Abstract
Background:
Workplace stress is known to be related with many behavioral and disease outcomes. However, little is known about its prospective relationship with measures of cognitive decline.
Objective:
To investigate the association of job strain, psychological demands and job control on cognitive decline.
Methods:
Participants from Framingham Offspring cohort (n=1429), were assessed on job strain, and received neuropsychological assessment approximately 15 years and 21 years afterwards.
Results:
High job strain and low control were associated with decline in verbal learning and memory. Job strain was associated with decline in word recognition skills. Active job and passive job predicted decline in verbal learning and memory relative to low strain jobs in the younger subgroup. Active job and demands were positively associated with abstract reasoning skills.
Conclusions:
Job strain and job control may influence decline in cognitive performance.
Keywords: Job strain, Cognition disorders, Work, Job demand, Active work, Passive work, Stress, psychological
TAKE-HOME MESSAGE
Chronic exposures to workplace psychosocial stressors have been related to a number of health outcomes and behavioral outcomes such as cardiovascular diseases and depression.
Chronic psychosocial stress, might play a significant role in the development of degenerative cognitive impairment, various neuropsychiatric ailments, and may also contribute to the disease severity as well.
Exposure to some chronic workplace psychosocial stressors, measured as high job strain and low job control, at midlife is associated with decline in verbal learning and memory, which is among early symptoms of Alzheimer's disease.
Active Job category and high psychological demand at work had a significant but protective association with abstract reason skills.
High job strain at work has a significant association with decline in memory and learning.
Introduction
Chronic exposures to workplace psychosocial stressors, a subtle outcome of work organization, have been known to predict adverse health and behavioral outcomes such as cardiovascular diseases and depression.1-7 The way work is organized, although anthropogenic, may expose workers to psychosocial stressors. These psychosocial stressors (stress risk factors) lead to stress response. The stress response from these workers, when prolonged and chronic, leads to risk of diseases, regardless of the workers’ awareness of the exposure and personality type.8,9 Although the Job Strain Hypothesis of the Demand-Control model10 has been related to a number of health outcomes,11-14 little or no work has been done studying its association with cognitive decline.15
Cognitive decline, a hallmark of dementia,16,17 is a public health problem. Alzheimer’s disease, the predominant subtype of dementia, and a corollary outcome of cognitive decline represents not only personal cost to the patient and their family but also a major economic burden on health care and social services.18 To date, possible etiology and pathogenesis for the cause of cognitive decline has been investigated and age and the presence of apolipoprotein (ApoE4) gene are established risk factors.19-21 Environmental factors, such as chronic psychosocial stress, might play a significant role in the development of degenerative cognitive impairment but has not been well studied.22 So far, no treatment has been found that can halt progression of Alzheimer’s disease after symptoms are detected.23 Identifying the risk factors associated with cognitive decline, and reducing or preventing exposure to these well before onset of symptoms may be the most effective protective measure.
Chronic psychosocial stress is a suspected risk factor for various neuropsychiatric ailments, and may also contribute to the disease severity as well.24-26 Chronic psychosocial stress has been found to significantly exacerbate impairment in cognition and long-term potentiation in rat models.27 A 5-year follow-up prospective study found long working hours to be associated with decline in cognitive performance.28 This, however, suggests that social and psychological exposure at the workplace may be associated with incidence and severity of cognitive decline.
Considering the lack of epidemiologic studies of the relationship between workplace psychosocial stressors and cognitive decline, the high and steady increase of workplace psychosocial stressors in the American workplace over the past two decades29 and the increasing incidence of Alzheimer’s disease,30 investigating the possible association between occupational workplace psychosocial stressors and cognitive decline is warranted. Epidemiologic evidence of association is of particular importance because it will provide insights into possible etiological factors.
The Job Strain hypothesis of the Demand-Control model developed by Karasek is based on a combination of psychological demand (job demand) and decision latitude (job control).10 The first hypothesis, the Job Strain Hypothesis, posits that adverse outcomes (diseases, fatigue, and depression) of psychological strain occur when psychological demands of the job are high and decision latitude is low. The second hypothesis, called Active Job Hypothesis, postulates that positive behavioral outcomes such as learning and growth and engagement in political and leisure activities (social participation) result when decision latitude is high and psychological demand is high. The Active quadrant is where decision latitude is high and psychological demand is high. The opposite of this quadrant is the passive quadrant where both decision latitude and psychological demand is low.10 Together, these two quadrants make up the Active Job Hypothesis. The low strain quadrant is where decision latitude is high and psychological demand is low. The opposite of this is the high strain quadrant where decision latitude is low and psychological demand is high. The high and low strain quadrants make up the Job Strain hypothesis.
In the model, decision latitude refers to the worker’s ability to control his or her own activities and skill usage, and not to control others. Decision latitude scales have two components: “decision authority,” a socially predetermined control over detailed aspects of task performance (also called autonomy); and “skill discretion,” control over use of skills by the individual, also socially determined at work (and often called variety). The latent variable (component), decision latitude (job control), is independently assessed as a primary independent variable. Psychological demand assesses mental work load (how hard a worker works) due to time pressure and role conflict (role ambiguity, concentration and mental work disruption). Psychological demand (job demand) is assessed as an independent variable. These two components (latent variables) are assessed by the items from the Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ).31,32
The specific questions addressed in this paper are whether a) decision latitude (job control), psychological and psychological demand (job demand) are independently associated with decline in cognitive performance, measured by Wechsler’s abstract reasoning test, Wechsler’s verbal learning and memory test, visual memory tests, and premorbid intelligence tests, and b) whether job strain (a combination of low job control and high job demand) is associated with cognitive decline, and how each category of the Demand-Control model is associated with cognitive decline with referent to low job strain category.
Materials and Methods
Study Participants
The Offspring cohort of the Framingham Heart Study (n=5124) consists of adult children (and their spouses) of the Original Cohort and were enrolled in the study in 1971 and longitudinally followed (examined) approximately every four years.33 At each examination (Exam), participants were screened for cardiovascular and other risk factors. Occupational and demographic information were also recorded. All participants provided signed consent approved by the Boston University Institutional Review Board.
During the 3rd examination (Exam 3) of the Offspring cohort, (1984-1987), participants job strain levels were assessed by the JCQ. Following the 7th and 8th examinations, (Exams 7 and 8), participants were invited for a call back study that included administration of a neuropsychological test battery (NP test).34,35 The acceptance rate for the neuropsychological battery test was 72%. The number of participants who attended Exam 3 and completed the JCQ (n=3682) and both the 1st NP test (n=2571) between 1999-2005, and the 2nd NP test (n=1723) between 2001-2007 was 1527. There was at least 1.0 year difference between the 1st and 2nd NP test and the median number of years between the 1st and 2nd NP tests was 6.0 years. After excluding participants who at the 1st NP test had prevalent stroke (n=22), dementia (n=2) or other neurological disorders that could affect cognitive testing (n=21), the study sample became 1482. After eliminating participants without ApoE4 data (n=53) the study sample size was 1429 (Fig 1).
Figure 1.
A flowchart of the sample selection criteria
Job insecurity and strain outside the job were, respectively, assessed using the statement “My job security is good” and the question “In general, do you find housework a big strain?” with a dichotomous response. Chronic and prolonged exposure to workplace psychosocial stressors was, in addition to the nature of the JCQ, ascertained by the number of times participants have changed their line of work and the number of times they have changed jobs in 10 years prior to the baseline assessment.
Job Strain
In measuring job strain, the psychological demand and decision latitude scales were divided into median based cut-points. Four possible combinations (groups) of job strain variables were created: a) low strain—low demands (below median) combined with high decision latitude (above median); b) active—high demands (above median) and high decision latitude (above median); c) passive—low demands (below median) and low decision latitude (below median); and d) high strain—high demands (above median) and low decision latitude (below median). In this analysis, all participants are grouped into one of the four categories—high strain, low strain, passive, and active categories.
Cognitive Decline (primary outcome)
Table 1 lists the cognitive test administered to the Offspring participants used in this study and the presumed domains for each test measures. The tests were administered and scored by trained examiners using standardized test administration procedures.36 For the primary outcomes, the variables of interest included are longitudinal annualized change (cognitive decline) in the scales in Table 1.
Table 1: The cognitive tests administered at the 1st and 2nd NP exams and the cognitive domains they measure | |
Neuropsyscological test | Latent cognitive ability tested |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) | |
Similarities | Abstract reasoning |
Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) | |
Paired Associate Learning—immediate recall (PASI) Paired Associate Learning—delayed recall (PASD) |
Verbal learning |
Logical Memory—immediate recall (LMI) Logical Memory—delayed recall (LMD) |
Verbal memory |
Visual Reproductions—immediate recall (VRI) Visual Reproductions—delayed recall (VRD) |
Visual memory |
Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)—III Word Reading Subtest |
Premorbid intelligence and word recognition skills |
In all analyses, a negative relationship between psychosocial exposures and cognitive decline indicates lower performance in the 2nd NP tests in comparison with the 1st NP test.
Abstract reasoning skills are measured by similarities test, a frequently used and validated measure from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS). Abstract reasoning skills represent the ability to analyze information to solve complex problems. It is important because it has been found to be very effective in differential diagnosis of dementias through cognitive decline.37 Learning and memory (including verbal memory) was measured by Wechsler Memory Scale’s (WMS) subsets: Paired Associate Learning (immediate and delayed recall) test and Logical Memory (immediate and delayed recall) test. The Logical Memory test has been used extensively for measuring verbal memory. Its delayed test is in particular significantly effective in assessing long-term memory.44 Paired Associate Learning measures the lack of ability to learn new information is a hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease. The Paired Associate Learning test is accurate in measuring learning and memory to assess cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease, and it is particularly effective in early detection of symptoms of dementia.44 Premorbid intelligence is the decline in intelligence attributed to cognitive decline and not old age. Wide Range Achievement test is an essential tool in accurately assessing the degree and rate of intelligence decline caused at the premorbid stage of demntia.38,44
Statistical Analysis
Baseline characteristics (median and frequencies) were determined for sociodemographic and relevant variables during the 1st and 2nd NP tests.
Multivariate linear regression analysis was performed on all annualized raw change scores between the 1st and 2nd NP tests and job strain, job demand and job control separately (n=1429). For each participant, the annualized raw change was calculated as follows:
Baseline performance of each cognitive performance was adjusted for in all regression analysis. Two models for adjustments were tested to control the covariates as follows:
1) Model 1
i) For Job Demand:Outcome = Age + Sex + Education + Control + 1st NP test
ii) For Control:Outcome = Age + Sex + Education + Job demand + 1st NP test
iii) For Job Strain:Outcome = Age + Sex + Education + 1st NP test
2) Model 2 (for demand, control and job strain): Outcome = Age + Sex + Education + Socioeconomic status (income) + Strain outside work + Job insecurity + ApoE4 + 1st NP test
Age stratified analysis was carried out with younger subgroup (median age at baseline ≤46 years, ie , the collection on job strain data) and elderly subgroup (median age at baseline >46 years).
ApoE, a genetic marker of risk for cognitive impairment39 was adjusted for in Model 2. The process how information on ApoE was obtained is explained elsewhere.40 A significant level of 0.05 was set for all analyses. SAS ver 9.3 was used for statistical analysis.
Results
Basic Characteristics
Demographics, baseline and follow-up characteristics of the 1429 study participants are provided in Table 2. The median age at baseline was 46 (IQR 40 to 54) years, and at 1st NP test was 61 (IQR 55 to 69) years. The median years of work experience at baseline was 24 (IQR 17 to 31) years. The median number of times participants changed job in the last 10 years prior to the baseline was 1 (IQR 0 to 2) and the median number of times the participants changed their line of work in 10 years was 0 (IQR 0 to 1). The median time between the 1st and 2nd NP tests was 6 (IQR 5 to 6) years. About 67% of workers said they have good job security; 27.4% of the participants were in the active job category; 53.0% had low job control; and 47% had high psychological demands in their workplace. Sixty-two and two-tenth percent of the participants did not find housework a big strain. More than half (53.9%) of the participants were female and over 92% had a minimum of high school degree. Twenty and half percent of the participants were ApoE4 genotype carriers.
Table 2: Demographic and risk factors characteristics. Values are either median (IQR) or percentage. | |||
Job strain, NP tests and ApoE4 | All (n=1429) | Middle age (n=866) | Elderly (n=563) |
Age at Exam 3 (yrs) | 46 (40–54) | 41 (37–45) | 56 (53–60) |
Age at 1st NP test (yrs) | 61 (55–69) | 56 (52–60) | 71 (68–74) |
Time between Exam 3 and 1st NP test (yrs) | 14 (13–15) | 14 (14–15) | 14 (13–15) |
Time between 1st and 2nd NP test (yrs) | 6 (5–6) | 6 (5–6) | 6 (5–6) |
Years of work experience | 24 (17–31) | 20 (15–25) | 34 (27–40) |
No. of times change job in past 10 yrs | 1 (0–2) | 1 (0–2) | 0 (0–1) |
No. of times change line of work in 10 yrs | 0 (0–1) | 0 (0–1) | 0 (0–0) |
Gender | |||
Men | 46.1% | 45.5% | 46.2% |
Women | 53.9% | 54.5% | 53.8% |
Educational level | |||
No high school | 5.5% | 3.8% | 8.0% |
High school degree | 47.7% | 44.2% | 52.9% |
Associate degree | 9.7% | 10.7% | 8.2% |
College degree and above | 35.1% | 39.2% | 29.0% |
Do you find housework a big strain | |||
Yes | 21.1% | 23.8% | 16.9% |
No | 62.2% | 61.7% | 63.1% |
Unknown/not applicable | 16.7% | 14.5% | 19.0% |
Income before tax at Exams 3 (per yr) | |||
≤ US$ 40 000 | 66.7% | 67.2% | 65.9% |
> US$ 40 000 | 18.9% | 19.8% | 16.9% |
Unknown | 14.6% | 12.9% | 17.2% |
Job security is good | |||
Yes | 67.1% | 77.5% | 74.4% |
No | 7.5% | 8.7% | 6.9% |
Unknown | 25.4% | 13.9% | 18.7% |
Job strain | |||
Low | 25.5% | 23.9% | 27.9% |
High strain | 12.5% | 13.1% | 11.7% |
Job control | |||
High | 47.0% | 48.7% | 40.5% |
Low | 53.0% | 51.3% | 59.5% |
Active job | 27.4% | 32.3% | 19.9% |
Passive job | 34.6% | 30.7% | 27.9% |
Psychological demand | |||
High | 46.3% | 48.5% | 38.2% |
Low | 53.4% | 51.5% | 61.8% |
Job Strain and Cognitive Decline
There was a significant negative correlation between measures of decline in verbal learning and memory (PASI, Table 3a), and high job strain in all studied models, (Model 1: mean β –0.14 [SEM 0.06], p=0.006; Model 2: –0.15 [0.06], p=0.005). The association was significant in the younger subgroup (Model 1: –0.14 [0.06], p=0.008). High job strain was also associated significantly with a decline in delayed verbal memory and learning (PASD) (Model 1: –0.05 [0.02], p=0.02; Model 2: –0.06 [0.03], p=0.008).
Table 3a: Association of job strain with annual change in cognitive performance, n=1429. Values are mean β (SEM), and p values. | |||||||||||
NP Test | Model 1—Job Strain | Model 2—Job Strain | |||||||||
All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | ||||||
ΔPASI | |||||||||||
High strain | –0.14 (0.06), 0.006 | –0.14 (0.06), 0.008 | –0.14 (0.1), 0.16 | –0.15 (0.06), 0.005 | –0.11 (0.06), 0.06 | –0.20 (0.12), 0.06 | |||||
Active | –0.06 (0.04), 0.12 | –0.10 (0.04), 0.02 | 0.01 (0.04), 0.96 | –0.07 (0.05), 0.10 | –0.10 (0.05), 0.02 | 0.02 (0.05), 0.78 | |||||
Passive | –0.06 (0.04), 0.14 | –0.09 (0.04), 0.05 | –0.02 (0.04), 0.77 | –0.08 (0.05), 0.10 | –0.08 (0.05), 0.12 | –0.04 (0.05), 0.62 | |||||
ΔPASD | |||||||||||
High strain | –0.05 (0.02), 0.02 | –0.04 (0.02), 0.07 | –0.07 (0.04), 0.11 | –0.06 (0.03), 0.008 | –0.03 (0.03), 0.14 | –0.08 (0.03), 0.06 | |||||
Active | 0.02 (0.02), 0.25 | 0.03 (0.02), 0.24 | 0.01 (0.02), 0.74 | 0.02 (0.02), 0.67 | 0.02 (0.02), 0.23 | 0.03 (0.02), 0.39 | |||||
Passive | –0.01 (0.02), 0.17 | –0.01 (0.02), 0.51 | –0.01 (0.02), 0.71 | –0.03 (0.02), 0.26 | –0.24 (0.02), 0.31 | –0.02 (0.02), 0.66 | |||||
ΔLMI | |||||||||||
High strain | –0.04 (0.06), 0.22 | –0.002 (0.06), 0.23 | –0.10 (0.06), 0.25 | –0.04 (0.06), 0.29 | 0.003 (0.06), 0.96 | –0.11 (0.06), 0.32 | |||||
Active | –0.01 (0.05), 0.53 | –0.01 (0.05), 0.52 | –0.01 (0.05), 0.52 | 0.01 (0.05), 0.51 | 0.02 (0.05), 0.73 | –0.05 (0.05), 0.57 | |||||
Passive | –0.03 (0.05), 0.12 | –0.10 (0.06), 0.21 | 0.05 (0.06), 0.35 | –0.04 (0.06), 0.26 | –0.05 (0.06), 0.29 | 0.003 (0.06), 0.33 | |||||
ΔLMD | |||||||||||
High strain | –0.01 (0.06), 0.80 | 0.003 (0.06), 0.96 | –0.05 (0.06), 0.54 | –0.02 (0.07), 0.68 | 0.04 (0.07), 0.58 | –0.10 (0.07), 0.52 | |||||
Active | –0.03 (0.05), 0.81 | 0.01 (0.05), 0.86 | 0.04 (0.05), 0.85 | 0.01 (0.05), 0.57 | 0.03 (0.05), 0.58 | –0.02 (0.05), 0.26 | |||||
Passive | –0.03 (0.06), 0.23 | –0.11 (0.06), 0.04 | 0.07 (0.06), 0.30 | –0.04 (0.06), 0.31 | –0.08 (0.06), 0.18 | –0.03 (0.06), 0.96 | |||||
ΔSimilarities | |||||||||||
High strain | 0.03 (0.06), 0.50 | 0.07 (0.06), 0.21 | –0.03 (0.06), 0.95 | –0.01 (0.06), 0.82 | –0.001 (0.06), 0.99 | –0.08 (0.06), 0.58 | |||||
Active | 0.08 (0.04), 0.05 | 0.11 (0.04), 0.02 | 0.01 (0.04), 0.54 | 0.09 (0.04), 0.03 | 0.11 (0.04), 0.02 | 0.06 (0.04), 0.23 | |||||
Passive | 0.01 (0.04), 0.76 | –0.002 (0.04), 0.96 | 0.03 (0.04), 0.47 | 0.04 (0.05), 0.35 | 0.01 (0.05), 0.87 | 0.10 (0.05), 0.52 | |||||
ΔWRAT | |||||||||||
High strain | –0.10 (0.04 ), 0.02 | –0.10 (0.04), 0.08 | –0.10 (0.05), 0.29 | –0.13 (0.05), 0.004 | –0.11 (0.05), 0.04 | –0.11 (0.05), 0.17 | |||||
Active | –0.01 (0.03), 0.68 | –0.01 (0.03), 0.86 | –0.03 (0.03 0.38 | –0.02 (0.03), 0.51 | –0.02 (0.03), 0.64 | –0.04 (0.03), 0.71 | |||||
Passive | 0.001 (0.04), 0.97 | 0.03 (0.04), 0.23 | 0.04 (0.04 ), 0.91 | 0.002 (0.03), 0.99 | 0.02 (0.03), 0.36 | 0.02 (0.03), 0.63 | |||||
Δ: Change in Model 1 = age + sex + years of education + (job demand or control or job strain) Model 2 = age + sex + education + socioeconomic status (income) + strain outside work + job insecurity |
High job strain was also significantly related with a decline in measures of premorbid intelligence (WRAT) (Model 1: –0.10 [0.05], p=0.02; Model 2: –0.13 [0.05], p=0.004). The association was significant in the younger subgroup (Model 2: –0.11 [0.05], p=0.04). The remaining NP tests had no significant association with job strain (Table 3a).
Active and Passive Work and Cognitive Decline
There was a significant association between active job and decline in verbal learning and memory (PASI) in the younger subgroup (Table 3a) (Model 1: mean β –0.10 [SEM 0.04], p=0.02; Model 2: –0.10 [0.05], p=0.02). The association of passive job was marginally significant with decline in verbal learning and memory in the younger subgroup (Model 1: –0.09 [0.04], p=0.05). Passive job was significantly related to lower premorbid intelligence (WRAT) in the younger group category (Model 1: –0.10 [0.04], p=0.02; Model 2: –0.13 [0.04], p=0.01). Increase in abstract reasoning skills measured by similarities scores was significantly associated with increase active job in Model 2 (0.09 [0.04], p=0.04), but that relationship was attenuated in Model 1 (0.08 [0.04], p=0.05). This appeared driven by the significant protective association in the younger age group (Model 1: 0.11 [0.04], p=0.02; Model 2: 0.11 [0.04], p=0.02) since there were no significant associations within the elderly group.
All the other NP tests had no significant association with passive and active work.
Demand and Cognitive Decline
Multivariate analysis revealed a significant association between increased psychological demand at work and better performance on abstract reasoning skills (Model 1: mean β 0.08 [SEM 0.03], p=0.02) (Table 3b). The association was present in the younger subgroup (Model 1: 0.08 [0.03], p=0.02; Model 2: 0.11 [0.04], p=0.004) but not the elderly group. The younger subgroup also showed a positive correlation between demand and delayed logical memory in both models (Model 1: 0.10 [0.04], p=0.01; Model 2: 0.11 [0.04], p=0.01). There were no other significant relationships between demand and other NP tests (Table 3b).
Table 3b: Association of demand with annual change in cognitive performance, n= 1429. Values are mean β (SEM), and p values. | |||||||
NP Test | Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | ||
ΔPASI | –0.05 (0.03), 0.10 | –0.04 (0.03), 0.19 | –0.06 (0.03), 0.32 | –0.05 (0.04), 0.14 | –0.03 (0.04), 0.36 | –0.07 (0.04), 0.31 | |
ΔPASD | 0.01 (0.01), 0.53 | 0.02 (0.02), 0.29 | 0.005 (0.02), 0.85 | 0.02 (0.02), 0.27 | 0.03 (0.02), 0.09 | –0.003 (0.04), 0.9 | |
ΔLMI | –0.01 (0.04), 0.84 | 0.05 (0.04), 0.17 | –0.11 (0.04), 0.08 | –0.01 (0.04), 0.81 | 0.05 (0.04), 0.24 | –0.12 (0.04), 0.08 | |
ΔLMD | 0.05 (0.04), 0.16 | 0.10 (0.04), 0.01 | –0.05 (0.04), 0.38 | 0.04 (0.04), 0.30 | 0.11 (0.04), 0.01 | –0.07 (0.04), 0.29 | |
ΔSimilarities | 0.07 (0.03), 0.02 | 0.12 (0.04), 0.004 | –0.02 (0.04), 0.74 | 0.06 (0.04), 0.09 | 0.11 (0.04), 0.004 | –0.05 (0.04), 0.64 | |
ΔWRAT | 0.01 (0.03), 0.57 | 0.02 (0.03), 0.54 | 0.0001 (0.03), 0.99 | –0.004 (0.03), 0.89 | 0.005 (0.03), 0.90 | –0.02 (0.03), 0.72 | |
Δ: Change in Model 1 = age + sex + years of education + (job demand or control or job strain) Model 2 = age + sex + education + socioeconomic status (income), + strain outside work + job insecurity |
Control and Cognitive Decline
Multivariate analysis indicated a significant association for low control at the workplace and decline in verbal learning and memory (Table 3c), and even stronger association after adjusting for all relevant covariates (Model 1: mean β –0.08 [SEM 0.04], p=0.011; Model 2: –0.11 [0.04], p=0.003), similarly there was significant association for low control and decline in delayed verbal learning and memory even after adjustments of all covariates (Model 1: –0.03 [0.01], p=0.03; Model 2: –0.05 [0.02], p=0.001). Similar associations were found in the younger subgroup but not the elderly subgroup (Model 1: –0.05 [0.01], p=0.03; Model 2: –0.06 [0.02], p=0.004). There was a marginal negative association between low control and WRAT (Model 2: –0.06 [0.04], p=0.05) and significant association in both Models for the younger subgroup (Model 1: –0.07 [0.03], p=0.04; Model 2: –0.08 [0.03], p=0.03). Control was not significantly associated with the remaining NP tests.
Table 3c: Association of Job Control with Annual Change in Cognitive Performance, n= 1429. Values are mean β (SEM), and p values. | |||||||
NP Test | Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | All | <65 yrs | ≥65 yrs | ||
ΔPASI | –0.08 (0.04), 0.02 | –0.06 (0.04), 0.08 | –0.10 (0.04), 0.11 | –0.11 (0.04), 0.003 | – 0.06 (0.04), 0.12 | – 0.15 (0.04), 0.03 | |
ΔPASD | –0.03 (0.01), 0.02 | –0.05 (0.01), 0.01 | –0.02 (0.01), 0.35 | –0.05 (0.02), 0.001 | –0.06 (0.02), 0.004 | –0.02 (0.04), 0.37 | |
ΔLMI | –0.03 (0.04), 0.34 | –0.03 (0.04), 0.36 | –0.02 (0.04), 0.90 | –0.06 (0.04), 0.10 | –0.05 (0.04), 0.29 | –0.05 (0.04), 0.54 | |
ΔLMD | –0.04 (0.04), 0.22 | –0.04 (0.04), 0.34 | –0.04 (0.04), 0.53 | –0.07 (0.04), 0.12 | –0.06 (0.04), 0.41 | –0.07 (0.04 0.37 | |
ΔSimilarities | –0.01 (0.04 0.33 | 0.0001 (0.04), 0.99 | 0.01 (0.04), 0.87 | –0.001 (0.04), 0.18 | –0.3 (0.04), 0.41 | 0.05 (0.04), 0.45 | |
ΔWRAT | –0.03 (0.03), 0.21 | –0.07 (0.03), 0.04 | 0.01 (0.03), 0.74 | –0.06 (0.03), 0.05 | –0.08 (0.03), 0.03 | 0.01 (0.03), 0.90 | |
Δ: Change in Model 1 = age + sex + years of education + (job demand or control or job strain) Model 2 = age + sex + education + socioeconomic status (income), + strain outside work + job insecurity |
Discussion
This study indicates that the exposure to some chronic workplace psychosocial stressors, measured as high job strain and low job control, at midlife, in a sample of 1429 Caucasian residents of Framingham without dementia, stroke and/or other neurodegenerative diseases, is associated with decline in verbal learning and memory and the association appears to be driven by the younger subgroup of participants. Also high job strain and low job control is associated with decline in word recognition skills and word reading performance. An unexpected finding, however, was improved abstract reasoning skills associated with increasing job demands and active job category.
The study results indicate that being exposed chronically to workplace psychosocial stressors as measured by lack of job control significantly predicted a decline in verbal learning and memory, which is mainly driven by exposed middle-aged participants. Lacking the ability to learn new information is a hallmark of Alzheimer’s, in its early symptoms. Lack of job control again predicted a decline in word recognition skills, which is a measure of premorbid intelligence, which again was driven mainly by the exposed middle-aged participants.
Contrary to expectation, high psychological demand at work had a significant but protective association with abstract reasoning skills as measured by WAIS Similarities test mainly driven by the middle-aged subgroup. Demand again had a significantly protective association with verbal memory. None of the other NP tests was susceptible to psychological demands due mental load due to time pressure and role conflict at the workplace.
Chronic exposure to high job strain, a combination of deleterious levels of psychological demand and job control, was a significant predictor for decline in verbal learning and memory, long-term memory and word recognition skills with respect to low job strain.
Exposure to the active job category and passive job category also predicted a decline in verbal learning and memory, respectively, relative to low job strain category in the younger subgroup, suggesting that the best work organization design with regards to the demand-control model is the low job strain category (with respect to memory and verbal learning). A positive and significant association in abstract reasoning skills was predicted by active jobs in relation to low job strain. This could be explained by the active learning hypothesis and the concept of “flow”41 where there is a high control or wide latitude for decision making to overcome the challenges of demands and instantly receiving feedback of actions. With this concept, the employee is cognitively stimulated and has no time to brood over failures as he is moving on to the next challenge even in the upper decades of life. Active jobs therefore could improve positive outcomes such as abstract reasoning. Karasek42 found an association between active job category workers and leisure and popular activities off work despite their high demands at work.
Overall, abstract reasoning skills seem to increase with increase in demand and active job especially in a middle-aged worker. Reasons that could explain the effect of these positive associations between increases in demands with abstract reasoning skills may include the fact that psychological demand measures the mental load from challenges, and average psychological demand could be translated into direct actions, ie , problem-solving challenges, with little residual strain to cause disturbance.43 A subgroup of participants with moderately high job demand, ie , slightly above the median cut-point, just close to the tip of the “inverse U relationship” in eustress as explained in Selye’s General Adaptation syndrome,44 could be contributing to this effect. This may also explain the positive association between job demand and logical/verbal memory.
Decline in learning ability and memory are common after chronic exposure to all measures of job strain hypothesis: control, high job strain, active job category, and passive job category, except demand. These associations are in congruence with the stress physiological mechanism that chronic infusion of glucocorticoid to the hippocampus disrupts memory.9 Additionally, the lack of discretion limits the use of skills one possesses to execute the challenges of the demands, and thus the process of learning new things is inhibited.
With one exception, all the negative associations were to employees in the younger group (≤46 years at baseline). This result is consistent with previous studies on job strain and cardiovascular outcomes where increasing age attenuated the association between job strain and cardiovascular diseases. Reasons for this could be multifactorial. For example, it could be that only employees who remain mentally intact (due to better coping mechanism, personality type, better susceptibility, etc ) stay at their jobs longer and this may create an over-representation of people in the high strain group at the older age group category. Furthermore, the impact of job stress on cognitive function may be temporary and whatever impact job stress has on cognition may dissipate as one grows and become adapted. Also, considering that relatively higher percentage of the younger group were in the high demand and high strain jobs (Table 2), it may be that younger professionals in the process of building their career are exposed to more stressors relative to their older counterparts who may be more settled into their maximum level and/or well versed or adapted in their job. This may therefore result in an age-restricted positive association or no significant associations in the older group. Moreover, the cognitive performance of the older group may have already been lowered and stabilized before the 1st NP test and hence resulting in minimal or no decline between the two NP tests.
The association of job strain and its components to cognitive decline has not been previously reported although the demand-control model posits that adverse reactions of psychological strain occur when psychological demands of the job are high and the worker’s job control is simultaneously low.10 However, recent studies have linked low cognitive performance to long working hours33 and also low job control at old age.45 In Virtanen, et al ,33 a 5-year follow-up study that explored long working hours (a workplace psychosocial stressor) and cognitive decline of middle-aged white-collar office workers, showed a decline in performance on tests of intelligence. Other human studies have resulted that stressful events and experiences have significant association with a reduction in word recall in older healthy individuals46 and reduction in recall of words on a complex memory task.47
In spite of the paucity of epidemiological research studies of workplace stress and cognitive performance, the physiological mechanism is quite clear. In response to threatening events, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis releases glucocorticoids that affects specific areas of the brain including hippocampus, a structure critical for memory. Chronic exposure to stress would cause prolong release of glucocorticoids that results in damage to the hippocampus that adversely affects memory.48 Exposure to chronic stress disrupts hippocampal-dependent memory in laboratory animals by a) releasing tons of glucocorticoids which activate low affinity receptors. These low affinity receptors disrupt the activities of the high affinity receptors (which enhances long-term potentiation) and consequently weaken long-term potentiation, b) chronic stress causes atrophy in the axons and dendrites and thus destroys the neural network, c) neurogenesis is also inhibited with all forms of growth during sympathetic response, d) chronic stress also results in endangering hippocampal neurons mainly due to inhibition of glucose storage and glucocorticoid neurotoxicity.
Overall, this study is consistent with theories and epidemiological findings that chronic stress leads to a decline in some aspects of cognition that may lead to higher risk for Alzheimer’s disease and other dementia subtypes. Additionally, this study has shown the significant associations between midlife exposure to workplace psychosocial stressors alone—devoid of other stressors elsewhere—and cognitive decline, over two decades later.
A number of the NP tests showed no significant association with job strain and its components, but the results is comparable to other studies that used these NP tests on even easily measurable or tangible factors. In Debette, et al ,49 about 17% of the six NP tests used (the present study used eight), had some form of association with either hypertension, systolic blood pressure, diabetes mellitus, smoking, hypercholesterolemia, obesity and/or waist-to-hip ratio. Bangen, et al ,50 reported less than 20% associations between nine NP tests with four vascular risk factors such as hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, diabetes mellitus, and smoking. This study shows 17% association between the job strain variables and the NP tests.
The strengths of this study include that it was based in a community-based sample, the comprehensive assessment of both workplace psychosocial stressors with prospective follow up for cognitive decline, and the ability to adjust for relevant confounders. This study however, has some limitations. Participants of this study were primarily of European decent, limiting the generalizability of the study results to other populations. Also, over 96% of participants had at least high school diploma, which may somewhat limit the generalizability for populations with lower educational background. Multiple analyses and comparisons were used in the study. As an exploratory study, multiple analyses were used to explore and gain ideas and insights regarding possible associations and we acknowledge these findings need to be duplicated in other studies. The approximately 6-year follow-up on the 1st and 2nd NP test may not be sufficient to detect more subtle changes in cognition, and also is not sufficient for relating to incident dementia.
In conclusion, workplace psychosocial stressors as measured as high job strain, low job control and high psychological demand at midlife independently predicts some hallmarks and measures of cognitive decline, generally considered endophenotypes of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. This finding suggests that primary prevention of workplace psychosocial stressors at midlife may reduce the incidence of old age dementia. Primary prevention is crucial in combating Alzheimer’s disease since no treatment has been found to halt the disease after symptoms have begun. This is an initial exploratory study. Therefore, the importance and need for further research that could lead to confirmation of predictive associations of workplace psychosocial stressors on later life cognitive decline is warranted. Also future studies involving neuroimaging or neuropathological data will help identify the mechanisms underlying these relationships. These studies could potentially impact occupational health preventive strategies and workplace policies.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Karen Mutalik of Framingham Heart Study for facilitating acquisition of the data.
Conflicts of Interest:
None declared.
Funding
This study was supported by grants from National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contract (N01-HC-25195), from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 NS17950), and from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG16495; AG08122).
Cite this article as: Agbenyikey W, Karasek R, Cifuentes M, et al. Job strain and cognitive decline: A prospective study of the Framingham offspring cohort. Int J Occup Environ Med 2015;6:79-94.
References
- 1.Kawakami N, Fujigaki Y. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of Job Content Questionnaire: replication and extension in computer company employees. Ind Health. 1996;34:295–306. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.34.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schwartz J, Pieper C, Karasek RA. A procedure for linking job characteristics to health surveys. Am J Public Health. 1988;78:904–9. doi: 10.2105/ajph.78.8.904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Theorell T, Karasek RA. Current issues relating to psychosocial job strain and cardiovascular disease research. J Occup Health Psychol. 1996;1:9–26. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.1.1.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schnal PL, Landsbergis PA. Job strain and cardiovascular disease. Annu Rev Public Health. 1994;15:381–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.15.050194.002121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kristensen TS. Cardiovascular diseases and the work environment: a critical review of the epidemiologic literature on chemical factors. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1989;15:245–64. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kivimaki M, Virtanen M, Elovainio M. et al. Work stress in the etiology of coronary heart disease–a meta-analysis. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2006;32:431–42. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Melchior M, Caspi A, Milne B. et al. Work stress precipitates depression and anxiety in young, working women and men. Psychol Med. 2008;37:1119–29. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Johnson JV. Occupational Stress, in Preventing Disease and Injury. eds. Weeks JL, Levy BS, Wagner GR. Washington DC, American Public Health Association, 2004.
- 9. Sapolsky RM. Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers. 3rd ed. New York, Henry Holt and Co, 2004.
- 10. Karasek RA, Theorell T. Healthy Work. New York, Basic Books, 1990.
- 11.Belkic KL, Landsbergis PA, Schnall PL. et al. Is job strain a major source of cardiovascular disease risk? A critical review of the empirical evidence, with a clinical perspective. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2004;30:85–128. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Stansfeld S, Candy B. Psychosocial work environment and mental health – a meta-analytic review. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2006;32:443–62. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Östergren PO, Hanson BS, Balogh I. et al. Incidence of shoulder and neck pain in a working population: effect modification between mechanical and psychosocial exposures at work? Results from a one-year follow-up of the Malmö shoulder and neck study cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59:721–8. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.034801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bongers PM, de Winter CR, Kompier MA, Hilderbrandt VH. Psychosocial factors at work and musculoskeletal disease. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1993;19:297–312. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Peterson RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M. et al. Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognition impairment (an evidence-based review): report of the quality standards subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2001;56:1133–42. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.9.1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP. et al. Mild cognitive impairment represents early stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:397–405. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Chertkow H. Mild cognitive impairment. Curr Opin Neurol. 2002;15:735–41. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200208000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ernst RL, Hay JW. The US economic and social costs of Alzheimer’s disease revisited. Am J Public Health. 1994;84:1261–4. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.8.1261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schiepers OJ, Harris SE, Gow AJ. et al. APOE E4 status predicts age-related cognitive decline in the ninth decade: longitudinal follow-up of the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:315–24. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bretsky p, Guralnik JM, Launer L. et al. The role of APOE-ε4 in longitudinal cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Neurology. 2003;60:1077–81. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000055875.26908.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Master CL, Beyreuther K. Science, medicine and the future: Alzheimer’s disease. BMJ. 1998;316:446–8. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7129.446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Grant WB, Cambell A, Itzhaki Savory J. The significance of environmental in the etiology of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2002;4:179–89. doi: 10.3233/jad-2002-4308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. BU ADC Bulletin. Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center. Spring/Summer 2012.
- 24.Vanitallie TB. Stress: A risk factor for serious illness. Metabolism. 2002;51:40–5. doi: 10.1053/meta.2002.33191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.McEwen BS. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1999;22:105–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gerges NZ, Alzoubi KH. Adverse effect of the combination of hypothyroidism and chronic psychosocial stress on hippocampus-dependent memory in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2004;155:77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Srivareerat M, Tran TT, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA. Chronic psychosocial stress exacerbates impairment of cognition and long-term potentiation in β-amyloid rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:918–26. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Virtanen M, Singh-Manoux A, Ferrie JE. et al. Long working hours and Cognitive function. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169:596–605. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. NIOSH Publications and Products. STRESS: Stress at work. Available from www.cdc.gov/niosh/docs/99-101/ (Accessed November 18, 2011).
- 30. Alzheimer’s Association. Alzheimer’s Disease Fact and Figures. 2011, Available from www.alz.org/downloads/Facts_Figures_2011.pdf (Accessed November 18, 2011).
- 31.Theorell T, Harms-Ringdahl K, Ahlberg-Hulten G. et al. Psychosocial job factors and symptoms from the locomotor system–a multicausal analysis. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1991;23:165–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Karasek R, Choi B, Ostergren P. et al. Testing two methods to create comparable scale scores between the Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ) and JCQ-like Questionnaires in the European JACE study. Int J Behav Med. 2007;14:189–201. doi: 10.1007/BF03002993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kannel WB, Feinleib M, McNamara PM. et al. An investigation of the coronary heart disease in families The Framingham Offspring Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;110:281–90. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press; 1983. pp: 107, 145-463, 556-63.
- 35.Farmer ME, White LR, Kittner SJ. et al. Role of age, education, and gender on cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study: community-based norms. Psychol Rep. 1987;60:1023–40. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Au R, Wolf PA, Beiser A, Seshadri S. New norms for a new generation: cognitive performance in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Exp Aging Res. 2004;30:333–58. doi: 10.1080/03610730490484380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Caffara P, Scaglionni A, Malvezzi L. et al. Fuld’s formula and WAIS subtests in differential diagnosis of dementia. Funct Neurol. 1991;6:23–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bushell C, Martin A. Automatic semantic priming of nouns and verbs in patients with Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:1059–67. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00046-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Farlow MR, He Y, Tekin S. et al. Impact of APOE in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2004;63:1898–901. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144279.21502.b7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lahoz C, Schaefer EJ, Cupples LA. et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype and cardiovascular disease in the Framingham Heart Study. Atherosclerosis. 2001;154:529–37. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9150(00)00570-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Csikszentmihalyi M. Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass, 1975.
- 42. Karasek RA. Job socialization: a longitudinal study of work, political and leisure activity in Sweden. Paper presented at the Ninth Congress of Sociology (RC30), Upsala, Swedish Institute for Social Research, Stockholm University, August 15, 1978.
- 43. White RW. Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychol Rev 1959;66297-333. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 44. Selye H. Stress without Distress. Philadelphia, JB Lippincott Co, 1974.
- 45.Andel R, Crowe M, Kareholt I. et al. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;66:287–91. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lupien SJ, Gaudreau S, Tchiteya BM. et al. Stress-induces declarative memory impairment in healthy elderly subjects: relationship to cortisol reactivity. J Clin Endocrinol Metabol. 1997;82:2070–5. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.7.4075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Wilfing J, Andrews B, Hejdenberg J. Relationship between life difficulties, measures of working memory operation, and examination performance in a student sample. Memory. 2007;15:57–62. doi: 10.1080/09658210601106447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Peavy GM, Salmon DP, Jacobson MW. et al. Effect of chronic stress on memory decline in cognitively normal and mildly impaired older adults. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:1384–91. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Debette S, Seshadri S, Au R. et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurobiology. 2011;77:461–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bangen KJ, Beiser A, Delano-Wood L. et al. APOE genotype modifies the relationship between midlife vascular risk factors and later cognitive decline. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2013;22:1361–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]