Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016 Apr 15;22(9):944–949. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000278

Decision-Making Does Not Moderate the Association between Cannabis Use and Body Mass Index among Adolescent Cannabis Users

J Megan Ross 1, Paulo Graziano 1, Ileana Pacheco-Colón 1, Stefany Coxe 1, Raul Gonzalez 1
PMCID: PMC5280071  NIHMSID: NIHMS844811  PMID: 27079834

Abstract

Objective

Results from research conducted on the association between cannabis use and body mass index (BMI) reveal mixed findings. It is possible that individual differences in decision-making (DM) abilities may influence these associations.

Methods

This study analyzed how amount of cannabis use, DM performance and the interaction of these variables influenced BMI and clinical classifications of weight among adolescents (ages 14 to 18; 56% male; 77% Hispanic). The sample consisted primarily of cannabis users (n = 238) without a history of significant developmental disorders, birth complications, neurological conditions, or history of mood, thought, or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder at screening. Further, few participants engaged frequently in other drug use (except for alcohol and nicotine).

Results

Analyses revealed that more lifetime cannabis use was associated with a higher BMI and greater likelihood of being overweight/obese. Interactions between DM and cannabis use on BMI were not significant and DM was not directly associated with BMI.

Discussion

Our findings suggest that among adolescents, cannabis use is associated with a greater BMI regardless of DM abilities and this association is not accounted for by other potential factors, including depression, alcohol use, nicotine use, race, ethnicity, or IQ.

Keywords: Drug use, risk-taking, addiction, adolescence, obesity and executive abilities

Introduction

The importance of studying the consequences of cannabis use among adolescents continues to grow given the trends to legalize use and accompanying reductions in perceived risk (Miech et al., 2015). In the United States, states that have decriminalized cannabis use have higher rates of use and lower perceived risk of use among adolescents (Miech et al., 2015). Overall, annual prevalence of use is high, with approximately 40% of high school seniors reporting use in the past year (Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg, & Miech, 2014).

A well-established side effect of cannabis (Green, Kavanagh, & Young, 2003) – appetite stimulation – has received attention as a potential therapeutic use, but its potential adverse impact on the weight of otherwise healthy individuals has been studied less. Another potential mechanism for cannabis to influence weight is decreased motivation, which has been observed in chronic cannabis users (Kouri, Pope, Yurgelun-Todd, & Gruber, 1995). Decreased motivation could potentially lead to increases in sedentary behavior. Given this, it is surprising that findings from prior studies examining associations between cannabis use and weight have yielded mixed results. Studies among adults have consistently shown that cannabis users have a lower prevalence of obesity (Le Strat & Le Foll, 2011), but among adolescents, no associations between these variables is more often the case (Lanza, Grella, & Chung, 2014; Pasch, Velazquez, Cance, Moe, & Lytle, 2012). One exception is a longitudinal study reporting that increased cannabis use during adolescence increased the likelihood of obesity in young adulthood (Huang, Lanza, & Anglin, 2013). Given disparate findings, it is likely that previously unexamined variables may be moderating the association between cannabis use and weight. In this study, we examine how cannabis use among adolescents is associated with body mass index (BMI) and the potential moderating role of a relevant neurocognitive factor: decision-making (DM).

Drug addiction shares underlying neural mechanisms with excessive eating, including increases in dopamine concentrations in reward-related brain regions and decreases in dopamine receptors in brain regions involved with DM, including the orbitofrontal cortex and cingulate gyrus (Volkow, Fowler, Wang, & Telang, 2008). Behaviorally, both involve the choice of short-term reward despite long-term negative consequences. DM deficits have been reported in adolescents with cannabis use disorders (Dougherty et al., 2013) and who are obese (Verdejo-García et al., 2010). It is possible that individual differences in DM may influence the association between cannabis use and weight status.

The current study examined the influence of DM on the association between cannabis use and weight status among a sample of adolescents that varied in their exposure to cannabis. This study is unique in its examination of DM as a possible moderator of cannabis-associated weight changes, as well as in its examination of the association between cannabis use and BMI in a sample of adolescent cannabis users. We hypothesized that more cannabis use would predict a higher BMI as well as a greater likelihood of being overweight/obese; however, we anticipated that this association would be strongest when DM performance was poor. Moreover, we explore whether the influence of DM may vary across DM tasks that differ in how explicitly they present risk to participants.

Methods

Participants

Participants are 238 adolescents ages 14 to 17, recruited primarily through Miami-Dade County middle and high schools, flyers distributed throughout the community, and word-of-mouth.

Eligibility for participation was ascertained via phone screen. Inclusion criteria were developed to obtain a sample consisting predominantly of youth at risk for escalation in cannabis use. The majority of the sample (95%) reported some use of either alcohol, cigarettes or other drugs (even if only minimal) during screening, were between the ages of 14 to 16 at baseline, and were able to read and write in English. Exclusion criteria at screening included self-reported developmental disorders, birth complications, neurological conditions, or history of mood, thought, or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Lastly, participants were excluded at screening for frequent or recent use of drugs other than alcohol, nicotine, or cannabis, or for evidence suggestive of an alcohol or cannabis use disorder. Participants underwent in-depth assessment of their mental health, substance use, and medical history during their baseline visit. All participants underwent oral fluid toxicology screening to test for recent drug use, using the Intercept oral fluid drug test (OraSure Technologies, Inc.: Bethlehem, PA).

Most participants were Hispanic/Latino and reported cannabis as the most commonly used drug. At screening for the parent project, participants were excluded if they reported a mental health disorder diagnosis or frequent/recent drug use (i.e., used drugs other than cannabis more than 10 times, consumed more than 13 drinks per week on three or more occasions in their lifetime, had more than three occurrences of consuming 6 drinks per day for women and 7 drinks per day for men, or had evidence suggestive of dependence for alcohol or cannabis). However, substance use and mental health status of some participants did change over the course of the parent project and participants were not excluded for emergence of these disorders over the course of the parent project. Substance use characteristics of the current study sample are presented in Table 1. Few met criteria for a current mental health disorder, and only 4 participants reported using drugs other than cannabis more than 20 times in their lifetime. Oral fluid testing for recent drug use revealed eleven participants had recently used cannabis, three participants had recently used cocaine, and one participant had recently used amphetamines.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics

n = 238
Age 15.62 (0.74)
Years of Education 9.45 (0.93)
Years of Education (Mother) 14.22 (2.58)
WRAT-4 Reading Standard Score 108.94 (15.42)
Ethnicity/race (%)
 Hispanic/Latino 76.5
 Caucasian 4.2
 African-American 4.2
 More than one race 14.7
 Other 0.4
Male (%) 56
Body Mass Index (z-score) 0.64 (1.06)
Amount of Lifetime Nicotine (cigarettes; MD, IQR) 0 (0, 6)
Amount of Lifetime Alcohol (1 serving; MD, IQR) 13.00 (0, 59.25)
Amount of Lifetime Cannabis (g; MD, IQR) 22.80 (0.70, 158.45)
Ever Used Cannabis (%) 84.0
Used Cannabis in Past Six Months (%) 76.9
Ever Used Alcohol (%) 87.8
Ever Used Nicotine (%) 46.6
Ever Used Other Drugs (%) 21.8
Iowa Gambling Task (total net) 1.25 (24.84)
Cups Task (risky choices) 34.36 (10.22)
Game of Dice Task (risky choices) 7.11 (5.13)
Current Panic Disorder (%) 0.80
Current Generalized Anxiety Disorder (%) 3.40
Current Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (%) 3.80
Current Major Depression Diagnosis (%) 1.70
Current ADHD Diagnosis (%) 2.10
Current ODD Diagnosis (%) 3.40
Current Conduct Disorder Diagnosis (%) 8.80
Current Cannabis Abuse Diagnosis (%) 13.4
Current Cannabis Dependence Diagnosis (%) 3.8
Current Alcohol Abuse Diagnosis (%) 2.5
Current Alcohol Dependence Diagnosis (%) 1.3
Current Other Drug Abuse Diagnosis (%) 0.8
Current Other Drug Dependence Diagnosis (%) 0.8

Note: All values are means and standard deviations unless otherwise specified. WRAT = Word Reading Achievement Test, MD = median, IQR = interquartile range, ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, ODD = oppositional defiant disorder.

Procedures

All study procedures and protocols were approved by the IRB at FIU and this research was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. Participant assent and parental consent were obtained for all participants. The participants in this study are part of a larger longitudinal study designed to assess how DM influences cannabis use trajectories and how neurocognition is affected by cannabis use. The study involves five measurement waves conducted every six months over two years. BMI data collection commenced about one year after study onset; therefore, BMI data for the current sample was collected for the first time during the baseline visit for 134 participants and during the third measurement wave for 104 participants. All other data used in analyses was collected at the same measurement wave as a participant’s BMI data.

Measures

Substance Use

The Drug Use History Questionnaire (DUHQ) assesses lifetime frequency and quantity of 15 different drug classes (Rippeth et al., 2004; Gonzalez et al., 2012). Amount of cannabis use was calculated (in grams) using self-report of frequency and quantity of use across participants’ lifetime.

Decision-Making

DM was measured via three computerized tasks: the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), the Cups Task (CT), and the Game of Dice Task (GDT), all of which assess DM but vary on how explicitly risk is presented to participants.

The IGT assesses DM under conditions of ambiguous risk (Bechara, 2007; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994). Participants are shown a computer display of four decks of cards and are told the goal in the game is to win as much money as possible. Participants are instructed that every time they choose a card they win some money, but sometimes also lose money and that some decks are worse than others. More choices from “good decks” yield a positive total score while more choices from “bad decks” yield a negative total by the end of the game. The IGT net score – the choices from “good decks” minus choices from “bad decks” – was used as the measure of DM for this task.

The GDT assesses DM when the participant is given specific rules and probabilities for gains and losses throughout the task (Brand et al., 2005). For each of 18 trials, participants predict the outcome of a dice roll by choosing options that vary in the probability of winning (e.g., one possible number vs. multiple numbers). Choices with higher probability of winning are accompanied by lesser rewards compared to those with lower probability of winning. The two lowest probability choices with the highest reward are considered risky choices. The total number of risky choices was the measure of DM used in our analyses.

The CT is different from the previously mentioned DM tasks in that it was specifically designed to assess DM in children and adolescents (Levin, Hart, Weller, & Harshman, 2007). Similar to the GDT, the CT measures DM under conditions of specific risk. The CT gives the participant a visual display of 2, 3 or 5 cups on both the left and right side of the screen. The participants are told to choose a cup from either side; choices from one side always yield a definite reward, whereas choices from the opposite side provide a chance for a greater reward or loss. There are 54 trials of either a “gain” or “loss.” Gain trials have two options: 1) definite gain of one quarter or 2) the chance to win multiple quarters or no quarters. The loss trials also have two options: 1) definite loss of one quarter or 2) a chance to lose multiple quarters or no quarters. The total number of risky choices (i.e., choosing side with chances to lose multiple quarters or chance to win multiple quarters) made during all of the trials was used as the measure of DM for this task.

Body Mass Index

Following standard anthropometric procedures, trained personnel measured adolescents’ height and weight (removing shoes and heavy outer clothing) in triplicate (using the median value) using a wall mounted stadiometer and a balance beam scale. BMI z-scores were calculated based on age/gender norms from the Centers for Disease Control (Kuczmarski et al., 2002). Percentiles based on age and gender were used to categorize participants into two clinically relevant categories: 1) overweight or obese (85th percentile and above) and 2) underweight or normal weight (Less than 85th percentile). The BMI z-score and clinical classification variables were used as the outcome variables for this study.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted in SPSS 22.0 using linear or logistic regression. In order to identify potential covariates, correlations (either Spearman’s rho, Pearson, or point-biserial) were calculated for BMI z-score and several variables thought to potentially influence BMI, including nicotine and alcohol use, diagnosis of major depressive episode, race/ethnicity, education, and estimated IQ. None were significantly correlated with BMI z-score (p-values > .07), and were not included as covariates. As previously noted, BMI data for the current study were collected at baseline for some participants or during the third measurement wave for others. Participants whose data came from their third measurement wave were exposed to the neurocognitive measures twice (during baseline and their visits). To control for any influence this may have had on our results, a dummy-coded variable was created to indicate whether a participant had completed the DM tasks once or twice and was included in all analyses as a covariate. All independent variables were mean-centered. Assumptions for multiple regression were evaluated (i.e., linearity, normality of residuals, constant variance of residuals, independence of residuals; Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2013). Although Q-Q plots suggested some evidence for minor heteroscedasticity, examination of the overall residuals distribution from each regression model showed approximately a normal distributions. Data were examined for outliers and no outliers were present. Bootstrapping was used to make more robust standard error estimates. Linear regressions were conducted with BMI z-score as the dependent variable and logistic regressions were conducted with the clinical classification of weight as the outcome variable. Three linear regressions and logistic regressions were conducted (one for each DM task). Independent variables were amount of lifetime cannabis use, DM task performance, and their interaction.

Results

IGT performance was not correlated with either the performance on the CT (r = −0.10) or the GDT (r = 0.01), however performance on the CT and GDT were significantly correlated (r = 0.23).

Across all three DM tasks, no significant interaction effects were observed between DM performance and cannabis use on either BMI z-score or clinical classification (see Table 2 for all statistical results).

Table 2.

Results of cannabis use, decision-making tasks, and the interaction of cannabis use and decision-making tasks predicting BMI z-score and clinical classifications of weight (n = 238)

BMI z-score Standardized Regression Coefficient Standard Error t-score p-value
CU 0.187 0.0002 2.580 0.01
IGT 0.008 0.003 0.128 0.90
IGT x CU 0.037 0.000004 0.513 0.61

CU 0.160 0.0001 2.394 0.02
CT −0.061 0.007 −0.905 0.37
CT x CU 0.036 0.00002 0.525 0.60

CU 0.146 0.0002 1.856 0.07
GDT −0.022 0.013 −0.337 0.74
GDT x CU −0.041 0.00003 −0.519 0.60

 Clinical Classification Odds Ratio Standard Error Wald p-value

CU 1.001 0.0004 5.546 0.02
IGT 0.998 0.006 0.103 0.75
IGT x CU 1.000 0.00001 0.008 0.93

CU 1.001 0.0004 4.876 0.03
CT 0.988 0.014 0.734 0.39
CT x CU 1.000 0.00005 0.459 0.50

CU 1.001 0.0004 3.600 0.06
GDT 0.968 0.027 1.362 0.24
GDT x CU 1.000 0.00008 1.346 0.25

Note: CU = cannabis use, IGT = Iowa Gambling Task, CT = Cups Task, GDT = Game of Dice Task.

Similarly, no significant main effects were observed for DM performance predicting BMI or clinical classification. In contrast, cannabis use was significantly associated with BMI z-score across most models, regardless of which DM task was included (β = 0.19, β = 0.16, β = 0.15, respectively when the IGT, CT and GDT were included in the analysis); however, the model including the GDT was just shy of statistical significance (p = .07). Semipartial correlation coefficients across these models ranged from 0.12 (for the model including GDT) to 0.17 (for the model including the IGT).

The same pattern emerged for clinical classification, with more cannabis use associated with a higher probability of being overweight/obese (odds ratio: 1.001, when all DM tasks were included in the analysis). A post-hoc power analysis using G-Power 3.1 (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) indicated that our analyses were sufficiently powered to detect an interaction effect for a small effect size (d =.06).

Discussion

The current study examined how amount of cannabis use was associated with BMI and weight status among adolescents, as well as whether differences in DM performance influenced this association. Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that more cannabis use was associated with a higher BMI and greater likelihood of being overweight/obese. This association could not be accounted for by the influence of alcohol use, nicotine use, parent education, depression, gender, race or ethnicity, as none of these were found to be associated with BMI in our sample. Contrary to expectations, DM performance had no significant impact on this association and did not account for unique variance in BMI or influence the probability of being classified as overweight/obese.

These findings, although consistent with known effects of cannabis on appetite and potential effects on motivation, differ from other cross-sectional studies examining associations between cannabis use and weight among adolescents (Lanza et al., 2014; Pasch et al., 2012). These differences could potentially be explained by the relatively larger sample size (and increased power) of our study. Another potential explanation is that, compared to the aforementioned studies, our sample contained a greater number of adolescents who had tried cannabis at least once (84%) and used in the past year (76%) compared, for example, with Lanza and colleagues (2014) study, of which 32% of their sample reported using cannabis in the past year. One longitudinal study reported an association between cannabis use (increasing and sporadic patterns of use) during adolescence with a greater likelihood of stable and increasing obesity patterns from adolescence to young adulthood. Such study reported odds ratios of 1.6 (increasing cannabis use and increasing obesity), 0.2 (sporadic cannabis use and stable obesity) and 0.1 (sporadic cannabis use and increasing obesity; Huang et al., 2013). The somewhat smaller effects we observed may be due to the cross-sectional design of our study, however it is difficult to equate cross-sectional and longitudinal effects because they measure different things (i.e., relationships at a single time point versus the relationship between one variable and change in the other variable).

The most surprising finding from our study is that DM abilities were not associated with BMI or weight status, nor did they influence the relationship between cannabis use and BMI, despite our use of three different measures of DM. We did find that the IGT was not associated with either the CT or GDT. Decision-making tasks that vary in whether risk is clearly specified or ambiguous may rely on different brain systems as demonstrated in fMRI studies conducted with the CT (Weller, Levin, Shiv & Bechara, 2007), GDT (Brand, Recknor, Grabenhorst, & Bechara, 2007), and the IGT (Xue et al., 2009). Several prior studies have reported DM, mainly IGT, as a moderator of the associations between amount of recent cannabis use and problems from cannabis use (Gonzalez, Schuster, Mermelstein, & Diviak, 2015), DSM-IV symptoms of cannabis use disorder (Gonzalez et al., 2012), and risky sexual behaviors (Schuster et al., 2012). Yet despite this, and evidence from others indicating a correlation between DM performance and weight among adolescents (Verdejo-García et al., 2010), we found no significant effects. The significantly larger sample size in our study (n = 238) would have allowed us to detect small effects, yet this was not the case. One notable difference between the current study and prior studies is our unique sample of participants. Our sample consisted of adolescents who were primarily Hispanic/Latino as well as cannabis users with a broad range of use. Prior studies from our group focused on young adults, the majority of which were non-Hispanic white. In addition, few participants in the current study met criteria for a mental health disorder. In contrast, Verdejo-García and colleagues (2010) included a sample of participants enrolled in a research-based treatment program for weight loss, which may indicate that they had greater problems with obesity compared to participants in the current study. The more severe nature of obesity among those adolescents may account for poorer DM performance observed among overweight adolescents compared to normal weight adolescents.

Our findings should be interpreted with some limitations in mind. Perhaps the biggest limitation of the current study is its reliance on a cross-sectional design. A longitudinal study that assesses how cannabis use, DM performance and the interaction of these variables are associated with BMI over time will allow us to make causal inferences about how cannabis use influences BMI. We note that the one longitudinal study examining associations between adolescent cannabis use and BMI in young adulthood reported that greater amounts of adolescent cannabis use were associated with a higher BMI in young adulthood (Huang, Lanza, & Anglin, 2013). Another important consideration is that our sample consists predominantly of Hispanic/Latino (76%) participants; thus, our results may not generalize to other groups. It is worth noting that the Hispanic/Latino population has one of the highest rates of obesity in the United States (Hedley et al., 2004). Although our findings cannot definitively establish causal associations between cannabis use and adolescent obesity, they suggest that among adolescents more cannabis use is associated with a higher BMI and greater likelihood of being classified as having an unhealthy weight. Thus, it follows that increases in cannabis use among teens may be accompanied with a higher incidence of unhealthy weight. Interventions for adolescent substance use should incorporate exercise components to reduce cannabis use and unhealthy weight as well as potentially improve cognition (Lisdahl, Gilbart, Wright, & Shollenbarger, 2013).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants R01 DA031176 and R01 DA033156 (PI: Gonzalez) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors have no other conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task™ Professional Manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50(1):7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson SW, Nathan PE. Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(4):376–389. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00136-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brand M, Fujiwara E, Borsutzky S, Kalbe E, Kessler J, Markowitsch HJ. Decision-making deficits of korsakoff patients in a new gambling task with explicit rules: Associations with executive functions. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(3):267. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brand M, Recknor EC, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A. Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: Correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2007;29:86–99. doi: 10.1080/13803390500507196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  7. Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Dawes MA, Furr RM, Charles NE, Liguori A, … Acheson A. Impulsivity, attention, memory, and decision-making among adolescent marijuana users. Psychopharmacology. 2013;226(2):307–319. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2908-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gonzalez R, Schuster RM, Mermelstein RM, Diviak KR. The role of decision-making in cannabis-related problems among young adults. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2015;154:214–221. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.06.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gonzalez R, Schuster RM, Mermelstein RJ, Vassileva J, Martin EM, Diviak KR. Performance of young adult cannabis users on neurocognitive measures of impulsive behavior and their relationship to symptoms of cannabis use disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2012;34(9):962–976. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.703642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Green B, Kavanagh D, Young R. Being stoned: A review of self-reported cannabis effects. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2003;22(4):453–460. doi: 10.1080/09595230310001613976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hedley AA, Ogden CL, Johnson CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, Flegal KM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity among US children, adolescents, and adults, 1999–2002. JAMA. 2004;291(23):2847–2850. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.23.2847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Huang DY, Lanza HI, Anglin MD. Association between adolescent substance use and obesity in young adulthood: A group-based dual trajectory analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38(11):2653–2660. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Miech RA, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national results on drug use: 1975–2013: Overview, Key Findings on Adolescent Drug Use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  15. Kouri E, Pope HG, Yurgelun-Todd D, Gruber S. Attributes of heavy vs. occasional marijuana smokers in a college population. Biological Psychiatry. 1995;38(7):475–481. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00325-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, … Johnson CL. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital and health statistics. Series 11, Data from the national health survey. 2002;246:1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lanza HI, Grella CE, Chung PJ. Does adolescent weight status predict problematic substance use patterns? American Journal of Health Behavior. 2014;38(5):708–716. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.38.5.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Le Strat Y, Le Foll B. Obesity and cannabis use: results from 2 representative national surveys. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2011:200. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Levin IP, Hart SS. Risk preferences in young children: Early evidence of individual differences in reaction to potential gains and losses. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2003;16(5):397–413. [Google Scholar]
  20. Lisdahl KM, Gilbart ER, Wright NE, Shollenbarger S. Dare to delay? The impacts of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use onset on cognition, brain structure, and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2013;53(4):1–19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Miech RA, Johnston L, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg J, Patrick ME. Trends in use of marijuana and attitudes toward marijuana among youth before and after decriminalization: The case of California 2007–2013. International Journal of Drug Policy. 2015;26(4):336–344. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2015.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pasch KE, Velazquez CE, Cance JD, Moe SG, Lytle LA. Youth substance use and body composition: Does risk in one area predict risk in the other? Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2012;41(1):14–26. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9706-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rippeth JD, Heaton RK, Carey CL, Marcotte TD, Moore DJ, Gonzalez R, … Grant I. Methamphetamine dependence increases risk of neuropsychological impairment in HIV infected persons. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10(01):1–14. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704101021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Schuster RM, Crane NA, Mermelstein R, Gonzalez R. The influence of inhibitory control and episodic memory on the risky sexual behavior of young adult cannabis users. Journal of International Neuropsychology. 2012;18(5):827–833. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Verdejo-García A, Pérez-Expósito M, Schmidt-Río-Valle J, Fernández-Serrano MJ, Cruz F, Pérez-García M, … Marcos A. Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity. 2010;18(8):1572–1578. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: Evidence of systems pathology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2008;363(1507):3191–3200. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Weller JA, Levin IP, Shiv B, Bechara A. Neural correlates of adaptive decision making for risky gains and losses. Psychological Science. 2007;18(11):958–964. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02009.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Xue G, Lu Z, Levin IP, Weller JA, Li X, Bechara A. Functional dissociations of risk and reward processing in the medial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(5):1019–1027. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES