Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Jan 27;7:41128. doi: 10.1038/srep41128

Quantifying mating success of territorial males and sneakers in a bower-building cichlid fish

I S Magalhaes 1,2, A M Smith 1, D A Joyce 1,a
PMCID: PMC5269580  PMID: 28128313

Abstract

The strategies and traits males evolve to mate with females are incredible in their diversity. Theory on the evolution of secondary sexual characters suggests that evolving any costly trait or strategy will pay off and stabilise in the population if it is advantageous compared to the alternative less costly strategy, but quantifying the relative success of the two can be difficult. In Lake Malawi, Africa, there are >200 species of cichlid fish in which the males form leks and spend several weeks per year building sand-castle “bowers” several times their size. We tested the idea that a less costly “sneaking” strategy could be successful by quantifying the mating success of bower-holding versus non-bower-holding males. We PIT-tagged every fish in a semi-natural experimental set-up and placed tag-readers on the side of bowers to determine which fish held a bower. We then genotyped the eggs removed from females’ mouths to assign paternity of each egg. Broods were fathered by up to 3 different males. Although paternity was mostly assigned to males that held a bower, a small number of males who did not own a bower were more successful than some of those that did, indicating a role for an alternative strategy in these bower builders.


The investment by males in ornaments and armaments is one reason that organismal phenotypes are so diverse. Female preference for certain characteristics means that sexual selection drives males to develop traits specifically to attract mates, i.e. secondary sexual characters; on which females base their mate choice1. Males that provide no parental care can devote their reproductive efforts to mating resulting in intense competition over mates2. This is particularly visible in species that form leks, where males aggregate and engage in competitive displays to attract females. Male mating success in leks is typically skewed towards a small number of individuals, with high social dominance3, or a more central position4, yet the skew varies according to lek size.

One outcome of strong competition for mates is the development of alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs)5,6,7. An example of this is “sneaking” behaviour, whereby a male will attempt to mate with a female that has chosen another, usually more dominant male, as a partner. Lekking species, as any species with a highly reproductive skew towards a few dominant individuals, might be predicted to exhibit ARTs more often, since this would be advantageous to males with little chance to reproduce otherwise. In some lekking species males build structures that act as non-morphological extensions of their phenotype (i.e. extended phenotypes8). Such structures act as mate choice signals and this in particular may facilitate ARTs because of the substantial cost added by the construction and maintenance of such a structure, which might be prone to take-over by a conspecific male9. Theoretical work on the evolution of secondary sexual characters suggest that evolving a costly strategy, such as having a territory that an individual needs to defend and that will more likely expose him to predators, will pay off and stabilise in the population only if it gives an advantage compared to the alternative less costly strategy2. The prevalence of ART in lekking species, particularly those using extended phenotypes is however understudied10.

Here we used a species of bower-building haplochromine cichlid (Nyassachromis cf. microcephalus) from Lake Malawi to quantify the prevalence and relative success of ARTs in a lekking fish species, and test the hypothesis that males who invest in building and defending a bower have higher reproductive success than those that don’t. Approximately 200 species of bower-building haplochromine cichlid have evolved in the last ~5 million years in Lake Malawi11,12. These species congregate in leks, and build and defend sand-castle structures, termed “bowers” because of their apparent lack of utility beyond male signalling, analogous to the structures of bower birds. They are mouth-brooding cichlids; females spawn and then pick up the eggs in their mouth, while males simultaneously release sperm over the eggs. The fertilized eggs remain in the females’ mouths for about three weeks until they hatch and the fry are released into the water. Mouth-brooding females gain no direct benefits, such as food, protection, or territorial space from their mates. Bowers tend to be species specific, and are used in both direct female choice13,14,15, and male-male signalling16,17,18.

Studies on species of sand-dwelling cichlids of the genera Copadichromis and Otopharynx19 have indicated females mate with up to three males in a single spawning20, and in wild Copadichromis cyclicos broods, between 1–6 males were found to have fathered a single brood21. However, multiple mating is not necessarily indicative of ARTs, particularly in species in which females visit leks and may choose a number of mates. Therefore quantifying the paternity that is contributed by non-territorial males is necessary in order to assess whether sneaking occurs in these fish. Sneaking behaviour has only rarely been observed in bower-building species22,23 so the relative success of this tactic compared with bower building and defence is unknown. Given the apparent reproductive skew observed for this species24, we might expect ARTs to confer an advantage to males who are unable to build and defend territories.

We conducted a large semi-natural experiment in which we tracked fish movement and bower ownership, and employed microsatellite markers for paternity assignment of broods to (i) determine whether sneaking behaviour is prevalent, and (ii) quantify mating success of territorial bower holders when compared to males that do not hold a bower.

Results

Bower ownership

We conducted 5 trials, each lasting between 19 and 26 days (see Table S1 for detailed schedule of each trial and video S1 for an overview of the fish in the experimental pool). Computational problems with the system meant we could not keep the tag readers on for 24 hours a day, but we recorded data for 696 hours across all trials and readers. Over the five trials we monitored a total of 21 bowers (3 to 5 per trial) and obtained 138,547 reads. Bower ownership was assigned on a daily basis using PIT tag reading antenna placed over one of the sides of any bower and counting the number of reads per tag per day. Each bower was assigned to the male with the most reads per day, since bower-holding males typically spend their time swimming just above their bower, maintaining and defending it. We were able to detect that out of 21 bowers, 15 had their ownership consistently assigned to a single male for the whole time the tag-readers were on, 5 bowers were assigned to 2 males and 1 bower was owned by 3 males on separate days in one trial (Table S2). Two males appeared to dominate more than one bower within a trial: in trial two male 24-987B had the most read male tag on 3 different bowers on 4 separate days and in trial 4 male 24-8506 had the most read tag on two different bowers on two separate days. Twenty four males were used in more than one trial and only two of these were dominant in all trials they were part of.

Paternity assignment and relative male mating success

In order to quantify male mating success we genotyped 20% of each brood of eggs (but not less than five eggs if the brood size was larger than five) removed from females’ mouths at the end of each trial. A total of 239 eggs from 35 broods were assigned to 22 fathers across all trials, with an average of 4.4 males fathering offspring per trial (see Table 1, and Tables S4 and S5 for detailed results of the paternity analyses and assignment levels). Based on 20% of offspring being genotyped, females mated with an average of 1.51 males, and a maximum of 3 males (of 15 possible in each trial). Of the 35 broods, 20 had all eggs assigned to a single father, in 12 broods eggs were assigned to two different fathers and in 3 broods to three fathers.

Table 1. Relative mating success.

Trial Male ID SL (cm) Weight (g) CF tag reader number # offspring # females Male MS
1 1B-E516 NA 27.2 NA 1 26 3 0.464
24-82B0 NA 27.35 NA 2 13 3 0.232
24-987B NA 30.61 NA 3 12 3 0.214
24-90DA NA 31.26 NA 4B 5 2 0.089
1B-E918 NA 25.9 NA 4 A 0 0 0.000
2 24-8799 NA 36.05 NA 4 A 25 4 0.658
24-987B NA 30.61 NA 1, 2 A, 4B 8 1 0.211
24-82B0 NA 27.35 NA 3 3 1 0.079
1B-E918 NA 25.9 NA no bower 1 1 0.026
34-BCEC NA 19.5 NA 1 1 1 0.026
24-7FDA NA 19.67 NA 3 0 0 0.000
2A-01A7 NA 21.88 NA 1, 5 0 0 0.000
38-13F6 NA 42.23 NA 2B 0 0 0.000
3 24-8799 10.5 31.75 4.822 1 20 5 0.444
24-7FDA 10.4 31.28 4.878 3 19 4 0.422
24-987B 9.8 24.1 4.428 no bower 3 1 0.067
2A-01A7 10 28.88 5.019 2 2 2 0.044
29-F20E 9.5 27.67 5.54 no bower 1 1 0.022
2A-F8B7 9.9 31.61 5.648 2 0 0 0.000
34-BCEC 9.8 24.56 4.513 2 0 0 0.000
4 1C-04CF 9.8 23.37 4.294 2 35 6 0.833
24-8506 10 25.79 4.482 1, 3 6 2 0.143
24-9889 8.7 20.39 5.204 no bower 1 1 0.024
24-9579 10.2 34.51 5.678 1 0 0 0.000
5 1C-04CF 10.4 31.34 4.888 3 25 5 0.431
1B-F658 10.6 38.09 5.636 3 21 3 0.362
24-8506 11 37.21 4.971 1 10 2 0.172
24-8F52 9.4 25.52 5.261 no bower 1 1 0.017
31-B7F0 10.4 30.5 4.757 no bower 1 1 0.017
1C-A29A 10.6 30.34 4.489 2 0 0 0.000
2B-11FC 10.3 31.19 4.996 2 0 0 0.000

Only results for males who fathered offspring and/or owned bowers are shown (see Supplementary Information for detailed results including all males). Within trials males are ordered by decreasing mating success. From left to right: trial number, male ID, standard length (SL) weight and condition factor (CF) of each male; bower ownership (no bower, or tag reader number of bower owned by each male); number of offspring fathered by the male, number of females male mated with; male mating success (Male MS).

Bower-holding males had significantly higher mating success than males that had no bower assigned to them (unpaired t-test, n1 = 25, n2 = 48, t = 4.044, p (two tailed) < 0.001) (Fig. 1, Table S6). Out of 25 males that held a bower at some point in the experiment, 16 (64%) were assigned paternity of a whole or part of a brood, and on average 96% of offspring were fathered by bower holders. Of the 48 males that did not hold a bower, 6 (12.5%) had offspring assigned to them. All the offspring sired by these 6 males were part of clutches with mixed paternity, and the proportion of offspring they sired in each clutch ranged from 0.07 (1 out of 14 eggs) to 0.19 (3 out of 16 eggs). The fact that the tag-readers were not on for the whole length of each trial meant that some males could have had a lower number of reads because they were holding a bower while the tag-readers were not on. In order to account for this we repeated the t-test including the second most read male as bower holders (Table S2), but the result was identical (unpaired t-test, n1 = 31, n2 = 42, t = 3.829, p (two tailed) < 0.001, Figure S2). Out of the 6 fish that were moved from not holding to holding a bower only one had fathered offspring.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(a) Number of males with and without bowers (red and blue respectively) who fathered offspring (lighter coloured section) and who didn’t (darker coloured section) and (b) Mean individual relative mating success of bower holding males (blue bar) and non- bower holding males (red bar). Black bars represent standard errors (unpaired t-test, n1 = 25, n2 = 48, t = 4.044, p (one tailed) < 0.001).

We also assessed the effect of bower-holding on mating success, relative to the effects of other male traits and found no effect of male weight, size or condition factor on male mating, but found a significant effect of holding a bower (Table 2).

Table 2. Results of the generalized linear mixed-effects models.

  Value Std.Error DF t-value p-value
(a) MS~Bower + Weight
Intercept −0.012 0.075 35 −0.160 0.874
Bower 0.17 0.034 35 5.009 0.000
Weight 0.001 0.002 35 0.281 0.780
(b) MS~Bower + SL + Weight + CF
Intercept 1.624 2.721 33 0.597 0.555
Bower 0.204 0.05 7 4.081 0.005
SL −0.137 0.269 7 −0.511 0.625
Weight 0.018 0.036 7 0.514 0.623
CF −0.158 0.2188 7 −0.726 0.492

Significant results in bold.

Males used in more than one trial had a significantly higher mating success when they held a bower than when they didn’t (paired t-test, n = 10, t = −2.88, p (two tailed) = 0.018).

Discussion

We used a semi-natural experiment to recreate a Nyassachromis cf. microcephalus lek in order to monitor bower ownership, quantify paternity, and assess the role of ARTs in lekking, bower-building cichlids. The building of bowers that act as intra and intersexual signals characterizes a whole radiation of sand dwelling cichlid fish from Lake Malawi12,25, with bower shape being used to define several closely related species26. Wild males invest a considerable amount of time and effort building their bowers, depositing a mouthful of sand on the bower approximately every 15 seconds during the courtship period14. Why these relatively small fish spend so much time and energy building bowers with base diameters more than eight times their own standard length has been in question for decades13,16,22. Most studies, however, have focused solely on the males that have bowers and the role bower size and location play on male mating success13,14,17,22. Even though solitary males are often seen passing by bowers and engaging in fights with bower-holding males or interrupting the mating process16,17,18, no study until now had addressed the advantage of holding a bower versus not holding one and quantified mating success of the two mating strategies. Our genetic assessment of parentage suggests that although bower holding males father the vast majority of the offspring, bower ownership was not a guarantee of mating success, reflecting the reproductive skew observed in the wild24. Such a skew may sometimes allow sneaking to be a more successful strategy than owning a bower.

An experiment such as this would be very difficult or impossible to carry out in the wild, but there are necessarily some limitations to our findings, which use a small number of individuals in a restricted environment where individuals were not monitored 24 hours a day. This last problem is particularly relevant for the interpretation of our results on the success of sneaking males. As the tag-readers were only working part of the time it is almost impossible to be certain that a male sneaker that fathered offspring was not actually holding a bower when he mated. This could lead to an overestimation of the number of sneakers that were assigned paternity. Nonetheless we find this possibility unlikely for almost all of the successful sneakers. Our tag-reader data suggest that males tend to maintain their bowers for long periods of time, not just for a few minutes or hours. Therefore, even if the tag-readers were not able to record the whole period of time a male held a bower for, it is likely that there would be a relatively high number of reads for that male. Of the 6 sneakers that were assigned paternity, only one had such a number of reads (the second highest number of reads for one bower). All others had very few reads and were not recorded over a bower for more than a few seconds in a row. It is therefore possible that one of the males we considered a sneaker might have held a bower, but we find it unlikely for the five other males.

Since it is very difficult to count non-bower holding males in the wild, their prevalence is not known and so the ratio of bower holding: non-bower holding males we created could be either too high or too low. This may lead to comparatively more or fewer sneaking opportunities than would be expected in the wild. Nevertheless, sneaking seems to be a viable strategy. In wild populations, bowers tend to be built in densely populated leks and central bowers are thought to have an advantage due to more female encounters17. Males with bowers in the periphery of the lek have more fights with heterospecific fish18 and may be more exposed to predators. It is possible that in the wild it may be more advantageous to be a sneaker than to build and maintain a bower in a location where attacks are frequent and female encounters low. This would explain the maintenance of a sneaking strategy in wild populations, despite a relatively low paternity likelihood.

Bowers are used by males to compete for the optimal positions on large leks preferred by females16,24 and are likely to be an honest indicator of competitive ability. Bowers used in indirect female choice27 may be a signal that has been co-opted for use in direct female choice, similar to bowerbird skrraa calls used in aggression and courtship28. Several bower traits, such as height are known to be used by females choosing among males who hold a bower13,14,29. Choosing males with bowers might be the first step of a sequential assessment of several bower and male traits used by females to evaluate the quality of their potential mates15,30. There are indications that bower construction in cichlids is an honest indicator of male quality31,32. When females can accurately assess male quality according to some signal, high quality males will be chosen, and there will be a greater incentive for lower quality males to switch to an alternative tactic33. Additionally, assessing male quality does not preclude the choice of sneaking males. However it is possible that a sneaker’s reproductive success derives from interrupting a courting territorial male and attempting to fertilize the female rather than being chosen by the female during courtship. Multiple paternity (with territorial or non-territorial males) may however allow a bet-hedging strategy by females, assuming there is some heritability in reproductive tactic34. The fact that males did not appear to consistently use one tactic or another between trials in the experiment indicates some plasticity, and the heritability of bower ownership or sneaking needs to be properly estimated before ARTs can be considered an evolutionary strategy upon which selection can act.

An unexpected result of our experiment was that, even though the majority of the bowers were assigned to one owner during a trial, some bowers changed ownership. Observational studies of bower-building cichlid species from Lake Tanganyika found that males that were removed from or deserted their bowers were quickly replaced by other males on the bowers30,35, with desertion of a bower being attributed to exhaustion resulting from bower maintenance and reproduction30. For Lake Malawi bower-building cichlids it is known that wild males temporarily abandon their bowers, usually during the afternoons, to feed nearby14,22, but replacement of one male by another as a bower owner had never been reported and past field experiments on this species have been carried out under the assumption that a bower was owned by the same male throughout the whole season. Our results indicate that, either due to desertion or competition, some bowers changed ownership during a trial. This finding, together with the fact that the same male did not always hold a bower across trials also suggest that building and/or holding a bower has an element of plasticity. Size and weight did not seem to reflect bower ownership, and male quality was something other than “condition” as we measured it. Fixed genetic differences in ARTs have been reported in several organisms36,37 (reviewed by ref. 38), and some cichlid species have ARTs that are fixed for life, whereas other studies have found that ARTs change throughout the life of a male with male size being important when acquiring and maintaining a territory and bigger males having the advantage39,40.

To conclude, our results show a large skew of paternity towards males who hold a bower, indicating that the costs of building and maintaining it pay off when compared to the alternative strategy of sneaking. However, paternity is most likely not constrained to bower holders, so a potential role for sneaky mating exists, as might be expected in a lekking system with high reproductive skew towards a few individuals. The use of an extended phenotype in sexual signaling may facilitate ARTs, and the independence of the signal from the body could select for plasticity in tactics because of the possibility of bower acquisition over a mating season.

Methods

Individuals used in experiments

Males and females used in this experiment were Nyassachromis cf. microcephalus laboratory bred F1s. They derived from a population of fish collected at Thumbi West Island in the Lake Malawi National Park (14°01′22 S, 34°49′24E) in 2007, and maintained at the University of Hull, UK. Females of this species are grey but breeding males are colourful with metallic blue heads and flanks, dark vertical stripes and yellow/blue on the mid body and black fins. The wild caught individuals occupied a shallow water lek at the shore of the Island.

Males and females were kept separately in stock tanks before the start of the experiment and were returned to stock tanks between trials. Each of these tanks was 180 × 40 × 45 cm, holding approximately 285 litres of water, and part of a 5,500 litre recirculation system through which 720 litres passes per hour, and that receives a 15% daily water change. Water temperature was maintained between 24–26 °C. Aquaria were illuminated on a 12D: 12 L regime using full-spectrum fluorescent tubes including UV light. Fish were fed once a day with dry food.

We used a total of 36 males and 84 experimental females over the 5 trials. Out of the 36 males, 12 were used only in one trial, 11 were used in two trials and 13 in three trials. Out of the 84 females, 58 were used in one trial, 15 in two trials, 10 in three trials and 1 in four trials. Fish used in the experiment were individually PIT (Passive Integrated Transponder) tagged, fin clipped and weighed by A.M.S. (For all trials, only mature, colourful and potentially reproductively active males were used. For males in the last three trials we also took their length (standard length, SL) and estimated their condition factor. Condition factor for each male was calculated as 100*weight (g)/length (cm)2.7626. In some cichlid species size and weight are characteristics used by females in male choice41 and this could confound the effect of holding a bower, so we used these measurements to control the effects of weight, size and condition factor on male success. The experiment was carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations established by the UK Home Office and was part of a UK Home Office licensed project (60/3295).

Experimental setup and design

The experiment was conducted in 5 trials between July 2009 and April 2010. It was carried out in one circular above ground pool with 3.6 m diameter and 1.2 m height in a greenhouse at the University of Hull’s Botanical Gardens (video S1). There was a 20 cm depth of fine sand (3 tonnes) and the pool was equipped with a pump, biofilter, UV steriliser and heater. Water temperature was maintained between 23 and 25 °C. Tagged fish were allowed to swim freely and there was full interaction between males and females. Food was distributed three times per week. On the first day of each trial 15 males were introduced into the pool with 8–12 non-experimental females to stimulate males to establish territories and start building bowers. After 7–10 days, fish were removed from the pool, males immediately returned and 25 experimental females added. During the last week of each trial, a circular PIT tag reading antenna was placed over any bower that had been built (see Table S1 for detailed schedule of each trial and video S1 for an overview of the fish in the pool). These tag readers detect and record any PIT tags within 40 cm, approximately every second.

On the last day of each trial (19–26 days after the start) all fish were removed, and brooding females were weighed, standard length measured, and their eggs were removed from their mouth and preserved in 95% ethanol.

Bower ownership assignment

We assigned bower ownership by assuming that the male with the most reads on a particular bower owned that bower, since males typically spend their time swimming just above their bower, maintaining and defending. We collected data for the longest periods possible, but computational problems with the system meant we could not keep the tag readers on for 24 hours a day (see Table S2 for details). Bower ownership was assigned on a daily basis by dividing the number of reads for each male by the total number of reads, and by the total number of male reads.

DNA extraction and genotyping methods

We obtained a total of 45 broods from all the trials. However, as the tag readers were only working for part of the time the fish were in the tank, we discarded 10 broods that we thought might have been fathered before bower ownership was assigned. Nyassachromis eggs hatch after 5 days and the egg yolk is completely absorbed after 21 days. Extensive breeding and rearing of known broods means we can relatively accurately estimate the age of the embryo by the size of the fish compared to the yolk.

For each trial the mother, all 15 potential fathers and 20% of each clutch (but not less than five eggs if the brood size was larger than five) were genotyped at four microsatellite loci using fluorescently labelled primers: Ppun5; Ppun7; Ppun2127 and TmoM528. Clutch size ranged from 1 to 75 eggs. The number of eggs genotyped varied from 1 to 19 eggs per clutch. Only three broods had less than five eggs genotyped. With a minimum of five eggs per clutch, the probability that an allele at a given locus was missed is 0.55 per parent29. DNA was extracted using the HotSHOT method30 and microsatellite loci were amplified using the QIAGEN multiplex PCR kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Fragments were separated using a Beckman Coulter CEQ 8000 capillary sequencer and sized using the CEQ 8000 Series Genetic Analysis System (see Table S3 for summary statistics of the microsatellite data).

Paternity assignment

As broods were removed from the mother’s mouth, maternity was unambiguous. Nonetheless, we checked for possible scoring errors by checking for mismatches in microsatellite alleles between mother and eggs. Paternity was assigned using Cervus31, which allows for inclusion of an input file with the identity of the mother. Paternity was checked by two complementary methods: (1) exclusion from paternity of males that had any mismatches in microsatellite alleles with an egg and (2) relaxed (80%) and strict (95%) confidence level. Possible scoring errors were considered by allowing a 0.01 error rate per locus.

Relative male mating success

The mating success of each individual male relative to all other males in each trial (“Mating Success”) was calculated from the results of the paternity assignment using the equation 129.

graphic file with name srep41128-m1.jpg

where Si is the number of offspring sired by an individual male per trial (i); Si_total is the total number of offspring sired by all males per trial (i). Mating success potentially ranges from 0 (a male did not sire offspring) to 1 (if a male was the only one that sired offspring in each trial in which it was used). The mating success for a male was calculated for each day and trial separately because a male’s dominance condition (i.e. bower-holding vs. not bower-holding) might change across days.

To test the prediction that bower-holding males have higher mating success than non-bower holding males, we compared the mating success of males holding bowers and males not holding bowers using unpaired two-tailed t-tests allowing for unequal variance. In order to account for the fact that the tag-readers were not switched on 24 hours a day we repeated this analysis by also including the second most read male as a bower holder. This lead to 6 fish (38-13F6, 2A-01A7, 34-BCEC, 24-9579, 2B-11FC, 1C-04CF, Table S2) being moved from the “no-bower” to the “bower” category.

To assess the effect of bower-holding on mating success, relative to the effects of other male traits, we used linear mixed effects models. Explanatory variables in the models were bower holding (yes or no) and male weight for the analyses on the complete dataset, and bower, male weight, standard length and condition factor for the data set composed of the last three trials. To account for repeated use of some individuals across trials we included individual ID as a random effect. All analyses were done in R (www.r-project.org).

To assess the effect of holding a bower on the mating success of the same male we used a paired two-tailed t-test comparing the mating success between a trial when they held a bower and a trial when they didn’t hold a bower.

Ethical considerations

All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations established by the UK Home Office. All experimental protocols were approved by the UK Home Office (project licence no. 60/3295).

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Magalhaes, I. S. et al. Quantifying mating success of territorial males and sneakers in a bower-building cichlid fish. Sci. Rep. 7, 41128; doi: 10.1038/srep41128 (2017).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material
srep41128-s1.doc (800KB, doc)
Supplementary Video S1
Download video file (3.3MB, avi)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Paul Nichols, Boye Gricar, Michael Orchard and Victor Swetez for help with building the experimental set up, members of the HIFI group for assistance with running the experiment, Topi Lehtonen and Ben Chapman for comments on earlier versions of the manuscript and the members of the Evolutionary and Environmental Genomics Group at the University of Hull (EvoHull) for helpful feedback and discussions. This research was supported by the Leverhulme Trust (ECF/40195) to DAJ and the Swiss National Science Foundation (PBBEP3-123641) to ISM.

Footnotes

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Author Contributions I.S.M., D.A.J. and A.M.S. carried out the experiment. I.S.M. and D.A.J. conceived and designed the study, did the statistical data analysis and wrote the manuscript.

References

  1. Andersson M. & Iwasa Y. Sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 53–58 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersson M. Sexual Selection: (Monographs in Behavior and Ecology). (Princeton University Press, 1994). [Google Scholar]
  3. Beehler B. M. & Foster M. S. Hotshots, Hotspots, and Female Preference in the Organization of Lek Mating Systems. Am. Nat. 131, 203–219 (1988). [Google Scholar]
  4. Bradbury J. W. & Gibson R. M. In Mate Choice (ed. Bateson P.) 109–138 (Cambridge University Press). [Google Scholar]
  5. Taborsky M. I., Oliviera R. F. & Brockmann H. J. In Alternative Reproductive Tactics: An Integrative Approach 1–21 (Cambridge University Press, 2008). [Google Scholar]
  6. Dominey W. J. Alternative Mating Tactics and Evolutionarily Stable Strategies. Am. Zool. 24, 385–396 (1984). [Google Scholar]
  7. Jirotkul M. Operational sex ratio influences female preference and male–male competition in guppies. Anim. Behav. 58, 287–294 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dawkins R. Replicator selection and the extended phenotype. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie- Journal of Comparative Ethology 47, 61–76 (1978). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Schaedelin F. C. & Taborsky M. Extended phenotypes as signals. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 84, 293–313 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Reynolds S. M. et al. Behavioral paternity predicts genetic paternity in Satin Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus), a species with a non-resource-based mating system. Auk 124, 857–867 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  11. Genner M. J. et al. Age of cichlids: new dates for ancient lake fish radiations. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24, 1269–1282 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Konings A. Malawi Cichlids in their Natural Habitat, New 4th Edition (Hardcover). (Cichlid Press, 2007). [Google Scholar]
  13. Stauffer J. R. Jr., Kellogg K. A. & McKaye K. R. Experimental Evidence of Female Choice in Lake Malawi Cichlids. Copeia 2005, 657–660 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  14. Mckaye K. R., Louda S. M. & Stauffer J. R. Bower size and male reproductive success in a cichlid fish lek. Am. Nat. 135, 597–613 (1990). [Google Scholar]
  15. Young K. A., Genner M. J., Haesler M. P. & Joyce D. A. Sequential female assessment drives complex sexual selection on bower shape in a cichlid fish. Evolution 64, 2246–2253 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Martin C. H. & Genner M. J. A role for male bower size as an intrasexual signal in a Lake Malawi cichlid fish. Behaviour 146, 963–978 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  17. Genner M. J., Young K. A., Haesler M. P. & Joyce D. A. Indirect mate choice, direct mate choice and species recognition in a bower-building cichlid fish lek. J. Evol. Biol. 21, 1387–1396 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Magalhaes I. S., Croft G. E. & Joyce D. A. Altering an extended phenotype reduces intraspecific male aggression and can maintain diversity in cichlid fish. PeerJ 1, e209 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eccles D. H. & Trewavas E. Malaŵian cichlid fishes: The classification of some haplochromine genera. (Lake Fish Movies, 1989). [Google Scholar]
  20. McKaye K. R. In Cichlid fishes: behaviour, ecology, and evolution (ed. Keenleyside M. H.) 241–257 (Chapman & Hall New York, NY, USA, 1991). [Google Scholar]
  21. Kellogg K. A., Markert J. A., Stauffer J. R. & Kocher T. D. Microsatellite variation demonstrates multiple paternity in lekking cichlid fishes from Lake Malawi, Africa. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 260, 79–84 (1995). [Google Scholar]
  22. McKaye K. R. Ecology and breeding behavior of a cichlid fish, Cyrtocara eucinostomus, on a large lek in Lake Malawi, Africa. Environ. Biol. Fishes 8, 81–96 (1983). [Google Scholar]
  23. Oliveira R. F. & Almada V. C. Mating tactics and male-male courtship in the lek-breeding cichlid Oreochromis mossambicus. J. Fish Biol. 52, 1115–1129 (1998). [Google Scholar]
  24. Young K. A., Genner M. J., Joyce D. A. & Haesler M. P. Hotshots, hot spots, and female preference: exploring lek formation models with a bower-building cichlid fish. Behav. Ecol. 20, 609–615 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  25. Kidd M. R., Kidd C. E. & Kocher T. D. Axes of differentiation in the bower-building cichlids of Lake Malawi. Mol. Ecol. 15, 459–478 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Stauffer J. R. & Konings A. F. Review of Copadichromis (Teleostei: Cichlidae) with the description of a new genus and six new species. Ichthyol. Explor. Freshw. 17, 9 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  27. Wiley R. H. & Poston J. Indirect mate choice, competition for mates, and coevolution of the sexes. Evolution 50, 1371–1381 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Borgia G. & Coleman S. W. Co-option of male courtship signals from aggressive display in bowerbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 267, 1735–1740 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Schaedelin F. C. & Taborsky M. Female choice of a non-bodily ornament: an experimental study of cichlid sand craters in Cyathopharynx furcifer. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1437–1447 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  30. Karino K. Tactic for bower acquisition by male cichlids Cyathopharynx furcifer, in Lake Tanganyika. Ichthyological Research 43, 125–132. (1996). [Google Scholar]
  31. Taylor M., Turner G. F., Robinson R. L. & Stauffer J. R. Sexual selection, parasites and bower height skew in a bower-building cichlid fish. Anim. Behav. 56, 379–384 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Schaedelin F. & Taborsky M. Mating craters of Cyathopharynx furcifer (Cichlidae) are individually specific, extended phenotypes. Anim. Behav. 72, 753–761 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  33. Luttbeg B. Female mate assessment and choice behavior affect the frequency of alternative male mating tactics. Behav. Ecol. 15, 239–247 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  34. Reichard M., Le Comber S. C. & Smith C. Sneaking from a female perspective. Anim. Behav. 74, 679–688 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  35. Morita M. et al. Bower-building behaviour is associated with increased sperm longevity in Tanganyikan cichlids. J. Evol. Biol. 27, 2629–2643 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dominey W. J. Female mimicry in male bluegill sunfish—a genetic polymorphism? Nature 284, 546–548 (1980). [Google Scholar]
  37. Gross M. R. Sunfish, salmon, and the evolution of alternative reproductive strategies and tactics in fishes. In Fish reproduction: strategies and tactics (ed. Gross M. R.) 55–75 (1984). [Google Scholar]
  38. Neff B. D. & Svensson E. I. Polyandry and alternative mating tactics. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20120045 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Taborsky M. Sneakers, Satellites, and Helpers: Parasitic and cooperative behavior in fish reproduction. Advances in the Study of Behavior 23, 100 (1994). [Google Scholar]
  40. Sato T., Hirose M., Taborsky M. & Kimura S. Size-Dependent Male Alternative Reproductive Tactics in the Shell-Brooding Cichlid Fish Lamprologus callipterus in Lake Tanganyika. Ethology 110, 49–62 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  41. Nelson M. C. Male size, spawning pit size and female mate choice in a lekking cichlid fish. Anim. Behav. 50, 1587–1599 (1995). [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material
srep41128-s1.doc (800KB, doc)
Supplementary Video S1
Download video file (3.3MB, avi)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES