Skip to main content
Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring logoLink to Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring
. 2016 Dec 22;6:40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2016.12.004

Cognitive complaints in older adults at risk for Alzheimer's disease are associated with altered resting-state networks

Joey A Contreras a,b,c,1, Joaquín Goñi a,b,d,e,1, Shannon L Risacher a,b, Enrico Amico f, Karmen Yoder a, Mario Dzemidzic a,g, John D West a,b, Brenna C McDonald a,b,g, Martin R Farlow b,g, Olaf Sporns a,b,c,h, Andrew J Saykin a,b,c,g,
PMCID: PMC5266473  PMID: 28149942

Abstract

Introduction

Pathophysiological changes that accompany early clinical symptoms in prodromal Alzheimer's disease (AD) may have a disruptive influence on brain networks. We investigated resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI), combined with brain connectomics, to assess changes in whole-brain functional connectivity (FC) in relation to neurocognitive variables.

Methods

Participants included 58 older adults who underwent rsfMRI. Individual FC matrices were computed based on a 278-region parcellation. FastICA decomposition was performed on a matrix combining all subjects' FC. Each FC pattern was then used as a response in a multilinear regression model including neurocognitive variables associated with AD (cognitive complaint index [CCI] scores from self and informant, an episodic memory score, and an executive function score).

Results

Three connectivity independent component analysis (connICA) components (RSN, VIS, and FP-DMN FC patterns) associated with neurocognitive variables were identified based on prespecified criteria. RSN-pattern, characterized by increased FC within all resting-state networks, was negatively associated with self CCI. VIS-pattern, characterized by an increase in visual resting-state network, was negatively associated with CCI self or informant scores. FP-DMN-pattern, characterized by an increased interaction of frontoparietal and default mode networks (DMN), was positively associated with verbal episodic memory.

Discussion

Specific patterns of FC were differently associated with neurocognitive variables thought to change early in the course of AD. An integrative connectomics approach relating cognition to changes in FC may help identify preclinical and early prodromal stages of AD and help elucidate the complex relationship between subjective and objective indices of cognitive decline and differences in brain functional organization.

Keywords: Connectome, Functional connectivity, MRI, Memory, Subjective cognitive decline, Mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease

1. Introduction

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is often recognized as a disconnection disorder where plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, and neurodegeneration of the brain lead to reduced communication and coordination among regions important for cognition [1]. Therefore, brain connectomics studies designed to examine disruptions of connectivity in AD have become increasingly common [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7]. Brain connectomics is a field of study that uses graph theory to analyze the ensemble of brain connectivity networks, including functional networks. While neurodegenerative diseases are often studied in a focal manner (i.e., a particular region of interest [ROI] or relatively small neural circuit), brain connectomics provides a more complete systematic view by modeling the entire human brain as a set of networks and assessing whole-brain organizational changes or disruptions.

Functional connectivity (FC) estimates the level of functional coupling of regional brain activity, measured as time series [8], among brain regions regardless of distance or structural connections [9]. This method uses resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) data to measure correlations in low-frequency blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal fluctuations between distinct brain regions. Implementation of this technique combined with a gray-matter parcellation allows for extraction of whole-brain FC data. Brain regions can then be further organized into resting-state networks (RSNs), which have been shown to correlate with well-described brain functions and are defined by coordinated neural activity between distinct, spatially separate brain regions [10], [11], [12], [13]. Grouping brain regions according to RSN allows for dimension reduction and provides psychophysiologically relevant results [14]. There is evidence that abnormalities in brain networks appear in early stages of AD [15], [16], [17]. Because current evidence indicates that neurodegenerative changes begin before clinical manifestations become apparent [18], it is crucial to identify the earliest changes during preclinical and prodromal stages of disease. Investigating early stages of AD from a FC connectomics perspective may give rise to new hypotheses about underlying pathophysiology that cannot be ascertained using data from isolated regions or circuits [7], [19].

Therefore, this study examined FC differences across the continuous spectrum of early-stage AD, from at-risk and prodromal stages to clinical AD dementia with significant cognitive and functional impairment. Participants included those categorized as experiencing subjective cognitive decline (SCD) [20], as well as mild cognitively impaired (MCI) individuals and those diagnosed with AD. SCD participants reported a significant burden of subjective decline in memory and cognition in the absence of psychometric deficits [20], [21]. As individuals age, there are increased reports of self-perceived memory decline compared with earlier periods in life [22], [23]. However, older adults with SCD have a greater number of complaints and may even subjectively consider their own cognition to be impaired relative to their peers despite objective cognitive test performance (i.e., SCD individuals are only clinically distinguishable from healthy participants in the number of reported cognitive complaints). Relatively high levels of these memory complaints or concerns have been regarded as relevant to the diagnosis of prodromal AD and are believed to be predictive of subsequent development of amnestic MCI and, at later stages, AD. Therefore, early detection of network changes associated with cognitive complaints could help inform diagnosis and treatment planning and may be useful as a biomarker for enrichment in or as an end point for therapeutic trials.

Because neurodegeneration typically progresses gradually in AD, with subtle transitions between preclinical and prodromal stages rather than discrete changes [24], [25], [26], we applied a recently developed analytic framework to identify FC network patterns across early stages of AD. This data-driven methodology, denominated connectivity independent component analysis (connICA) [27] and detailed in the methods section, is distinct from voxel-level and a priori region–based independent component analyses (ICAs) [28]. Our approach can uncover inherent and independent FC patterns that represent different functional RSN features within the population (our group cohort). In addition, it includes an ability to test associations of these FC patterns with subjective and psychometric cognitive parameters relevant to AD. The latter is particularly useful when investigating a continuum of states in which stratification into diagnostic groups may not be clear-cut because of the inherent complexity of diagnostic criteria.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The Indiana University Institutional Review Board approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were older adults from a larger cohort recruited for a longitudinal study of brain aging and memory (Indiana Memory and Aging Study) and were included based on the availability of resting-state imaging data during their first visit. The present sample included 16 participants with significant cognitive complaints despite cognitive test performance within the normal range (SCD group) [21], 21 participants with amnestic MCI, 8 AD patients, and 13 cognitively normal (CN) controls with minimal cognitive complaints. Further details regarding participant recruitment, selection criteria, and characterization are described in previous reports [21], [29] and in Table 1. No differences were found between groups regarding age and education (one-way analysis of variance [ANOVA]) or gender (chi-squared test).

Table 1.

Demographics

CN SCD MCI AD P-value
N 13 16 21 8
Mean age (y) 67.15 (5.50) 73.38 (7.95) 73.33 (8.98) 76.38 (8.98) NS
Gender (M/F) 1/12 8/8 9/13 2/6 NS
Mean education (y) 17.32 (1.93) 17.38 (1.9) 16 (2.8) 16.13 (3.7) NS
Mean CVLT-delayed (episodic memory) 44.21 (7.66) 43.59 (7.41) 29.56 (8.1) 9.25 (11.75) <.05
Mean WCST—# of categories (executive function) 4.1 (1.19) 3.5 (1.41) 2.67 (1.49) 1.17 (0.41) <.05
Mean subject CCI score 9.97 (4.54) 27.92 (16.01) 20.85 (11.76) 51.29 (28.38) <.05
Mean informant CCI score 7.7 (11.46) 15.82 (13.41) 14.54 (16.1) N/A <.05

Abbreviations: CN, cognitively normal; SCD, subjective cognitive decline; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer's disease; NS, not significant; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; CCI, cognitive complaint index; N/A, not applicable.

NOTE. The participants of this study were selected from a larger Indiana Memory and Aging Study cohort. Above are the listed means and standard deviations within each group for selected demographic and neurocognitive variables. Between-group differences in age, education, and cognitive variables were tested using a one-way analysis of variance, while chi-squared test was used to detect gender differences between groups.

One-way ANOVA.

Chi-squared.

2.2. Neurocognitive variables of interest

All participants underwent a comprehensive clinical assessment and neuropsychological battery. Five cognitive variables were included in the present analysis: (1) the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) long-delay free recall [30], a measure of episodic memory; (2) the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) correct number of categories, a measure of executive function [31]; (3 and 4) a cognitive complaint index (CCI) score from both the participant (“self”) and an informant calculated as the percentage of all items given that was endorsed as a complaint [21]; and (5) the highest CCI score from either self or informant, labeled CCImax (see Table 2), included to address the diminished self-awareness that may occur in later stages of AD. Significant differences between groups were detected in all cognitive measures in the expected direction using a one-way ANOVA (see Table 1).

Table 2.

Neurocognitive variables of interest

Assessment Brain region affected Effect in AD How it is measured
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)—# of categories Frontal lobe: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex; parietal lobe Executive function declines with the disease progression Stimulus cards are presented, and the participant is told to match the cards, but not how to do the matching
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)-II Medial temporal lobe: hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, etc.; precuneus; medial prefrontal cortex MCI: loss of both immediate and delayed episodic memory recall; AD: episodic memory is devastated 16 nouns read aloud over five learning trials. After each trial, the subject is asked to recall as many words as they can in any order—then again after a delay
Cognitive complaint index (CCI) score (Saykin et al., Neurology. 2006) Negative correlation with the hippocampal volume (Saykin et al., 2006) Early-stage patients have many complaints. As AD progresses, complaints decrease due to loss of cognitive awareness Combined score across multiple assessments that allows participant (self) or informant to subjectively rate cognitive function; CCImax: the higher complaint score either self- or informant-provided

Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer's disease; MCI, mild cognitive impairment.

NOTE. The following variables were included in the multilinear regression model as independent variables: (1) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test score to evaluate executive function; (2) CVLT II-delayed score to measure episodic memory, (3) cognitive complaint index score allowing either the participant or a trusted informant to subjectively rate cognitive function. Also listed are affected brain regions, effects in AD, and more details about each score measure.

2.3. MRI data acquisition

All subjects were imaged on a 3T Siemens Tim Trio MRI scanner with a 12-channel head coil array. A detailed anatomical magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) with whole-brain coverage was acquired using a 3D MPRAGE sequence (repetition time [TR]/echo time [TE] = 2300/2.98 ms, 220 sagittal slices with slice thickness = 1.2 mm, 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm voxels) following the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative imaging protocol [32]. rsfMRI data were collected with instructions given to subjects to think of nothing in particular while remaining still with their eyes closed. Whole-brain functional images were obtained using a Siemens product gradient echo sequence (echo planar imaging [EPI]; scan duration 6 minutes 9 seconds; 161 BOLD contrast-sensitive volumes; TR/TE = 2250 ms/29 ms; 2.5 × 2.5 × 3.5 mm3 voxels; 39 interleaved axial slices with 3.5 mm thickness and no gap; GRAPPA acceleration factor 2; 3D PACE prospective motion correction [33]).

2.4. BOLD preprocessing

rsfMRI preprocessing procedures, including head motion, were adapted from Power et al. [34]. These included slice timing correction, registration to MPRAGE volume (FLIRT six degrees of freedom [DOFs] and boundary-based registration), detrending, band-pass filtering (0.009–0.08 Hz), and intensity normalization to mode 1000. In addition to including motion regressors from the realignment and their derivatives, three image quality control measures (framewise displacement, DVARS, and standard deviation) were applied to identify and remove (“scrubbing”) outlier BOLD volumes (D referring to temporal derivative of timecourses, VARS referring to RMS variance over voxels, DVARS). EPI volumes with an excessive (>50%) fraction of outliers were dropped from subsequent analyses. This resulted in the exclusion of imaging data from seven subjects (one CN, two SCD, and four MCI), with the remaining 58 subjects comprising the final sample. The first five signals obtained from principal component analysis from three tissue compartments were regressed out to address confounding effects of physiologic noise and residual head motion. This procedure relies on eroded masks in the whole-brain gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid of the third ventricle and includes global signal regression [35].

The T1-weighted MPRAGE volume of each participant was used to extract the brain (FSL “BET”) [36] and perform a sequence of transformations (FSL's FLIRT 6 DOFs, FLIRT 12 DOFs, and nonlinear FNIRT) to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template to provide tissue segmentation. The inverse MNI-to-native transformation then enabled functionally derived brain parcellation [37] into 278 cortical and subcortical ROIs to be performed in native rsfMRI space. Correlation coefficients (Pearson r) between mean BOLD time series of each ROI pair were sorted into a 278 × 278 FC matrix for each participant and used to quantify the degree of connectivity between the paired ROIs. Brain regions were then further ordered into seven independently well-defined cortical RSNs [14], with subcortical and cerebellar regions added for a total of nine networks (Supplemental Fig. S1). This effectively results in a 9 × 9 FC matrix, where the nine RSNs compromise the cells on the diagonal, and the remaining cells termed “off-diagonal.”

2.5. connICA method

connICA [27] is a data-driven method based on ICA to obtain cohort-level independent “FC patterns” while estimating the presence of each pattern in an individual subject. Here, ICA is applied directly onto the human connectome domain (as opposed to time-series domain at the voxel level). Specifically, ICA input consists of FC profiles of all participants sorted into a single data set matrix with 58 rows (one row per subject) and 38,503 columns corresponding to the upper triangular elements of each subject's FC matrix (see Fig. 1). These independent patterns extracted from the full FC data set then represent “independent connectome subsystems” that characterize the whole sample and comprise FC profiles of all subjects without any a priori group stratification. ICA decomposition into FC pattern matrix was performed with the fastICA algorithm [38], where the number of components was fixed to 15 as guided by voxel-based ICA studies [39]. Owing to a nondeterministic nature of ICA, we performed 100 connICA runs and kept only independent component patterns deemed to be “robust”, that is, appearing in at least 75 out of the 100 runs. In particular, “appearance” required a Pearson correlation coefficient above 0.75 (absolute value) with respect to the first run. This method provides two outputs. The first one, “FC pattern,” represents an independent pattern of FC similar to the representation of each subject's FC—a square symmetric matrix with brain regions in rows and columns (and therefore referenced as the FC pattern). However, the FC pattern matrix values are in connectivity units (“loadings”), with their range not restricted to the [−1,1] range, unlike the Pearson correlation coefficient values from the individual FC matrices. The second connICA output is a vector of the signed weights of the FC pattern present on each participant, which quantifies the prominence/presence of the trait in each individual FC matrix as illustrated by Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Connectivity independent component analysis (connICA) methodology. Individual functional connectivity (FC) matrices are concatenated into a group matrix where each row corresponds to one subject and columns are the functional connectivity entries in the FC matrix. FastICA extracts components (i.e., FC patterns) associated to the cohort and their relative weights across subjects. Color bars indicate positive (red) and negative (blue) values; Pearson correlation coefficient values for individual FC matrices (left side of figure) and unit-less connectivity weights for the FC patterns (right side of figure).

connICA yielded six FC patterns. To establish the biological relevance of these patterns, we organized brain regions into canonical 9 × 9 RSNs as illustrated by Fig. S2 and detailed in Section 2.4. A pattern was considered in further analyses only if the average FC loadings within at least one element included a significant similarity with either one of the canonical RSNs or their interaction (Supplemental Fig. S2, diagonal or off-diagonal elements, respectively). Specifically, the significance testing included the following steps: (1) generating a frequency histogram of FC loadings from the matrices of all six patterns, with each pattern contributing (278*277)/2 values (not including the diagonal); (2) setting a threshold value at the 95th percentile of the FC value distribution; (3) calculating the average FC loading for each pattern matrix in the 9 × 9 RSN format; and (4) testing a selection criterion that at least one canonically defined RSN cell must have an average FC value that equals or exceeds the 95th percentile value of 0.72. This significance criterion was met by three out of six FC patterns.

2.6. Statistical analysis

The statistical inferences based on multilinear regression analysis were performed using MATLAB [40] function “regress”. The subject weights associated with each FC pattern were entered as a response (dependent) variable in an incremental multilinear regression model with up to five predictors (independent variables). The predictors included nuisance variables, including age, gender, and years of education, and a single neurocognitive variable of interest at a time (CVLT long-delay free-recall score, WCST correct number of categories score, CCI-self score, CCI-informant score, and CCImax score). To assess the level of predictability that each neurocognitive variable of interest added, we compared the R2 coefficient value of the multiple regression model run with the neurocognitive variable of interest to the R2 value of the model containing only nuisance variables as predictors.

Finally, the aforementioned cognitive variables that significantly increased the predictability of the chosen FC pattern were identified (P ≤ .05) to extract FC patterns most associated with cognitive performance and subjective cognitive ratings, which would likely be most sensitive to functional brain changes in preclinical and prodromal AD.

3. Results

3.1. Identification of FC patterns

The three FC patterns identified using connICA and passing the prespecified selection criteria were (1) resting-state network or RSN-pattern; (2) visual network or VIS-pattern; and (3) frontoparietal and default mode networks or FP-DMN pattern. RSN-pattern primarily captures a within-network coherence of the RSNs introduced by Yeo et al [14]. The VIS-pattern was characterized primarily by a significant increase of within-network connectivity in the visual network. Lastly, FP-DMN pattern primarily showed an increased interaction between DMNs and frontoparietal networks (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Robust FC patterns and individual weights as obtained by connICA. (A) Visualization of the FC patterns sorted according to Yeo et al. (2011) functional RSNs. (B) Lines represent the quantified presence of each FC pattern on each individual's functional connectome (across all runs), termed as “weights.” All 58 subjects are represented along the x axis and ordered according to subjects with high presence of the corresponding FC pattern within their functional connectome to those with low presence of the FC pattern within their functional connectome. Additionally, each colored line represents a single ICA run.

3.2. Relationship of neurocognitive variables of interest and FC patterns

As expected, the CN group had the highest CVLT-delayed and WCST performance followed by the SCD group, MCI, and AD groups (Table 1). Similarly, CCI scores differed across diagnoses, in part, by definition as cognitive complaints are part of the criteria for SCD and MCI.

The multilinear regression models including age, gender, education, and neurocognitive variables of interest from Table 2 were performed for each of the three identified FC patterns, with significant results summarized in Table 3.

Table 3.

Significance values for multilinear regression models

Model tested F-value for model P-value for model t-Value for predictor P-value for predictor
RSN model 3.40 .015*
 Age −1.69 .097
 Gender 0.29 .773
 Education 0.55 .582
 CCI-self −2.39 .020*
VIS model 1.35 .267
 Age 0.44 .659
 Gender 0.84 .407
 Education −0.6 .550
 CCImax 2.04 .047*
FP-DMN model 1.96 .116
 Age −0.23 .823
 Gender 0.63 .535
 Education 2.47 .017*
 CVLT delayed 0.27 .791
NOTE. Table shows significantly predictive multilinear regression models for the three FC patterns.
RSNmodel:RSNpatternweights=b0+b1age1+b2sex2+b3educ3+b4cognitivevariable4
VISmodel:VISpatternweights=b0+b1age1+b2sex2+b3educ3+b4cognitive variable4
FP-DMNmodel:FP-DMNpatternweights=b0+b1age1+b2sex2+b3educ3+b4cognitivevariable4.

NOTE. Variables included show added significance to individual models. Significance denoted by asterisk (P < .05).

The first significant finding was observed in the model that included age, gender, education, and CCI-self scores as predictors for subject's weights of the RSN-pattern (Fig. 3A, top panel). The multiple regression model with only nuisance variables as predictors yielded an R2 value of 0.11. However, the inclusion of CCI-self score increased the R2 value to 0.20 (Fig. 3A, middle panel), which was significantly predictive of RSN-pattern weight (Fig. 3A, bottom panel). Specifically, we found that a higher level of self-perceived cognitive decline (e.g., more complaints) was associated with a lower subject weight in the RSN-pattern.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Relationship of FC patterns and neurocognitive variables of interest. Visualization of the three identified FC patterns (top row). The contributions of neurocognitive variables of interest showing significant increase of the baseline R2 value in the multiple regression models are presented in the bar plots (middle row). A grouped bar plot where black bars indicate the baseline (only age, gender, and education) R2 value, while the hatched patterned bars indicate when an individual neurocognitive variable of interest has been added. The standard error bars were calculated across the 100 ICA runs. R2 value is shown above the cognitive variable that has the greatest increase from the baseline R2 value. The scatter plots (bottom row) show actual versus model-predicted subject weights with different symbols indicating group membership. The multilinear regression models include age, gender, education, and one of the neurocognitive variables of interest. The three columns illustrate the relationships of (A) RSN-pattern and CCI-self score; (B) VIS-pattern and CCImax score, and (C) FP-DMN pattern and CVLT score. These relationships are further detailed in the Results section. Significance denoted by asterisk (P < .05).

The second significant multilinear regression model included age, gender, education, and CCImax scores as predictors for subjects' weights for the VIS-pattern (Fig. 3B, top panel). If only nuisance variables were used as predictors for the subjects' weights of VIS-pattern, the R2 value was 0.01. However, the R2 value increased to 0.10 with the addition of CCImax as a predictor, with CCImax significantly predictive within the model for VIS-pattern weights (Fig. 3B, bottom and middle panels). In this analysis, we observed that a higher maximum complaint score (reported from either the subject or a knowledgeable informant; CCImax) was associated with a lower subject weight score in the VIS-pattern.

The third significant multilinear regression model included age, gender, education, and CVLT scores as predictors for subjects' weights of the FP-DMN-pattern (Fig. 3C, top and bottom panels). Using only nuisance variables as predictors yielded an R2 value of 0.11. CVLT-delayed score increased the R2 value to 0.14 (Fig. 3C, middle panel). In other words, the better the episodic memory performance (i.e., higher CVLT-delayed score), the more that participant exhibited a pattern of increased communication between two important RSNs known to be involved in episodic memory, the DMN and frontoparietal network.

4. Discussion

ICA is often used to evaluate the spatiotemporal structure of BOLD data and to extract functional connections from voxels' time series. In a similar vein, we applied a novel, data-driven connICA methodology directly onto functional connectome data sets to model and tease apart common underlying FC patterns of healthy and diseased human brains. We successfully extracted independent FC patterns and quantified the degree to which each pattern was present in each individual's functional connectome. Those individual estimations or pattern weights were used as the response variables in multilinear regression models (one per FC pattern) with nuisance covariates and cognitive performance variables included as predictors. This approach allowed a continuous mapping across different stages of AD within functional connectomes.

Of the initial six robust independent FC patterns, three (RSN, VIS, and FP-DMN) were considered biologically relevant and retained for further consideration. All three FC patterns showed associations with neurocognitive status including performance scores. Specifically, we found that SCD measures, considered to be among the earliest indicators of risk for AD, were negatively associated with FC in several important RSNs. These data indicate the potential utility of connectomics approaches in early detection and diagnosis.

The RSN-pattern, which resembles the canonically defined RSNs [14], was negatively associated with CCI-self score after controlling for age, gender, and education. In other words, lower FC within the RSN-pattern was seen in participants with more SCD (i.e., increased complaints). This finding is highly relevant because in early stages of AD, self and informant concerns may precede clinically significant psychometric deficits in cognition, and cognitive complaints are believed to confer an increased risk of cognitive impairment and progression to dementia [20], [21]. Our finding suggests that disordered brain function, characterized by decreased coherence in major RSNs, may be related to self-perceived cognitive decline. This result further adds to growing literature that SCD may indeed reflect neuronal changes and suggests that RSN coherence may serve as a good biological predictor of decline [20], [21], [41]. If FC patterns could be used to identify individuals in a preclinical state of AD, then this technique has the potential to enhance therapeutic or preventative trials to identify disease-modifying treatments that can stop or slow clinical progression [42], [43].

The VIS-pattern had a strong negative association with CCImax score. In other words, higher levels of SCD (taken from either the subject or the informant) are associated with lower within-network FC in the visual network. This result provides evidence that impairment in brain areas responsible for visual cognition may be associated with early indications of cognitive impairment. This is in agreement with studies that have found similar links between visual impairment and early neurodegeneration [29], [44]. In fact, tests of visual cognition may be particularly sensitive to the effects of AD [45]. Importantly, we may be able to use the integrity of the FC in the visual network as a diagnostic marker to better characterize or identify early sensory impairments caused by early AD that may result in decreased quality of life for patients.

Lastly, features of FC in FP-DMN-pattern, which was characterized by strong connectivity of FC between the frontoparietal network and DMN, were positively associated with episodic memory. This result is similar to those from previous studies that suggest that interdependent activity of DMN and parietal regions are involved in memory retrieval [46], [47]. Thus, our findings and others suggest that strong connections between brain regions in the frontoparietal network and in the DMN are important for better episodic memory performance.

There are limitations to the present study. The sample size is modest, and there is unequal sampling across groups (CN, SCD, MCI, and AD). Although we did not analyze results based on group, these factors could affect the relative distribution of neurocognitive scores across the sample. Future studies with significantly larger overall and subgroup samples are warranted. Second, we elected to use a specific brain parcellation scheme [37] and referenced our results to a particular set of RSNs [14]. It is possible that alternative anatomic and functional templates would return different results. Thus, future studies including multiple brain parcellation schemes and RSN definitions would help determine the validity of our findings. Third, the ICA approach is still being optimized, in particular with regards to the upper limit of components that the algorithm can extract. Fourth, permutation tests and cross-validation of the clinical cohort would enable nonparametric estimation of significance [48] and provide a data-driven approach to multiple testing correction. Fifth, we were unable to incorporate data from clinically relevant biomarkers such as β-amyloid and tau. To determine the impact of hallmark AD biomarkers on FC patterns, future studies will incorporate plasma and cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of amyloid and tau, as well as positron emission tomography data with radioligands that bind to these proteins.

Finally, the field of brain connectomics has great promise for elucidating the complex relationships between cognitive decline and FC patterns. This approach has strong potential to generate clinically relevant biomarkers for brain function in prodromal AD and other neurodegenerative disorders.

Research in context.

  • 1.

    Systematic review: This study examined functional connectivity (FC) differences across the spectrum of early-stage AD, from at-risk and prodromal stages to clinical AD dementia with significant cognitive and functional impairment. Because neurodegeneration typically progresses grad- ually in AD, with subtle transitions between preclinical and prodromal stages rather than discrete drops, we applied a recently developed analytic framework to identify FC network patterns across early stages of AD. Specifically, a data-driven methodology, denominated connICA, was used to uncover inherent and independent FC patterns within the population and their associations with subjective and psychometric cognitive parameters relevant to AD.

  • 2.

    Interpretation: Our finding suggests that disordered brain function, characterized by decreased coherence in major resting-state networks, may be related to self- perceived cognitive decline.

  • 3.

    Future directions: Future studies with significantly larger overall group and subgroup samples are warranted along with optimization of this novel ICA approach. Finally, FC patterns could be used to identify individuals in a preclinical state of AD, helping to enhance therapeutic or preventative trials to identify disease-modifying treatments that can stop or slow clinical progression.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported, in part, by National Institute on Aging grants R01 AG019771, P30 AG10133, and K01 AG049050, and the Alzheimer's Association, Indiana Clinical and Translational Science Institute (UL1 TR001108), and Indiana University-IU Health Strategic Research Initiative and the IU Network Science Institute (IUNI). We thank the staff of the Indiana Memory and Aging Study, the Indiana Alzheimer Disease Center, as well as the imaging staff of the Indiana Institute for Biomedical Imaging Sciences and the IU Center for Neuroimaging. We are very grateful to the study participants and their families who made this research possible.

Footnotes

Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2016.12.004.

Supplementary data

Fig. S1.

Fig. S1

Schematic flow of how brain regions are organized into functional connectivity matrices and into canonical RSNs. (A) Brain regions are parcellated into 278 brain regions using a functional parcellation by Shen et al., 2013. (B) Brain regions are further organized into canonical RSNs according to Yeo et al., 2011. (C) Group average whole-brain functional connectivity (colorbar shown on the right, Pearson correlation coefficient) based on aforementioned gray-matter parcellation. Brain regions were ordered according to a RSN layout (as denoted by different colors on left and top). (D) Layout of resting-state networks within the functional connectivity matrix color coordinated by specific resting-state networks.

Fig. S2.

Fig. S2

Functional connectivity pattern selection criteria. The six robust patterns generated by connICA are illustrated by matrices of the loading values averaged within each block either within the described RSNs (diagonal) or between RSN pairs (“interaction”; off-diagonal). Green asterisk denotes significant blocks (only the upper triangle of these symmetric matrices is denoted). Only three FC patterns shown in the top row had significant blocks and were considered in subsequent analyses. See text for details.

References

  • 1.Delbeuck X., Van der Linden M., Collette F. Alzheimer's disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol Rev. 2003;13:79–92. doi: 10.1023/a:1023832305702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Contreras J.A., Goñi J., Risacher S.L., Sporns O., Saykin A.J. The Structural and Functional Connectome and Prediction of Risk for Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. 2015;2:234–245. doi: 10.1007/s40473-015-0056-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Crossley N.A., Mechelli A., Scott J., Carletti F., Fox P.T., McGuire P. The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain. 2014;137:2382–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dai Z., Yan C., Li K., Wang Z., Wang J., Cao M. Identifying and Mapping Connectivity Patterns of Brain Network Hubs in Alzheimer's Disease. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:3723–3742. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Prescott J.W., Guidon A., Doraiswamy P.M., Roy Choudhury K., Liu C., Petrella J.R. The Alzheimer structural connectome: changes in cortical network topology with increased amyloid plaque burden. Radiology. 2014;273:175–184. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14132593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Thomas J.B., Brier M.R., Bateman R.J., Snyder A.Z., Benzinger T.L., Xiong C. Functional connectivity in autosomal dominant and late-onset Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:1111–1122. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sepulcre J., Johnson K.A., Sperling R. Tau and AB deposits relate to distinctive functional connectivity disruptions in the elderly brain. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10:P159–P160. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.van den Heuvel M.P., Hulshoff Pol H.E. Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20:519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hagmann P., Cammoun L., Gigandet X., Meuli R., Honey C.J., Wedeen V.J. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Damoiseaux J.S., Rombouts S.A., Barkhof F., Scheltens P., Stam C.J., Smith S.M. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Catani M., Thiebaut de Schotten M., Slater D., Dell'Acqua F. Connectomic approaches before the connectome. Neuroimage. 2013;80:2–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sporns O. The human connectome: origins and challenges. Neuroimage. 2013;80:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Smith S.M., Vidaurre D., Beckmann C.F., Glasser M.F., Jenkinson M., Miller K.L. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:666–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yeo B.T., Krienen F.M., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M.R., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cai S., Chong T., Peng Y., Shen W., Li J., von Deneen K.M. Altered functional brain networks in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9539-0. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zhu D., Li K., Terry D.P., Puente A.N., Wang L., Shen D. Connectome-scale assessments of structural and functional connectivity in MCI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:2911–2923. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zhan Y., Ma J., Alexander-Bloch A.F., Xu K., Cui Y., Feng Q. Longitudinal Study of Impaired Intra- and Inter-Network Brain Connectivity in Subjects at High Risk for Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52:913–927. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jack C.R., Jr., Knopman D.S., Jagust W.J., Petersen R.C., Weiner M.W., Aisen P.S. Update on hypothetical model of Alzheimer's disease biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:207. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brier M.R., Thomas J.B., Fagan A.M., Hassenstab J., Holtzman D.M., Benzinger T.L. Functional connectivity and graph theory in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35:757–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jessen F., Amariglio R.E., Van Boxtel M., Breteler M., Ceccaldi M., Chételat G. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10:844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Saykin A.J., Wishart H.A., Rabin L.A., Santulli R.B., Flashman L.A., West J.D. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy similar to that of amnestic MCI. Neurology. 2006;67:834–842. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000234032.77541.a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Koppara A., Wagner M., Lange C., Ernst A., Wiese B., Konig H.H. Cognitive performance before and after the onset of subjective cognitive decline in old age. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2015;1:194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jonker C., Geerlings M.I., Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:983–991. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<983::aid-gps238>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dickerson B.C., Sperling R.A., Hyman B.T., Albert M.S., Blacker D. Clinical prediction of Alzheimer disease dementia across the spectrum of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1443–1450. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chan M., Tay L., Chong M.S. Amnestic mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease in an asian memory clinic - evidence for a clinical spectrum. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2011;1:113–123. doi: 10.1159/000327519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Montine T.J., Phelps C.H., Beach T.G., Bigio E.H., Cairns N.J., Dickson D.W. National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association guidelines for the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer's disease: a practical approach. Acta Neuropathol. 2012;123:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s00401-011-0910-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Amico E., Marinazzo D., DiPerri C., Heine L., Annen J., Martial C. Mapping the functional connectome traits of levels of consciousness. NeuroImage. 2017;148:201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Calhoun V.D., Liu J., Adali T. A review of group ICA for fMRI data and ICA for joint inference of imaging, genetic, and ERP data. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S163–S172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Risacher S.L., Wudunn D., Pepin S.M., MaGee T.R., McDonald B.C., Flashman L.A. Visual contrast sensitivity in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and older adults with cognitive complaints. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:1133–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Delis D.C., Kramer J.H., Kaplan E., Thompkins B.A. Psychological Corporation; 1987. CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version: Manual. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Berg E.A. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol. 1948;39:15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jack C.R., Jr., Bernstein M.A., Fox N.C., Thompson P., Alexander G., Harvey D. The Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27:685–691. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thesen S., Heid O., Mueller E., Schad L.R. Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image-based tracking for real-time fMRI. Magn Reson Med. 2000;44:457–465. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200009)44:3<457::aid-mrm17>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Power J.D., Mitra A., Laumann T.O., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Power J.D., Barnes K.A., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Jenkinson M, Pechaud M, Smith S. BET2: MR-based estimation of brain, skull and scalp surfaces. Eleventh annual meeting of the organization for human brain mapping: Toronto, ON; 2005. p. 167.
  • 37.Shen X., Tokoglu F., Papademetris X., Constable R.T. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. Neuroimage. 2013;82:403–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hyvärinen A. Fast ICA for noisy data using Gaussian moments. Circuits and Systems, 1999 ISCAS'99 Proceedings of the 1999 IEEE International Symposium on: IEEE; 1999. p. 57–61.
  • 39.Calhoun V.D., Adali T., Pearlson G.D., Pekar J.J. A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;14:140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.MathWorks Inc . MathWorks; 2005. MATLAB: the language of technical computing. Desktop tools and development environment, version 7. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Reisberg B., Gauthier S. Current evidence for subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) as the pre-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage of subsequently manifest Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:1–16. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kivipelto M., Solomon A., Ahtiluoto S., Ngandu T., Lehtisalo J., Antikainen R. The Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER): study design and progress. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9:657–665. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Richard E., Andrieu S., Solomon A., Mangialasche F., Ahtiluoto S., Moll van Charante E.P. Methodological challenges in designing dementia prevention trials - the European Dementia Prevention Initiative (EDPI) J Neurol Sci. 2012;322:64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2012.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fujimori M., Imamura T., Yamashita H., Hirono N., Ikejiri Y., Shimomura T. Age at onset and visuocognitive disturbances in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1998;12:163–166. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199809000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Freeman R.Q., Giovannetti T., Lamar M., Cloud B.S., Stern R.A., Kaplan E. Visuoconstructional problems in dementia: contribution of executive systems functions. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:415–426. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Buckner R.L., Wheeler M.E. The cognitive neuroscience og remembering. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:624–634. doi: 10.1038/35090048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wagner A.D., Shannon B.J., Kahn I., Buckner R.L. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fornito A., Zalesky A., Bullmore E. Academic Press; London: 2016. Fundamentals of Brain Network Analysis. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES