Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2004 Nov 2;101(45):16058–16063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401472101

A clock gene, period, plays a key role in long-term memory formation in Drosophila

Takaomi Sakai *,†,, Takuya Tamura *, Toshihiro Kitamoto , Yoshiaki Kidokoro *
PMCID: PMC528738  PMID: 15522971

Abstract

The cAMP-responsive transcription factor, CREB, is required for formation of long-term memory (LTM) in Drosophila melanogaster and regulates transcription of a circadian clock gene, period (per). Involvement of CREB both in LTM and circadian rhythm raises the possibility that per also plays a role in LTM. Assaying the experience-dependent courtship inhibition in male flies as a measure for LTM, we show here that per mutants are defective in LTM formation. This defect was rescued by induction of a wild-type per transgene in a per-null mutant, and overexpression of per enhanced LTM formation in the wild-type background. Furthermore, we found that synaptic transmission through per-expressing cells is most likely to be required during retrieval of LTM. In contrast, mutations in other clock genes (timeless, dClock, and cycle) did not affect LTM formation. Thus, independent of the core oscillator of circadian clock, per plays a key role in LTM formation.


The cAMP signaling cascade plays a central role in the formation of memory (1). In Drosophila melanogaster, the genetic analyses of olfactory conditioning that are based on electric shocks associated with olfactory cues have identified several genes participating in the cAMP signaling cascade as critical contributors to memory formation (26). These genes include rutabaga (rut; Ca2+/calmodulin-responsive adenylyl cyclase), dunce (dnc; cAMP phosphodiesterase), DC0 (a catalytic subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase A) and amnesiac (amn; an adenylyl cyclase-activating peptide). Mutant flies for these genes are defective in memory that lasts minutes (rut and dnc) or hours (DC0 and amn) after conditioning. This type of short-lasting memory does not require protein synthesis. In contrast, long-term memory (LTM) that lasts more than several days requires protein synthesis (7). The cAMP-responsive element-binding protein (CREB) is critically involved solely in this process (811). Overexpression of a repressor isoform of CREB (dCREB2-b) in Drosophila selectively abolishes LTM formation induced by olfactory conditioning without affecting short-lasting memory (10), whereas overexpression of an activator isoform of CREB (dCREB2-a) greatly enhances LTM formation (11).

In addition to LTM formation, CREB also plays crucial roles in circadian rhythm (12, 13), the molecular mechanism of which has been studied most extensively in Drosophila (1416). Clock genes period (per) and timeless (tim) are essential for establishing circadian rhythm. A mutation in dCREB2 shortens the circadian locomotor rhythm and dampens the oscillation of PER protein level (12). Furthermore, the region 1.2–4 kbp upstream of the transcription start site of per, which includes the CREB binding sites, is required to maintain the normal expression level of PER (12). The involvement of CREB in both LTM formation and circadian rhythm, along with its role in regulation of per expression, has led us to the hypothesis that per, like CREB, is essential for LTM formation.

To determine the role of per in LTM, we studied the memory formation of Drosophila males engaged in courtship behaviors. The Drosophila male has characteristic courtship behaviors (17, 18), including orienting himself toward a female, following her movement, tapping her body with his forelegs, vibrating his wing unilaterally to generate courtship songs, licking her genitalia, and curling his abdomen in an attempt to copulate. A virgin male paired with a female that has mated earlier initially courts her but reduces his courtship activities after repeated rejections. With this experience, in addition to an exposure to courtship-inhibiting chemical cues that the fertilized female fly elicits (conditioning), the male fly reduces his courtship activities even toward a virgin female. This depression of courtship behaviors is experience-dependent (1923).

Jackson et al. (24) reported that per mutant males having a lengthened circadian rhythm (perl1 and perl2) were defective in short-lasting memory induced by courtship conditioning. It was subsequently shown, however, that perl2 males were normal in short-lasting memory and that perl1 males were defective in courtship-related chemosensory perception but not in memory formation per se (25). In any case, these previous studies have demonstrated that per-null mutations do not affect short-lasting memory (24, 25).

In this study, we tested whether circadian clock genes, including per, are involved in LTM formation and found that per is crucially involved in LTM formation but other clock genes are not. Furthermore, we found that disruption of dynamin function in per-expressing cells blocks the retrieval, but not the formation or storage, of LTM.

Materials and Methods

Flies. The four wild-type strains used in this study were Canton-S (CS), Oregon-R (OR), OGS-4, and 1619. Four memory mutants (rut178, dnc1, DC0B3, and amn28A), five circadian clock mutants [per01, perS, tim01, dClockJrk (dClkJrk), and cycle0 (cyc0)], and transgenic flies were also used. per01(+) has a per-null mutation with autosomes of unknown origin. The second and third chromosomes of per01(OR) and per01(CS) are derived from OR and CS, respectively. per-Gal4 (26) and UAS-shibirets1 (shits1; UAS, upsteam activation sequence) (27) lines were outcrossed for five or six generations to white flies with CS genetic background. All flies were raised on glucose–yeast–cornmeal medium at 24.5 ± 0.5°C in a 12-h light/12-h dark cycle (lights on at 9:00 a.m.).

Virgin males and females were collected without anesthesia within 6 h after eclosion and maintained individually in vials until experiments. All experiments, except for heat-shock treatment, were carried out during daytime at 24.5 ± 0.5°C.

Courtship Conditioning. For this study, we used two paradigms to measure memory induced by courtship conditioning. One is for LTM that lasts for days, and the other is for “1-h memory.”

Mated CS females were prepared the night before they were used for conditioning the males. For LTM, a 3-d-old virgin male was placed with a mated female (3, 4, or 5 d old) in a conditioning chamber (15 mm in diameter × 5 mm in depth) containing food for 4, 5, or 7 h, and kept in a food vial until tested 3 or 5 d later. For 1-h memory, a virgin male was conditioned for 30 min in a chamber (15 mm in diameter × 3 mm in depth) without food and tested 1 h after the end of conditioning. As a control, a naive male was introduced into another conditioning chamber without a mated female. In the test, we used an ether-immobilized or a freeze-killed virgin female as a target. We observed male courtship behaviors of individual flies and, as an indicator of male courtship activities, calculated a courtship index (CI), defined as the percentage of time spent in courtship behaviors during a given observation period. To measure the CI, we observed for 10 min or until occurrence of copulation or until initial movement of the ether-immobilized female due to recovery from anesthesia. We compared the courtship activities of a conditioned male with those of a naive male. Conditioning and test chambers were made of transparent acrylic plastic and covered with a rectangular acrylic plastic at the top.

Temperature-Shift Experiment. Three paradigms of temperature-shift were used in the experiments. (i) Males were conditioned and subsequently maintained at 25°C (permissive temperature) until 10 min before testing, at which point they were shifted to 30°C (restrictive temperature) until the end of test (see 25-25-30 in Fig. 5D). (ii) Males were conditioned and tested at 25°C and shifted to 30°C immediately after conditioning and shifted back to 25°C 10 min before testing (see 25-30-25 in Fig. 5D). (iii) Males were conditioned at 30°C and subsequently shifted to 25°C (see 30-25-25 in Fig. 5D).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Disruption of synaptic transmission in the per-expressing cells abolishes retrieval, but not formation or storage, of LTM. Open columns represent the mean CI of naive males, and filled columns represent the mean CI of conditioned males. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (A) We used per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 males (n = 30 for each column). (B) We used +/per-Gal4 males (n = 30 for each column). (C) We used +/UAS-shits1 males (n = 30 for each column). (D) Experimental paradigms of temperature-shift are indicated. The first and second arrows indicate the beginning of 7-h conditioning and 10-min test, respectively. (E) per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 males were used in the experiments: 25-25-30, disruption of synaptic transmission from per-expressing cells abolishes retrieval of LTM; 25-30-25, synaptic transmission from per-expressing cells is not required for storage of LTM; 30-25-25, synaptic transmission from per-expressing cells is not required for formation of LTM (n = 30 for each column).

Statistics. In all cases, CI values were not distributed normally. Even after arcsin transformation, the data were not normally distributed. Hence, the data were analyzed nonparametrically by using the Mann–Whitney U test or Kruskal–Wallis test.

Supporting Information for Methods. For details of the methods used, see Supporting Materials and Methods, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site.

Results and Discussion

Mutants for the cAMP Signaling Cascade Are Defective in LTM Induced by Courtship Conditioning. To assay the relationship between courtship behaviors and memory in Drosophila, we measured a CI, defined as the percentage of time that a male fly spent for courtship behaviors during a given observation period. We found that on the third and fifth days after 7-h conditioning, the CI was significantly lower in conditioned wild-type CS males compared with naive (unconditioned) males (Fig. 1A), whereas no significant difference was found after 4-h (data not shown) or 5-h (Fig. 1B) conditioning under the experimental conditions we used. The experience-dependent courtship inhibition shown in Fig. 1 A did not depend on wild-type strains. The CIs in conditioned males of three other wild-type strains (OR, OGS-4, and 1619) were significantly lower than those in naive males (Fig. 1D), although the CI values in naive males varied considerably among the four strains (Fig. 1D, open columns; Kruskal–Wallis test, P < 0.01).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Courtship conditioning in wild-type and memory mutants. In all figures, open columns and open squares represent the mean CI of naive males, and filled columns and filled squares represent the mean CI of conditioned males. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. Error bars show SEM. (A and B) Comparisons of the mean CI between naive and conditioned males of CS on the third and fifth day after 7 (A) or 5 (B) h of conditioning (n = 22–30 per point). (C) Ether-immobilized (ether) and freeze-killed (freeze) females were used in the tests. The test was carried out on the fifth day after 7-h conditioning by using CS (n = 30 for each column). (D) Comparisons of the mean CI between naive and conditioned males in four wild-type strains on the fifth day after 7-h conditioning (n = 26–30 for each column). (E) Comparisons of the mean CI between naive and conditioned males in memory mutants on the fifth day after 7-h conditioning. Because homozygous mutations of DC0B3 are lethal, we used F1 hybrid males between DC0B3/CyO males and CS females (DC0B3/+)(n = 21–26 for each column).

Because movements of female flies hinder an accurate measurement of the CI (19), we used ether-immobilized virgin females as a target of male courtship behaviors. In this situation, however, residual ether in the female body might affect experience-dependent courtship inhibition. To address this possibility, in one set of experiments we used freeze-killed females that had been kept at –80°C for 10 min and thawed. A significant difference in the CI was also detected between naive and conditioned males toward freeze-killed females (Fig. 1C). In CS naive males, the CI toward ether-immobilized virgin females was significantly lower than that toward freeze-killed females (Fig. 1C, open columns), suggesting a direct inhibitory effect of residual ether on male courtship behaviors. However, because ether anesthesia is easier to manipulate than freeze treatment, we used it routinely in this study unless otherwise stated.

In contrast to the results with wild-type strains, none of the four memory mutants (rut178, dnc1, DC0B3, and amn28A), which have defects in the cAMP cascade, displayed a significant difference in the CI between naive and conditioned males on the fifth day after 7-h conditioning (Fig. 1E). Thus, our results indicate that (i) 7-h conditioning yields memory lasting at least 5 d in wild-type flies, regardless of their genetic background (Fig. 1D), and (ii) mutations in genes that are involved in the cAMP signaling cascade block LTM (Fig. 1E). Our results were consistent with a previous report (7) that amn mutant flies are defective in LTM when assayed by olfactory conditioning. Taken together, our results suggest that the cAMP signaling cascade is involved in LTM induced by courtship conditioning.

Overexpression of a Repressor Isoform of CREB, dCREB2-b, Blocks LTM Formation. Given the involvement of the cAMP signaling cascade in LTM formation induced by courtship conditioning, we next asked whether CREB is required in its process. We used two transgenic lines, hs-dCREB2-b S30 and S64, both of which express a repressor isoform of CREB (dCREB2-b) under the control of a heat-shock promoter (refs. 10 and 28; see also Supporting Materials and Methods). When either S30 or S64 males were heat-shocked 3 h before conditioning (Fig. 2A Upper), LTM was not detected [Fig. 2 B and C, HS+(1)], whereas, without a heat shock, it was detected (Fig. 2 B and C, HS–). Timing of heat shock was critical for this suppression of LTM formation because S30 males acquired LTM (Fig. 2B) when they were heat-shocked after conditioning [Fig. 2 A Lower and B, HS+(2)]. A heat shock by itself did not affect LTM formation because CS males acquired LTM regardless of heat-shock treatment (Fig. 2D). Heat-shocked S30 or S64 naive males yielded significantly lower CIs than nonheat-shocked males (Fig. 2 B and C, open columns), suggesting that overexpression of dCREB2-b directly reduces male courtship activities toward immobilized virgins. Because the CI of heat-shocked hs-dCREB2-b naive males was lower than that of nonheat-shocked males (Fig. 2 B and C), it is possible that low courtship activities of naive males contribute to the lack of difference in the CI between naive and conditioned males that had been heat-shocked before conditioning [Fig. 2 B and C, HS+(1)]. However, LTM was detected in wild-type OGS-4 which showed a low CI (26%) (Fig. 1D). Therefore, the low CI per se is not the only cause for our failure in detecting LTM after overexpression of dCREB2-b. It is also possible that the lack of LTM in flies overexpressing dCREB2-b prior to conditioning is caused by their reduced courtship activity during the conditioning period. We found, however, that this might not be the case because overexpression of dCREB2-b did not significantly affect courtship activity toward free-moving mated females used for conditioning. [We observed courtship behaviors for 10 min at 3 h after heat-shock (CIHS– = 19.65 ± 4.08%, CIHS+ = 11.43 ± 2.86%; n = 20 in each female; Mann–Whitney U = 140.5, P = 0.108).]

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Overexpression of dCREB2-b selectively blocks LTM formation. Experimental paradigms are indicated in A and E.HS–, nonheat-shocked males; HS+(1), heat-shocked males before conditioning; HS+(2), heat-shocked males after conditioning. Open columns represent the mean CI of naive males, and filled columns represent the mean CI of conditioned males. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (BD) n = 28–34 for each column. (FI) n = 25–30 for each column. (BD, F, and I) Ether-immobilized females were used in the test. (G and H) Ether-immobilized and freeze-killed females were used in the test.

We next examined whether overexpression of dCREB2-b affects 1-h memory. S30 males were first heat-shocked then conditioned for 30 min and tested 1 h after conditioning (Fig. 2E). In this paradigm, 1-h memory was detected in CS males but not in amn28A males (Fig. 2F). Despite the fact that the CI in naive S30 males was extremely low 4.5 h after heat shock, we found that heat-shocked (Fig. 2H) as well as nonheat-shocked (Fig. 2G) S30 males showed statistical differences between naive and conditioned CIs measured 1 h after conditioning. The statistical difference was also detected when freeze-killed females that gave a higher initial CI value were used as target females. Thus, we deduce that overexpression of dCREB2-b does not significantly affect 1-h memory but blocks LTM formation.

LTM Is Defective in per Mutants. Because dCREB2 seems to be involved in LTM formation in the courtship conditioning paradigm and per is under the control of CREB in the circadian rhythm regulation, it is possible that per also plays a role in LTM formation. To test this possibility, we used three types of per-null mutants with different genetic backgrounds [per01(+), per01(OR) and per01(CS)], and a perS mutant that has a shortened circadian rhythm. As shown in Fig. 3A, LTM after 7 h of conditioning was not detected in these four per mutant males. Because this is the crucial experiment in this study, we carried out single-blind experiments (the experimenters were not informed of the strain of flies tested) to eliminate the bias of experimenters using OR (control) and per01(OR). In these single-blind experiments, LTM was detected in OR males, but not in per01(OR) males (data not shown). The LTM defect was also observed in per mutants when 7-h conditioning was performed during nighttime (Zeitgeber time, 12–19), suggesting that defective LTM of per doesn't depend on the assay timing (Fig. 6 A and B, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site). LTM was not detected in per01(CS) males even after 9-h conditioning, but was detected after 11-h conditioning (Fig. 3B), indicating that LTM is defective but not completely abolished by per mutations.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

LTM formation in mutant flies with defects in the circadian clock. Open columns and open squares represent the mean CI of naive males, and filled columns and filled squares represent the mean CI of conditioned males. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; NS, not significant. (A) LTM formation of per-null mutants [per01(+), per01(OR), and per01(CS)] and a perS mutant. The test was carried out on the third or fifth day after 7-h conditioning [n = 30–32 per point in per01(+), per01(OR), and per01(CS); n = 24 in perS]. (B) The test was carried out on the fifth day after 9- or 11-h conditioning using per01(CS) males. (C) per01;+;hsp-per(s13) males were used. Experimental paradigms are indicated above the graphs (n = 32–38 for each column). (D) per01(OR) or OR males were used in the experiments (n = 28–29 per point). The test was carried out on the second [per01(OR)] or third (OR) day after 5-h conditioning. (E) LTM formation of clock mutants (tim01, dClkJrk, and cyc0). The test was carried out on the fifth day after 7-h conditioning (n = 26–30 for each column). (F) CS males that had been kept under constant light were used (n = 30 for each column).

It has been reported that 5-h conditioning in a large chamber (15 × 75 mm) is sufficient to induce LTM in a triple mutant, yellow per01 white (23). Under the experimental condition copied from the above experiments, we found that per01(OR) did not show LTM on the second day after 5-h conditioning and that OR males acquired LTM lasting at least 3 d after 5-h conditioning (Fig. 3D and Supporting Materials and Methods). Presente et al. (29) have also reported that CS males acquires LTM lasting at least 2 days after 5-h conditioning in a similar chamber (12 × 75 mm). Taken together, our result indicates that per mutants are defective in the formation of LTM regardless of the size of conditioning chambers. Thus, the formation of LTM reported in yellow per01 white males cannot be explained simply by the difference in the size of apparatus. It is possible that yellow or white mutation somehow compensates for the per-null mutation in LTM formation.

It is known that per-null mutations do not affect short-lasting memory (24, 25). We confirmed 1-h memory formation in per01(CS) and per01(+) (Fig. 2I). Thus, mutations in per are selectively affecting LTM.

Induction of a Wild-Type per Transgene Rescues LTM Formation in the per-Null Background. Having found that per plays a role in LTM formation, we next asked whether expression of a wild-type per transgene driven by heat-shock promoter (hsp) rescues LTM formation in the per-null background. Transgenic flies, s13, (genotype, per01; +; hsp-per) show rhythmic locomotor and mating activities at high temperatures under free-running conditions (30, 31). We confirmed that after a heat shock these flies show rhythmic mating activities (data not shown). Furthermore, per01;+;hsp-per males acquired LTM when they were heat-shocked before conditioning [Fig. 3C, HS+(1)], whereas they did not without a heat shock (Fig. 3C, HS–). Nor did they acquire LTM when they were heat-shocked after conditioning [Fig. 3C, HS+(2)], indicating that per plays a critical role in formation of LTM.

It is possible that courtship activities of heat-shocked per01;+;hsp-per males toward mated females are more vigorous than those in nonheat-shocked per01;+;hsp-per males and that these vigorous courtship activities lead to acquisition of LTM. To test whether induction of the per transgene affects male courtship activities during 7-h conditioning, courtship behaviors of heat-shocked and nonheat-shocked per01;+;hsp-per males toward mated females were observed for 10 min at 0, 0.3, 1, 3, and 5 h during 7-h conditioning. No significant difference of the CI was detected during 7-h conditioning (Fig. 7A, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site). Thus, the rescue of LTM formation in heat-shocked per01;+;hsp-per males can't be simply attributed to an enhancement of courtship activities.

Overexpression of per Enhances LTM Formation in the Wild-Type Background. To determine whether overexpression of per enhances LTM formation, we conditioned for 5 h the transgenic flies, +;+;hsp-per (Fig. 4A). The 5-h conditioning was not sufficient in forming LTM in wild-type males (Figs. 1B and 4E), nor was it in nonheat-shocked +;+;hsp-per males (Fig. 4B, HS–). However, +;+;hsp-per males that were heat-shocked before conditioning did acquire LTM (Fig. 4B, HS+). Because there was no significant difference in the CI toward mated females during conditioning between nonheat-shocked and heat-shocked males (Fig. 7B), we conclude that overexpression of per enhances LTM formation in the wild-type background.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Enhancement of LTM formation induced by overexpression of per. Open columns represent the mean CI of naive males, and filled columns represent the mean CI of conditioned males. *, P < 0.05; NS, not significant. (A) An experimental paradigm. (B) +;+;hsp-per males were used (n = 30 for each column). (C) Results of an RT-PCR analysis of +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per males. The expected sizes of amplified fragments (908, 1,154, and 849 bp for tubulin, per, and dCREB2-b, respectively) were observed. +, heat-shocked flies; –, nonheat-shocked flies. (D) +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per males were used (n = 30 for each column). (E) CS males were used (n = 22 for each column for HS–; n = 30 for each column for HS+).

Simultaneous Overexpression of both dCREB2-b and per Enhances LTM Formation. dCREB2 and per participate in the same regulatory feedback loop for the normal circadian rhythm (12). To test the epistatic relationship between these two genes in LTM formation, we carried out 5-h conditioning with the transgenic flies, +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per, which have hs-dCREB2-b from S30 on the second chromosome and hsp-per from s13 on the third chromosome. First, by using RT-PCR analysis (see Supporting Materials and Methods), we confirmed overexpression of both dCREB2-b and per after heat-shock treatment (Fig. 4C). If dCREB2 acts downstream of per, we expect that a heat shock to +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per males does not enhance LTM formation, regardless of overexpression of per. In contrast, if per is epistatic to dCREB2-b, the LTM enhancement induced by per overexpression would not be affected by dCREB2-b overexpression. We found that heat-shocked +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per males acquired LTM after 5-h conditioning (Fig. 4D, HS+), whereas +;hs-dCREB2-b;hsp-per males without a heat shock did not (Fig. 4D, HS–), nor did a control [+;+;+(CS)] regardless of heat shock (Fig. 4E). These results suggest that per is operating downstream of dCREB2 in LTM formation.

Circadian Clock Activities Are Not Required for LTM Formation. PER forms a heterodimer with TIM, and the PER/TIM heterodimer is considered to be essential for generation of the circadian rhythm (1416). We found that LTM formation in tim01 was normal (Fig. 3E). Thus, TIM is not required for LTM formation. dCLK/CYC heterodimers activate transcription of per and tim by means of E-boxes located in their respective upstream sequences. PER and TIM levels are extremely low in dClkJrk and cyc0 mutants, and their behavioral rhythm is abolished (32, 33), indicating that dCLK and CYC are essential for the circadian timekeeping mechanism. In contrast, LTM formation in dClkJrk and cyc0 mutants was normal (Fig. 3E). These results suggest that per expression, which is regulated by transcriptional activators other than dCLK/CYC, is involved in LTM formation. Because it is unlikely that dCREB2 acts downstream of per (Fig. 4D), dCREB2-mediated per expression might be involved in LTM formation. Furthermore, these results indicate that per-expressing neurons, which are involved in LTM formation, are distinct from those involved in circadian rhythm.

It is known that per expression in wild-type flies dampens to arrhythmicity on the fourth day under constant light (34, 35). Hence, under this condition, the circadian clock of Drosophila is nonfunctional. We kept CS male flies under constant light after emergence and carried out 7-h conditioning on the fourth day and tested on the ninth day after emergence. LTM was detected in these males (Fig. 3F). These results indicate that the molecular mechanism of LTM formation is independent of the circadian clock.

Disruption of Dynamin Function Through per-Expressing Cells Blocks Retrieval, but Not Storage or Formation, of LTM. Mushroom bodies (MBs) play a central role for establishment of memory induced by olfactory conditioning (36, 37). Our results suggest that per-expressing cells are critically involved in LTM formation induced by courtship conditioning although the location of relevant per-expressing cells in the brain is not known. We used the GAL4/UAS system to demonstrate whether per-expressing cells affects LTM formation. The yeast transcriptional activator, GAL4, activates UAS-linked target genes. The Drosophila shi gene encodes Dynamin, which is essential for synaptic vesicle recycling. A temperature-sensitive allele, shits1, is defective in Dynamin function at restrictive temperatures (>29°C). In F1 males between per-Gal4, which expresses Gal4 in per-expressing cells (26), and UAS-shits1 (27), shits1 is expressed in per-expressing cells. In these males, Dynamin function is disrupted at restrictive temperatures in per-expressing cells.

First, all procedures in the experiments were carried out at 30°C (restrictive temperature; Fig. 5D, 30-30-30) or at 25°C (permissive temperature; Fig. 5D, 25-25-25). In these experiments, we used per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 (F1 males between per-Gal4 females and UAS-shits1 males), +/per-Gal4 (F1 males between CS females and per-Gal4 males), and +/UAS-shits1 (F1 males between CS females and UAS-shits1 males) males. We did not use F1 males between per-Gal4 females and CS males (per-Gal4/+) for the reason described in the Supporting Materials and Methods.

LTM was detected in per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 males at 25°C but not at 30°C (Fig. 5A). +/per-Gal4 and +/UAS-shits1 males acquired LTM either at 25°C or 30°C (Fig. 5 B and C). The difference of temperature (25°C versus 30°C) did not affect the CI of naive males of any transgenic line (Fig. 5 AC, open columns). Furthermore, we confirmed that disruption of Dynamin function in per-expressing cells did not affect locomotor activities (data not shown), 1-h memory (Fig. 8, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site), or the CI toward mated females during conditioning (Fig. 7C). These results indicate that disruption of Dynamin function in per-expressing cells specifically interferes with normal LTM, most likely because of the disruption of synaptic transmission.

To determine at what process of LTM formation Dynamin function in per-expressing cells is required, we shifted the temperature from permissive to restrictive during three experimental phases, namely, the period for courtship conditioning (acquisition of memory), the test period (retrieval), and the period in-between (storage) (Fig. 5D, 30-25-25, 25-25-30, and 25-30-25). When we tested at 30°C (Fig. 5D, 25-25-30), there was no significant difference in the CI between naive and conditioned per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 males (Fig. 5E, 25-25-30); i.e., LTM was not observed in these males. In this paradigm (25-25-30), we were testing the contribution of per-expressing cells to the retrieval of LTM. When the temperature during courtship conditioning or the interval between conditioning and test was shifted to 30°C, per-Gal4/UAS-shits1 males acquired, stored, and retrieved LTM (Fig. 5E, 25-30-25 and 30-25-25). Furthermore, no significant difference was detected in the CI of naive males in five sets of temperature combinations (Fig. 5E, open columns; Kruskal–Wallis test, P > 0.66), indicating that the temperature shift between 25°C and 30°C does not affect male courtship activities. Taken together, we suggest that Dynamin function, most likely synaptic transmission, in per-expressing cells is required during retrieval, but not formation or storage, of LTM.

Concluding Remarks. This study demonstrated in Drosophila that the per gene is critically involved in LTM formation induced by courtship conditioning. per mutations inhibited formation of LTM (Fig. 3A), and induction of a wild-type per–transgene preceding courtship conditioning rescued formation of LTM in the per-null background (Fig. 3C). Furthermore, overexpression of per in the wild-type background enhanced formation of LTM (Fig. 4B). These results indicate that LTM formation depends on the level of per expression. Because per mutations did not affect 1-h memory (Fig. 2I), this gene must be selectively involved in LTM formation.

We found that per expression was required for the formation of LTM (Fig. 3C), whereas synaptic transmission through per-expressing cells might be required for retrieval of LTM but not for formation or storage (Fig. 5E). Taken together, these results (Figs. 3C and 5E) indicate that per-expressing cells are critically involved both in the formation and retrieval of LTM. We still don't know which per-expressing neurons are important for LTM, but our results indicate that per-expressing neurons, which is involved in LTM formation, are distinct from pacemaker neurons of the circadian clock (Fig. 3E). MBs are known to be a critical region of the brain for retrieval of immediate or 3-h memory induced by olfactory conditioning to execute appropriate behaviors (36, 37), and LTM induced by olfactory conditioning is not detected in flies whose MBs are genetically ablated (38). In addition, McBride et al. (23) have reported that 1-h memory and LTM induced by courtship conditioning are not detected in flies whose MBs are chemically ablated. Because, apparently, per is not expressed in MBs (26), per-expressing neurons that directly or indirectly interact with MBs might be involved in formation or retrieval of LTM. Considering that LTM seems to be normal in dClkJrk and cyc0 mutants where overall per expression is greatly reduced, it is likely that particular neurons that maintain fairly normal per expression in these mutants are essential in LTM formation. One potentially important brain structure for control of LTM through PER function is the central complex (CX). The CX is known to be involved in learning and memory in Drosophila (3941), and per is expressed in a part of the CX. Furthermore, we confirmed that overexpression of per in the fan-shaped body within the CX enhanced LTM formation (T.S., T.K., and Y.K., unpublished data), suggesting that the CX is involved in formation of LTM induced by courtship conditioning. per expression in some CX neurons may not be affected by dClkJrk and cyc0 mutations. For example, perGal4-directed GFP expression in neurons sending fibers from the large neuronal clusters that are relatively ventral within laterally located neuronal clusters in the posterior optic tract seems unaffected by either dClkJrk or cyc0 (26). Such neurons are not likely to be involved in regulating circadian rhythm but may play a critical role in LTM formation.

In species ranging from invertebrates, such as Aplysia or Drosophila, to mammals, CREB has been shown to be involved in LTM formation (1, 811). Our findings reinforce this connection but also separate the formation of LTM from the circadian clock. Nevertheless, because the molecular mechanism of the mammalian circadian clock is similar to that in Drosophila (1416), it is intriguing to know whether per plays a key role in the formation of LTM in mammals.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. M. Saitoe (Tokyo Metropolitan Institute for Neuroscience, Fuchu, Japan) for providing hs-dCREB2-b and amn28A and critically reading the manuscript; Drs. J. C. Hall (Brandeis University, Waltham, MA), N. Ishida (National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Tsukuba, Japan), and A. Matsumoto (Kyushu University, Ropponmatsu, Japan) for the circadian rhythm mutants, s13, and per-Gal4; and Dr. Y. Oguma (University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Japan) for wild-type stocks; Drs. H. Kuromi, K. Ueno, and R. Davis for useful comments; and Mrs. M. Takahashi for technical assistance. This work was supported by a grant-in-aid from the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports, and Culture of Japan (to T.S. and Y.K.); a grant from the Uehara Memorial Foundation (to T.S.); and National Institutes of Health Grant MH62684 (to T.K.).

Author contributions: T.S. and Y.K. designed research; T.S. and T.T. performed research; T.S. and T.K. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; T.S. analyzed data; and T.S., T.K., and Y.K. wrote the paper.

This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.

Abbreviations: LTM, long-term memory; per, period; tim, timeless; rut, rutabaga; dnc, dunce; amn, amnesiac; shi, shibire; dClk, dClock; cyc, cycle; hsp, heat-shock promoter; CREB, cAMP-responsive element-binding protein; CI, courtship index; MB, mushroom body; UAS, upstream activation sequence; CX, central complex; CS, Canton-S; OR, Oregon-R.

References


Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES