9. Second, the lowest frequency at which the air within the vocal tract can resonate is also fundamentally limited by its length: longer tracts generate lower formants8,9. Thus, to the extent that vocal fold and vocal tract length scale in proportion to body size, the vocalizations of larger animals can, based on physical principles, be expected to comprise lower frequencies than the vocalizations of smaller animals, as Morton assumed.

Given this physical backdrop, it is somewhat surprising that empirical evidence for negative size frequency allometry is rather sparse, particularly among mammals. Traditionally, the best evidence has come instead from comparative studies of birds and frogs. For example, Wallschläger (1980) found evidence for negative allometry between body mass and center frequency (the geometric mean of a spectrum) across a sample of 90 European passerine bird species (R2 = 0.59)10. Likewise, Gingras et al.11 found evidence for negative allometry between body length and F0 across a sample of 136 frog species (R2 = 0.57)11. In both of these studies, the inclusion of species exhibiting a wide range of body sizes (~5–1400 g in10 and 14–155 mm in11) appears to have been critical. Studies examining narrower ranges of body sizes, e.g., within a single species, have produced mixed results, finding evidence for negative allometry in some cases12,13,14 but not others15,16,17,18.

The principal challenge in conducting large-scale interspecific studies of size-frequency allometry is characterizing the vocal behavior of each target species in a clear and consistent way19. The approach taken by most anuran and avian studies has been to limit the analysis to a single call type that can be reliably identified across species, sacrificing representativeness in favor of comparability. Wallschäger (1980), for example, focused on male “song” in birds, whereas Gingras et al.11 focused on male frog “advertisement calls”10,11. Unfortunately, this approach is not easily adapted to comparably diverse samples of mammals, where interspecific variability in vocal behavior is arguably greater and vocal repertoires often exhibit acoustically graded rather than discrete structures19,20,21. Both of these features complicate the definition and identification of clear call types as a basis for comparison, preventing straightforward extension of the avian and anuran approach to mammals.

To date, we are aware of only three studies that have attempted to derive interspecific size-frequency allometry in mammals. Jones (1996) found evidence for negative allometry between body mass and the frequency of maximal amplitude in bat echolocation calls (no R2 reported)22. More relevant here, Hauser (1993) found evidence for negative allometry between body mass and a parameter he referred to as “mean repertoire frequency” in a sample of 36 primate species (R2 = 0.54)23. Finally, in the most comprehensive study to date Charlton & Reby (2016) found evidence for negative allometry between body mass and F0 in a sample of 67 terrestrial mammal species (R2 = 0.59), as well as between body mass and formant frequency spacing in a smaller sample of 35 terrestrial mammal species (R2 = 0.58)5. Although these studies provide an important foundation for the study of mammalian size-frequency allometry, the accuracy of the relationships derived is limited by the way in which vocal behavior has been characterized across species. Hauser (1993)’s approach, for example, was to include everything he could obtain from published literature, provided that each included species had at least 5 described call types. Although reasonable at the time, this approach is problematic in that the number of vocalizations included for each species was highly variable (between 5–40), calling into question the validity of interspecific comparisons between mean values. A related point is that because original audio recordings were not typically obtained, the capacity for clear and consistent frequency measurements was inherently limited (mean repertoire frequency thus represented a poorly defined mixture of F0 and various frequencies of maximal amplitude, often estimated by visual inspection of published spectrograms). Finally, Hauser’s regression analyses were not phylogenetically controlled, making it likely that the reported relationships are distorted by statistical codependence between closely related species24. Charlton & Reby (2016) were considerably more careful, focusing on well-defined acoustic properties, conducting thorough phylogenetic analyses, and going further to incorporate data on habitat and sexual dimorphism. Even in this impressive study, however, the fact that the majority of the acoustic data was compiled from third-party sources may have introduced inconsistencies in measurement across species as well as variation in the representativeness of mean values.

The present study was designed to address these limitations as well as the general paucity of size-frequency data in mammals. We compiled data representing 91 mammalian species, including 50 primates and 41 carnivorans (hereafter ‘carnivores’) (Table 1). The selected species cover a wide range of body sizes – from the pygmy marmoset (Cebuella pygmaea; 110 g) to the Western gorilla (Gorilla gorilla; 120 Kg), and from the least weasel (Mustela erminea; 140 g) to the polar bear (Ursus maritimus; 250 Kg) – maximizing the likelihood of observing the effects of anatomical constraints on vocal production. In contrast with previous work, our method of obtaining vocalization data is focused on using original recordings for all species, taking full advantage of modern improvements in high-throughput computerized analysis, digital signal processing and the availability of digital recordings through online databases. We use a novel algorithm to capture spectral variability and standardize vocalization selection, giving rise to data specifically prepared for interspecific comparison. These data are compared to body size data using both traditional and phylogenetically controlled regression techniques, resulting in the derivation of empirical size-frequency allometry in primates and carnivores.

Table 1. Abbreviations, common names and Latin names for all 50 primates species and all 41 carnivore species.

# Abbreviation Common Name Latin Name # Abbreviation Common Name Latin Name
A. Primate Species
1 B capuchin Brown capuchin Sapajus apella 26 Guereza Mantled guereza Colobus guereza
2 B lemur Black-and-white ruffed lemur Varecia variegata 27 Human Human Homo sapiens
3 B macaque Barbary macaque Macaca sylvanus 28 J macaque Japanese macaque Macaca fuscata
4 B marmoset Buffy-headed marmoset Callithrix flaviceps 29 L gibbon Lar gibbon Hylobates lar
5 B spider Black-headed spider monkey Ateles fusciceps 30 L macaque Lion-tailed macaque Macaca silenus
6 B squirrel Black-capped squirrel monkey Saimiri boliviensis 31 L’Hoest L’Hoest’s monkey Allochrocebus lhoesti
7 Baboon Hamadryas baboon Papio hamadryas 32 Mandrill Mandrill Mandrillus sphinx
8 Bl. howler Black howler Alouatta caraya 33 N gibbon Northern white-cheecked gibbon Nomascus leucogenys
9 Bo. howler Bolivian red howler Alouatta sara 34 Orangutan Orangutan Pongo pygmaeus
10 C howler Colombian red howler Alouatta seniculus 35 P marmoset Pygmy marmoset Cebuella pygmaea
11 C macaque Crested macaque Macaca nigra 36 P tamarin Pied tamarin Saguinus bicolor
12 C marmoset Common Marmoset Callithrix jacchus 37 Proboscis Proboscis monkey Nasalis larvatus
13 C tamarin Cotton-top tamarin Saguinus oedipus 38 Putty-nosed Putty-nosed monkey Cercopithecus nictitans
14 Chimpanzee Chimpanzee Pan troglodytes 39 R spider Red-faced spider monkey Ateles paniscus
15 Colobus Black colobus Colobus satanas 40 Re. lemur Red-ruffed lemur Varecia rubra
16 De Brazza De Brazza’s monkey Cercopithecus neglectus 41 Ri. Lemur Ring-tailed lemur Lemur catta
17 Diana Diana monkey Cercopithecus diana 42 S macaque Stump-tailed macaque Macaca arctoides
18 Drill Drill Mandrillus leucophaeus 43 S marmoset Silvery marmoset Mico argentatus
19 E tamarin Emperor tamarin Saguinus imperator 44 Saki White-faced saki Pithecia pithecia
20 G capuchin Guianan weeper capuchin Cebus olivaceus 45 Siamang Siamang Symphalangus syndactylus
21 G marmoset Geoffroy’s tufted-ear marmoset Callithrix geoffroyi 46 Sun-tailed Sun-tailed monkey Allochrocebus solatus
22 G squirrel Guianan squirrel monkey Saimiri sciureus 47 Titi Red-bellied titi Callicebus moloch
23 GH tamarin Golden-headed lion tamarin Leontopithecus chrysomelas 48 Vervet Vervet monkey Chlorocebus pygerythrus
24 GL tamarin Golden lion tamarin Leontopithecus rosalia 49 W gibbon Western black-crested gibbon Nomascus concolor
25 Gorilla Western Gorilla Gorilla gorilla 50 Wolfi Wolf’s monkey Cercopithecus wolfi
B. Carnivore Species
1 A bear American black bear Ursus americanus 22 Lion Lion Panthera leo
2 A dog African hunting dog Lycaon pictus 23 M wolf Maned wolf Chrysocyon brachyurus
3 A fox Artic fox Alopex lagopus 24 Marten European pine marten Martes martes
4 B bear Brown bear Ursus arctos 25 Meerkat Meerkat Suricata suricatta
5 B dog Bush dog Speothos venaticus 26 Mongoose Common dwarf mongoose Helogale parvula
6 B fox Bat-eared fox Otocyon megalotis 27 O otter Oriental small-clawed otter Amblonyx cinerea
7 Binturong Binturong Arctictis binturong 28 Ocelot Ocelot Leopardus pardalis
8 C fox Corsac fox Vulpes corsac 29 P bear Polar bear Ursus maritimus
9 C leopard Clouded leopard Neofelis nebulosa 30 R panda Red panda Ailurus fulgens
10 C lynx Canadian lynx Lynx canadensis 31 Racoon Raccoon Procyon lotor
11 Cheetah Cheetah Acinonyx jubatus 32 Re. fox Red fox Vulpes vulpes
12 Coati South American coati Nasua nasua 33 Ru. fox Rüppell’s fox Vulpes rueppellii
13 Cougar Cougar Puma concolor 34 S leopard Snow leopard Uncia uncia
14 Dhole Dhole Cuon alpinus 35 Sea lion California sea lion Zalophus californianus
15 E lynx Eurasian Lynx Lynx lynx 36 Serval Serval Leptailurus serval
16 E otter European otter Lutra lutra 37 Sp. bear Spectacled bear Tremarctos ornatus
17 E wolf Eurasian wolf Canis lupus 38 Stoat Stoat Mustela ermine
18 Hyena Striped hyena Hyaena hyaena 39 Su. Bear Sun bear Helarctos malayanus
19 Jaguar Jaguar Panthera onca 40 Tiger Bengal tiger Pathera tigris tigris
20 Jaguarundi Jaguarundi Herpailurus yagouaroundi 41 Weasel Least weasel Mustela nivalis
21 Kinkajou Kinkajou Potos flavus        

Materials and Methods

Body size data

The measure of body size used in the comparisons presented here is body length, defined as “head + body” length, which excludes tail length and refers to the distance between the ischium of the pelvis and the tip of the snout in carnivores, or the top of the skull in primates (also known as “crown-rump” length). With the exception of California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) and humans, all body size data were obtained from the “Handbook of the Mammals of the World”25,26. This source typically reports length ranges (separated by sex for about 1/3 of the species in our sample). Ranges were converted to means, by species or by sex then species as required. For sea lions, mean body length was calculated from sex-specific range data in27; for humans, mean body length was calculated by multiplying sex-specific standing height values by average “sitting-ratios” as provided in28.

Vocalization database

With the exception of humans and Bolivian red howler monkeys, all vocalization recordings were compiled either from the Animal Sound Archive at the Museum für Naturkunde Berlin, or commercially available CDs29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38. Recordings for which there was information indicating that the vocalizing animal was not an adult were excluded from the database. The human vocalizations consisted of emotionally expressive speech, uttered by male and female speakers of multiple languages39. The Bolivian red howler vocalizations were obtained by JCD in collaboration with the Senda Verde Animal Refuge in Bolivia. The resulting database comprised ~13.5 hours of digital audio stored in 816 audio files (.WAV or.AIFF format). Depending on the quality of the original recordings, sampling rates were either 44.1, 48 or 96 kHz, and bit depths were 16-, 24- or 32-bits.

Preprocessing and vocalization selection

Prior to vocalization selection, each of the 816 audio files was automatically segmented using a custom Matlab (version R2015a; Nantick, MA) script that identified temporally contiguous segments of supra-threshold intensity (intensity was defined by low pass filtering rectified audio waveforms at 5 Hz using a 3rd order Butterworth filter; threshold was defined as >5% of the maximum intensity in a given file). A margin of 100 ms was used, such that segment start times were placed 100 ms prior to when intensity rose above threshold, and end times were placed 100 ms after when intensity fell back below threshold. If either of these 100 ms margins entered into another suprathreshold region, the regions were treated as contiguous and extracted as a single segment. This procedure resulted in 6527 audio segments, each of which was subsequently sorted into a “low” or “high” signal-to-noise ratio category on the basis of manual aural and visual inspection in Praat40. Low signal-to-noise segments – characterized by high-energy environmental noise, the presence of multiple individual vocalizers with overlapping spectra, and/or sounds other than the targeted vocalizations (e.g., cage-rattling) – accounted for 52% of segments overall and were excluded from further analysis. These preprocessing steps resulted in a total of 3151 segments with high signal-to-noise ratios, each representing a single temporally discrete vocalization. The determination of which vocalizations to include in further analyses was performed by a novel three-step selection algorithm also implemented in Matlab. First, the spectrum of each vocalization was determined by multiplying the entire segment by a Hamming window and computing a single discrete Fourier transform (frequency resolution was determined by the number of samples in a given segment, Mean = 3.18 Hz, SD = 4.77 Hz). Second, the frequency of maximum amplitude in each spectrum was identified and used as the basis for sorting the vocalizations of each species into an ordered list. Third, the sorted vocalization list was used to define a maximally-spaced selection of 10 vocalizations. For example, in a list of 100 sorted vocalizations, those selected would be in list positions 1, 12, 23, 34, 45, 56, 67, 78, 89 and 100. This algorithm provides a standardized approach to capturing the entire spectral variability range present in a given species’ vocalizations. Systematically applying it here ensured that the vocalization data selected to represent each species was derived in precisely the same manner, maximizing the validity of subsequent interspecific comparisons. All in all, following this procedure resulted in a subset of 910 vocalizations (10 per species) selected for more detailed frequency analysis.

Frequency analysis

Our analysis of vocalization frequencies focused on dominant frequency (or “DF”) and F0. DF is simply defined as the frequency of maximum amplitude in the spectrum of a vocalization, and was thus already determined during the selection process described above. Nevertheless, for each of the 910 vocalizations in our subset, the DF value ca.lculated by Matlab was manually verified in a second (more detailed) aural and visual inspection in Praat. If the DF value assigned by Matlab did not match acoustic energy present in the vocalization (e.g., because it corresponded to background noise instead), that vocalization was replaced with the neighboring vocalization from the species’ sorted list (this was necessary for 53 of the 910 selected vocalizations; ~6%). Once the accuracy of all DF values was confirmed, a ‘DF10’ value was calculated for each species, corresponding to the mean DF calculated across all 10 of their selected vocalizations.

In contrast to DF, which can be measured for any vocalization, F0 can only be measured for vocalizations that are produced by regular vocal fold vibration. Spectrally, the defining characteristic of these “tonal” vocalizations is the presence of clear harmonics (see Fig. 1B). The only exceptions are “pure tone” vocalizations, which exhibit focused energy at a single specific frequency and do not possess harmonics. For each of the 910 selected vocalizations, those that were harmonic or pure tone were identified for F0 measurement during the second aural and visual inspection in Praat. Following identification, F0 measurements were made using a manually supported algorithmic approach. First, a visual estimate of the frequency distance between harmonics was made from the spectrogram and used to initialize the parameters of Praat’s “To Pitch” algorithm (default values are defined for human speech and thus often needed to be adjusted; this was most often the case for “Pitch range”, followed by “Voicing threshold”, “Silence threshold”, “Octave cost”, “Voiced/unvoiced cost” and “Octave jump cost”). Second, these parameters were adjusted until the F0 values identified by the algorithm matched the estimate and corresponded to the first harmonic observed in the spectrogram. When visual and algorithmic methods could not be made to agree, or when harmonics were too vague or complex to estimate their spacing (e.g., in cases of bifurcation or subharmonics41), F0 was not measured. Following this procedure, F0 could be measured in 664 of the 910 selected vocalizations (~73%). In order to avoid differences in the number of vocalizations used to calculate means across species, comparisons involving F0 were limited to those species that had at least six vocalizations with measured F0 values. Using 6 vocalizations as our criterion allowed us to include 74 species in these analyses (41 primates and 33 carnivores). Each of these species was represented by an ‘F06’ value, corresponding to the mean F0 calculated across six vocalizations. To allow fair comparisons of size-frequency relationships based on DF and F0, ‘DF6’ values (also calculated on the basis of 6 vocalizations) were also determined for these 74 species. Which vocalizations to include in the DF6 and F06 calculations was determined using the same maximally-spaced selection technique described above for vocalization selection. For DF6, the ordered list always included all 10 vocalizations per species and was sorted from lowest to highest by DF; for F06, the starting list included however many of a species’ vocalizations that had F0 values and was sorted from lowest to highest F0.

Statistical analyses

Two types of linear regression analysis were used to model relationships between body length and vocalization frequencies: (1) an ordinary least squares (OLS) regression; and (2) a bisector regression based on the best supported of 5 different generalized least squares (GLS) models. Both approaches have distinct advantages and disadvantages here. The advantages of OLS are that it allows for the use of standard statistical tests (e.g., R2, F-test, ANCOVA) as well as direct comparison with other/older studies. The disadvantages are that it fails to control for the potential statistical codependence of data points representing closely related species24,42, and that it only minimizes error with respect to one of the two variables in a given comparison. This situation is reversed for the bisector regressions, which combine phylogenetic techniques to evaluate and control for statistical codependence between closely related species43, and bisector techniques that account for error in both variables (appropriate here because both body length and vocalizations frequencies are measured and thus subject to error44), but for which standard statistical tests like R2 and ANCOVA do not apply43. Following Charlton & Reby (2016), the GLS models examined here include one non-phylogenetic model (NP), and four phylogenetic models, each of which tests a different assumption about the evolutionary process based on phylogenetic tree data45 (shown in Supplementary Figs 1 and 2). The phylogenetic models were a pure Brownian motion model (BM), a Brownian motion + Pagel’s Lambda model (BM + λ), a Brownian motion + Grafen’s Rho model (BM + ρ), and an Ornstein-Uhlenbeck model (OU)5 (see Supplementary Text S1 for further details).

OLS regressions were calculated in R using the ‘stats’ package function ‘lm.m’46. GLS regressions were calculated in R using the ‘nlme’ package function ‘gls’47 in connection with ‘APE’ package correlation structures48. Correlation structure was set to ‘NULL’ for NP models, ‘corBrownian’ for BM models, ‘corPagel’ for BM + λ models, ‘corGrafen’ for BM + ρ models, and ‘corMartins’ for OU models. As a heuristic indicator of model support, we calculated Akaike Information Criteria corrected for sample size (AICc; R package ‘AICmodavg’ function ‘AICc’)49. The model with the lowest AICc was selected for bisector calculation, which was carried out in Matlab according to the procedure described in Supplementary Text S1. Differences between OLS regressions for primates and carnivores were evaluated for statistical significance using ANCOVA performed in Matlab (function ‘aoctool.m’). Finally, all variables were checked for normality prior to comparisons using Shapiro-Wilk tests performed in Matlab (function ‘swtest.m’). Log-transforms (base-10) were necessarily to achieve normality for body length, DF10, F06, and DF6. These transformations also improved the fits of linear regression.

Results

The results of comparing the logarithm of mean body length (‘logBL’) with the logarithm of mean dominant frequency (‘logDF10’) for all 91 species are shown in Fig. 2 and Table 2A. All regressions showed significant negative relationships for primates, carnivores, and both orders combined, providing empirical verification of negative size-frequency allometry within and across these mammalian orders. Similar results were obtained when the sample size was reduced to 74 species and DF10 was replaced with DF6 (Table 2B; Supplementary Fig. S3). In addition to these principle findings regarding negative size-frequency allometry, several interesting differences were apparent between primates and carnivores. First, the R2 values of the OLS regressions show that LogBL explained over twice as much of the variance in logDF10 for primates than for carnivores (67% and 30% respectively). The proportion of variance explained by the carnivore OLS regression could be improved considerably by excluding the two greatest outliers (spectacled bear and stoat; raising R2 to 0.50) but we know of no a priori reason to exclude these species and reexamination of their data confirmed the accuracy of their exceptional status. Together with the overall greater dispersion of the carnivore data, these results show that although the DF of a vocalization is generally a good predictor of body length, the relationship is much stronger for primates than it is for carnivores. Second, a significant difference between primates and carnivores was also found in OLS regression slopes (βprim = −1.72 vs. βcarn = −0.68; ANCOVA LogBL x order F1,87 = 18.37, P < 0.0001), such that DF drops much more rapidly with increasing body length for primates than for carnivores. Examination of the bisector regressions, however, indicated that much of this second difference (~50%) is attributable to limitations of OLS regression. While the primate bisector regression (based on BM models) was nearly identical to its OLS counterpart, the carnivore bisector regression (based on NP models) was considerably steeper, resulting in a much smaller difference in slope between orders (βprim = −1.71 vs. βcarn = −1.20).

Figure 2. Body length and dominant frequency.

Figure 2

The base-10 logarithm of mean body length plotted against the base-10 logarithm of mean dominant frequency (‘DF10’) for 50 primate species (left), 41 carnivore species (middle), and all 91 species combined (right). In the left and middle panels, color represents biological family (legend in Fig. 4) and each species is labeled with an abbreviated form of its common name (full names in Table 1); in the right panel primates are shown in blue and carnivores in red. Dashed lines depict ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions; dotted lines depict bisector regressions (equations at lower left). Primate and combined bisector regressions were based on phylogenetic Brownian motion (BM) models; carnivore bisector regressions were based on non-phylogenetic (NP) models. Statistics for regression analyses are given in Table 2A (see Supplementary Table S1a for AICc values of GLS models).

Table 2. Statistics for LogBL vs. LogDF10 and LogDF6 comparisons.

Model Slope ± SE Intercept ± SE R2 λ t d.f. P
A. LogBL vs. logDF10
PRIMATES (n = 50)
OLS −1.722 ± 0.173 6.149 ± 0.288 0.673 21.34 48  < 0.0001
Y-on-X (BM) −0.831 ± 0.286 4.666 ± 0.551 −2.9 48 0.0056
X-on-Y (BM) −5.568 ± 0.062 12.606 ± 0.239 −2.9 48 0.0056
CARNIVORES (n = 41)
OLS −0.679 ± 0.166 4.395 ± 0.32 0.299 −4.08 39 0.0002
Y-on-X (NP) −0.679 ± 0.166 4.395 ± 0.32 −4.08 39 0.0002
X-on-Y (NP) −2.267 ± 0.108 7.425 ± 0.337 −4.08 39 0.0002
COMBINED (n = 91)
OLS −1.010 ± 0.113 4.997 ± 0.203 0.471 24.65 89 <0.0001
Y-on-X (BM) −0.685 ± 0.186 4.452 ± 0.43 −3.69 89 0.0004
X-on-Y (BM) −5.157 ± 0.053 12.502 ± 0.222 −3.69 89 0.0004
B. LogBL vs. logDF6
PRIMATES (n = 41)
OLS −1.653 ± 0.197 6.069 ± 0.326 0.644 18.62 39 <0.0001
Y-on-X (BM + λ) −1.15 ± 0.364 5.209 ± 0.649 0.917 −3.16 39 0.003
X-on-Y (BM + λ) −5.913 ± 0.057 13.147 ± 0.21 0.938 −2.98 39 0.0049
CARNIVORES (n = 33)
OLS −0.652 ± 0.186 4.384 ± 0.356 0.284 12.33 31 <0.0001
Y-on-X (NP) −0.652 ± 0.186 4.384 ± 0.356 12.33 31 <0.0001
X-on-Y (NP) −2.298 ± 0.124 7.496 ± 0.394 −3.5 31 0.0014
COMBINED (n = 74)
OLS −0.959 ± 0.126 4.943 ± 0.224 0.445 22.08 72 <0.0001
Y-on-X (BM + λ) −0.671 ± 0.21 4.443 ± 0.437 0.953 −3.19 72 0.0021
X-on-Y (BM + λ) −5.921 ± 0.053 13.88 ± 0.222 0.996 −2.98 72 0.0039

Y-on-X and X-on-Y refer to the two regressions (Y-dependent-on-X and X-dependent-on-Y) from which bisectors were calculated (X = logBL in A & B, Y = logDF10 in A and logDF6 in B); abbreviations in parentheses specify the best-supported GLS model (BM = Brownian motion, NP = non-phylogenetic, BM + λ = Brownian motion + Pagel’s lambda). For the BM + λ models, λ describes the extent to which the dependent variable covaries with phylogeny. T-tests assess whether regression slopes are significantly different than 0. Regression intercepts were all significantly different from 0 (p < 0.0001).

The results of comparing logBL with the logarithm of mean fundamental frequency (‘logF06’) for the subset of 74 species are shown in Fig. 3 and Table 3. As in the previous analysis, all regression analyses between logBL and logF06 showed significant negative relationships for primates, carnivores, and both orders combined, providing further empirical evidence for negative size-frequency allometry within and across these mammalian orders. Also in parallel with the previous results, a difference between primates and carnivores was observed in the R2 values of the OLS regressions, such that the proportion of variance in logF06 explained by logBL was higher for primates than for carnivores (74% and 44% respectively). Finally, the primate OLS regression was determined to be significantly steeper than the carnivore OLS regression (βprim = −2.46 vs. βcarn = −1.00; ANCOVA: LogBL x order F1,70 = 21.48, P < 0.0001). Unlike the DF analyses, however, the magnitude of this slope difference was not much decreased between the bisector regressions (again based on BM models for primates and NP models for carnivores), which exhibited a comparable slope difference (βprim = −2.81 vs. βcarn = −1.46). These results thus provide strong evidence that F0 drops more rapidly with increasing body length for primates than for carnivores.

Figure 3. Body length and fundamental frequency.

Figure 3

The base-10 logarithm of mean body length plotted against the base-10 logarithm of mean fundamental frequency (‘F06’) for 41 primate species (left), 33 carnivore species (middle), and all 74 species combined (right). In the left and middle panels, color represents biological family (legend in Fig. 4), and each species is labeled with an abbreviated form of its common name (full common names and Latin names in Table 1); in the right panel all primates are shown in blue and all carnivores in red. Dashed lines depict ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions; dotted lines depict bisector regressions (equations at lower left). Primate bisector regressions were based on phylogenetic Brownian motion models; carnivore bisector regressions were based on non-phylogenetic (NP) models; combined bisector regressions were based on phylogenetic Brownian motion + Pagel’s Lambda (BM + λ) models. Statistics for regression analyses are given in Table 3 (see Supplementary Table S2 for AICc values of GLS models).

Table 3. Statistics for LogBL vs. LogF06 comparisons.

Model Slope ± SE Intercept ± SE R2 λ t d.f. P
PRIMATES (n = 41)
OLS −2.456 ± 0.233 7.177 ± 0.385 0.741 18.64 39 <0.0001
Y-on-X (BM) −1.681 ± 0.467 5.902 ± 0.869 −3.6 39 0.0009
X-on-Y (BM) −6.471 ± 0.041 14.347 ± 0.171 −3.6 39 0.0009
CARNIVORES (n = 33)
Model Slope ± SE Intercept ± SE R2 λ t d.f. P
OLS −1.004 ± 0.202 4.695 ± 0.386 0.443 −4.97 31 <0.0001
Y-on-X (NP) −1.004 ± 0.202 4.695 ± 0.386 0.443 −4.97 31 <0.0001
X-on-Y (NP) −2.265 ± 0.089 7.078 ± 0.251 0.443 −4.97 31 <0.0001
COMBINED (n = 74)
Model Slope ± SE Intercept ± SE R2 λ t d.f. P
OLS −1.483 ± 0.149 5.588 ± 0.265 0.579 −9.949 72 <0.0001
Y-on-X (BM + λ) −1.179 ± 0.227 5.06 ± 0.451 0.795 −5.19 72 <0.0001
X-on-Y (BM + λ) −5.441 ± 0.043 12.719 ± 0.181 0.989 −4.26 72 <0.0001

Format is the same as Table 2 (X = logBL, Y = logF06).

Aside from the differences between primates and carnivores, there were also clear differences between allometric relationships derived using DF and F0. Using F0 resulted in better regression fits and steeper slopes across all comparisons (cf. Figs 2, 3; see also Supplementary Fig. S3). Overall, F0 values tended to be lower than DF values, resulting in a downward shift of many of the data points in comparisons involving F0 relative to DF. This was particularly true for larger species, giving rise to steeper slopes for the F0 regressions. To examine the relationship between DF and F0 more closely, we calculated the ratio between DF and F0 for the subset of vocalizations used to calculate the F06 values (because these vocalizations had both F0 and DF values). The results of this analysis are shown in Fig. 4A. In accordance with the downward shift of data points observed in the F0 analyses, the DF/F0 ratio was found to be ≥1 for most vocalizations (95% for primates and 97% for carnivores). DF and F0 were approximately the same (i.e., DF/F0 ≈ 1) in 55% of the primate vocalizations and 36% of the carnivore vocalizations. When DF and F0 were not the same, DF corresponded to a harmonic multiple of F0 in a further 16% of the primate vocalizations and 28% of the carnivore vocalizations. These results show that F0 is frequently the most powerful component of mammalian vocalization (particularly for primates), and suggest that when it is not, vocal tract resonances may play an important role in determining the most powerful frequency content (see Fig. 1B).

Figure 4. DF/F0 ratios.

Figure 4

(A) Histograms of the DF/F0 ratios for each of the vocalizations used to calculate the F06 variable (n = 444) for 41 primate species (top) and 33 carnivore species (bottom). Histogram bins run from −0.125 to 11.125 in steps of 0.25. (B) The proportion of vocalizations (out of 6) in which DF/F0 ≈ 1 broken down by species.

The proportion of calls in which DF and F0 were the same is broken down by species in Fig. 4B. The prevalence of relatively large species towards the left-hand side and small species towards the right-hand side of these graphs suggests that the proportion of vocalizations in which DF/F0 = 1 may be related to body size. This possibility was tested by examining the correlation between body length and proportion of vocalizations in which DF/F0 = 1. The results indicated highly significant relationships for both primates and carnivores (Spearman’s r = −0.48, P = 0.0014, and r = −0.50, P = 0.0031 respectively). These results show that for the species considered here, the smaller an animal is the more likely it is that F0 will be the most powerful frequency component of their vocalizations.

Discussion

The results of the analyses presented here provide clear empirical verification of negative size-frequency allometry in primates, and the first empirical evidence for negative size-frequency allometry specifically in carnivores. Whether based on DF or F0, the relationships between primate body size and vocalization frequencies were considerably stronger than those previously reported (mean β = −2.18, mean R2 = 0.71 vs. for example β = −0.39, R2 = 0.54 in ref. 23), demonstrating advantages of our more rigorous methodology and showing that body size is even more closely related to vocalization frequencies in this order than previously shown. Intriguingly, these same relationships were much weaker for carnivores (mean β = −1.09, mean R2 = 0.37), suggesting that the capacity of fundamental and/or dominant frequency to signal body size in vocal communication should not be assumed to be equal across mammalian orders and should be examined independently in different clades whenever possible.

Focusing on the combined analyses of primates and carnivores, the relationship between body length and F0 derived here (see Fig. 3) can be compared with that between body mass and F0 derived in Charlton & Reby (2016), albeit with a different set of mammalian species (overlap = 12 primates and 4 carnivores)5. These relationships had very similar R2values (0.58 here vs. 0.59 in ref. 5), but the slopes of our regressions were considerably steeper (mean βs = −1.8 here vs. −0.5 in ref. 5). This remained true even when comparing the exact same regression model: the BM + λ regression was the best-supported model for F0 comparisons in both studies; prior to bisector calculation the β value for our model was −1.18 ( ± 0.23 SE; see Table 3) whereas the comparable β value in ref. 5 was −0.5 (±0.09 SE). While it is plausible that this difference was driven, at least in part, by our novel approach to collecting vocalization data, additional differences (e.g., in the index of body size and/or the species sampled) are also likely to have contributed.

Our results also show that, despite a strong tradition of using DF as the acoustic parameter in studies of size-frequency allometry (e.g., refs 15, 22, 23), F0 performs much better than DF at predicting body size among mammals. Further examination of the relationship between DF and F0 showed that these parameters are more likely to be the same in primate than carnivore vocalizations, and more likely to be the same in the vocalizations of smaller than larger species in both clades. The latter point is consistent with the fact that smaller species produce higher F0s. Higher F0s result in more widely spaced harmonics that are less likely to be emphasized by a vocal tract resonance.

Although it is presently unclear why the size-frequency relationships determined here are stronger for primates than carnivores, at least three possibilities come to mind. The first is that this finding may be partially artifactual. Carnivores have typically received less research attention than primates, and it is possible that the vocalization data compiled to represent them here somehow reflects this neglect. While our approach ensured equal representation for each species in the subset of selected vocalizations, it is possible that the vocal behavior of carnivores was less adequately represented in the original database from which the subset was selected. We did not, however, find any evidence in support of this possibility. Examination of the average number of high SNR vocalizations available for each species after preprocessing suggested better representation for primates (mean = 126, SD = 134) than carnivores (mean = 85, SD = 58), but this difference was not significant (Mann-Whitney U = 2494, n1 = 41, n2 = 50, P = 0.12). Nevertheless, it remains possible that there were differences between orders in how well the selected vocalizations represented the vocal ranges of the species in question.

A second possibility is that the differences observed here between primates and carnivores reflect real ecological differences between these clades, for example in habitat, social structure, diet and/or lifestyle5. Although within-clade variation for any one of these dimensions is quite large, variation between clades is likely to be larger, and thus could plausibly support important differences in the selective pressures acting on vocal production and behavior in these groups.

A final possibility, not mutually exclusive with the second, is that weaker size-frequency allometry in carnivores reflects greater variability in vocal behavior. Some evidence in support of this possibility is that the average number of non-tonal vocalizations in our selected subset was significantly greater for carnivores (mean = 3.2/10, SD = 2.1) than for primates (2.3/10; SD = 2.6; Mann-Whitney U = 2021.5, n1 = 41, n2 = 50, P = 0.02). The more frequent occurrence of non-tonal vocalizations in carnivores indicates less uniformity in acoustic structure and, potentially, a greater reliance on nonlinear modes of vocal production. While either of these factors would contribute noise to comparisons between DF and body length, they would presumably not affect comparisons between F0 and body length. The relative weakness of this later relationship in carnivores therefore remains a puzzle.

The influence of different modes of vocal production also provides a plausible answer to the question of why the size-frequency relationships established here were stronger for F0 than DF. The DF of a vocalization is ambiguously determined through the interaction of multiple vocal production mechanisms. For example, if F0 is the frequency of maximal amplitude, DF will correspond the lowest rate of vocal fold vibration; but if the frequency of maximal amplitude corresponds to the peak of a formant resonance, or a particular harmonic near that peak, DF will correspond instead to a particular vocal tract configuration, or an interaction between both vocal fold vibration and vocal tract configuration. The fact that DF is not directly related to any single aspect of vocal anatomy limits its usefulness in allometric comparisons (but not necessarily acoustic communication) because it cannot be expected to vary systematically with any single measurement. Using DF is thus likely to introduce noise into allometric comparisons. Using F0 instead addresses this problem because the F0 of a vocalization is determined by the rate of vocal fold vibration and can thus be expected to vary systematically with vocal fold length9. In addition to DF and F0, it should be noted that the spacing of formant frequencies is an important acoustic indicator of body size that was not examined here5,50. An assessment of formant frequencies across the variety of species and vocalizations included here was complicated by the fact that accurate measurement of formants typically requires calls with broadband source vibration51, which was only the case for a small subset of our vocalizations (and disproportionately from larger animals).

A final point concerns species that appeared as consistent outliers in both DF and F0 regressions. Among primates, these species included the howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya, A. seniculus and A. sara), the proboscis monkey (Nasalis larvatus) and the putty-nosed monkey (Cercopithecus nictitans), all of which exhibited lower DF and/or F0 values than expected on the basis of their body lengths (see Figs 2 and 3). In the case of the howler monkeys, these differences can be understood in terms of well-documented laryngeal hypertrophy52,53. In the case of the proboscis monkey, an elongated nasal appendage may provide an explanation for exceptionally low DF values, but the causal basis for their exceptionally low F0 values as well as those of the putty-nosed monkey remains unknown. Among carnivores, consistently outlying species included the spectacled bear (Tremarctos ornatus) and the African hunting dog (Lycaon pictus), which exhibited higher vocalization frequencies than predicted on the basis of body length, as well as the least weasel (Mustela nivalis) and the stoat (Mustela erminea), which exhibited lower vocalization frequencies than predicted. We are unaware of any documented specializations in the vocal anatomy of these species. The anatomical factors underlying their exceptional DF and F0 values may thus represent opportunities to further our understanding of biological adaptation in vocal communication.

In conclusion, the results presented here provide unequivocal empirical evidence of negative size-frequency allometry in mammals using novel methods to derive clear and consistent vocalization data across primates and carnivores. In accordance with the founding assumption of Morton’s motivation-structural rules hypothesis6, these results suggest that across a wide range of body sizes both the dominant and fundamental frequency of vocalizations are inherently related to the size of vocalizers, implying that biological associations based on these relationships are indeed likely to be adaptive. Thus, as for the other clades examined so far, for primates and carnivores negative size-frequency allometry appears to be a fundamental aspect of the bio-acoustic landscape. However, our results also suggest caution in applying the details of any specific relationship too broadly: even in the phylogenetically limited set of mammalian species considered here, large differences were apparent between biological orders.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Bowling, D. L. et al. Body size and vocalization in primates and carnivores. Sci. Rep. 7, 41070; doi: 10.1038/srep41070 (2017).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Information
srep41070-s1.pdf (1,011.6KB, pdf)

Acknowledgments

We thank Benjamin D. Charlton, Jeroen Smaers, and Max Kerney for their assistance in preparing the phylogenetic regressions. We also thank the Senda Verde Animal Reserve Bolivia for providing the vocalization data for Alouatta sara. This work was funded in part by ERC Advanced grant SOMACCA (ADG230604) to WTF, an Austrian FWF Lise Meitner Programme fellowship (M1773) to DLB, the Cambridge Humanities Research Grant Scheme (JCD), and The University of Vienna (MG).

Footnotes

Author Contributions D.L.B., W.T.F. and R.R. designed the study. D.L.B., R.R., A.S. and K.F. collected the data. D.L.B. and M.G. performed the acoustic analyses. D.L.B., J.C.D. and M.G. performed the statistical analyses. D.L.B. wrote the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript.

References

  1. Darwin C. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex (John Murray, 1871). [Google Scholar]
  2. Negus V. E. The comparative anatomy and physiology of the larynx (Hafner publishing company, 1949). [Google Scholar]
  3. Schneider R. Der Larynx der Säugetiere. Handb. der Zool. 5, 1–128 (1964). [Google Scholar]
  4. Puts D. A. et al. Sexual selection on male vocal fundamental frequency in humans and other anthropoids. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 283, 0–7 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Charlton B. D. & Reby D. The evolution of acoustic size exaggeration in terrestrial mammals. Nat. Commun. 7, 12739 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Morton E. S. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am. Nat. 111, 855–869 (1977). [Google Scholar]
  7. Schmidt-Nielsen K. Scaling: why is animal size so important? (Cambridge University Press, 1984). [Google Scholar]
  8. Fitch W. T. The evolution of language (Cambridge University Press, 2010). [Google Scholar]
  9. Titze I. R. Principles of voice production (National Center for Voice and Speech, 2000). [Google Scholar]
  10. Wallschläger D. Correlation of song frequency and body weight in passerine birds. Experientia 36, 412–412 (1980). [Google Scholar]
  11. Gingras B., Boeckle M., Herbst C. T. & Fitch W. T. S. Call acoustics reflect body size across four clades of anurans. J. Zool. 289, 143–150 (2013). [Google Scholar]
  12. Davies N. B. & Halliday T. R. Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274, 683–685 (1978). [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans S., Neave N. & Wakelin D. Relationships between vocal characteristics and body size and shape in human males: an evolutionary explanation for a deep male voice. Biol. Psychol. 72, 160–163 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hall M. L., Kingma S. A. & Peters A. Male songbird indicates body size with low-pitched advertising songs. PLoS One 8, 1–5 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Sullivan B. K. Advertisement call variation and observations on breeding behavior of Bufo debilis and B. punctatus. J. Herpetol. 18, 406–411 (1984). [Google Scholar]
  16. Asquith A. & Altig R. Male call frequency as a criterion for female choice in Hyla cinerea. J. Herpetol. 24, 198–201 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  17. Patel R., Mulder R. A. & Cardoso G. C. What makes vocalisation frequency an unreliable signal of body size in birds? A study on black swans. Ethology 116, 554–563 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  18. Rendall D., Kollias S., Ney C. & Lloyd P. Pitch (F0) and formant profiles of human vowels and vowel-like baboon grunts: the role of vocalizer body size and voice-acoustic allometry. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 117, 944–955 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fletcher N. H. A simple frequency-scaling rule for animal communication. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 2334–2338 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hammerschmidt K. & Fischer J. The vocal repertoire of Barbary macaques: a quantative analysis of a graded signal system. Ethology 104, 203–216 (1998). [Google Scholar]
  21. Garcia M. et al. Structural classification of wild boar (Sus scrofa) vocalizations. Ethology 122, 329–342 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jones G. Does echolocation constrain the evolution of body size in bats? In Miniature vertebrates, the implicactions of small body size: Zoological Society of London Symposia 69 (ed. Miller P. J.) 111–128 (Clarendon Press, 1996). [Google Scholar]
  23. Hauser M. D. The evolution of nonhuman primate vocalizations: effects of phylogeny, body weight, and social context. Am. Nat. 142, 528–42 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Felsenstein J. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat 125, 3–147 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Wilson D. E. & Mittermeier R. A. Handbook of the mammals of the world - Volume 1 Carnivores (Lynx Edicions, 2009). [Google Scholar]
  26. Mittermeier R. A., Rylands A. & Wilson D. E. Handbook of the mammals of the world - Volume 3 Primates (Lynx Edicions, 2013). [Google Scholar]
  27. MacDonald D. & Norris S. The new encyclopedia of mammals (Oxford University Press, 2001). [Google Scholar]
  28. Bogin B. & Varela-Silva M. I. Leg length, body proportion, and health: A review with a note on beauty. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 7, 1047–1075 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Bouchain C. & Gautier J. P. Le monde des singes - 2: singes forestiers/Primate world -2: forest monkeys [Audio CD]. (Sittelle, 1995).
  30. Dingler K.-H. & Frommolt K.-H. Die Stimmen der Wölfe, Schakale und Hunde [Audio CD]. (Musikverlag Edition AMPLE, 2010). [Google Scholar]
  31. Dingler K.-H. & Frommolt K.-H. Die Stimmen von 20 Groß-und Kleinbären [Audio CD]. (Musikverlag Edition AMPLE, 2011). [Google Scholar]
  32. Emmons L. H., Whitney B. M. & Ross D. L. Sounds of Neotropical rainforest mammals: an audio field guide [Audio CD]. (Library of Naural Sounds Cornell Lab of Ornithology in association with Conservation International, 1997). [Google Scholar]
  33. Gautier-Hion A., Colyn M. & Gautier J.-P. Histoire Natuelle des primates d’Afrique Centrale [Audio CD] (ECOFAC, 1999). [Google Scholar]
  34. Delta Music Inc. 101 Digital sound effects: sounds of nature [Audio CD] (Delta Music Inc., 1993). [Google Scholar]
  35. Ludwig R. C. Animals of Africa: sounds of the jungle, plain, and bush. [Audio CD] (Nonesuch Records, 2002). [Google Scholar]
  36. Pott E. & Roché J. C. Der Wald und seine Stimmen [Audio CD] (Franckh-Kosmos Verlags-GmbH & Co., 1999). [Google Scholar]
  37. Ried A. Animal calls of Africa [Audio CD] (Gillard bird cassette, 1997). [Google Scholar]
  38. Roché J. C. Le monde des singes - 1 [Audio CD] (Sittelle, 1994). [Google Scholar]
  39. Bowling D. L., Gingras B., Han S., Sundararajan J. & Opitz E. C. L. Tone of voice in emotional expression: relevance for the affective character of musical mode. J. Interdiscip. Music Stud. 7, 29–44 (2013). [Google Scholar]
  40. Boersma P. & Weenink D. Praat: doing phonetics by computer. at http://www.praat.org/ (2016). [Google Scholar]
  41. Fitch W. T., Neubauer J. & Herzel H. Calls out of chaos: the adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production. Anim. Behav. 63, 407–418 (2002). [Google Scholar]
  42. Harvey P. H. & Pagel M. D. The comparative method in evolutionary biology (Oxford University Press, 1991). [Google Scholar]
  43. Symonds M. R. E. & Blomberg S. P. In Modern phylogenetic comparative methods (ed. Garamszegi L. Z.) 105–130 (Springer, 2014). [Google Scholar]
  44. Isobe T., Feigelson E. D., Akritas M. G. & Babu G. J. Linear regression in astromony. I. Astrophys. J. 364, 104–113 (1990). [Google Scholar]
  45. Arnold C., Matthews L. J. & Nunn C. L. The 10kTrees website: A new online resource for primate phylogeny. Evol. Anthropol. 19, 114–118 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  46. R core team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. at https://www.r-project.org/(2015).
  47. Pinheiro J., Bates D., DebRoy S., Sarkar D. & Team R. C. nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. (2016).
  48. Paradis E., Claude J. & Strimmer K. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics 20, 289–290 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mazerolle M. J. AICcmodavg: Model selection and multimodel inference based on (Q)AIC(c).
  50. Fitch W. T. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 1213–22 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lieberman P. & Blumstein S. E. Speech physiology, speech perception, and acoustic phonetics (Cambridge University Press, 1988). [Google Scholar]
  52. Schön M. A. The anatomy of the resonating mechanism in howling monkeys. Folia Primatol. 15, 117–132 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Dunn J. C. et al. Evolutionary trade-off between vocal tract and testes dimensions in howler monkeys. Curr. Biol. 25, 2839–2844 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information
srep41070-s1.pdf (1,011.6KB, pdf)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES