Skip to main content
BMC Medical Genetics logoLink to BMC Medical Genetics
. 2017 Feb 3;18:12. doi: 10.1186/s12881-017-0374-y

Microsatellite polymorphisms associated with human behavioural and psychological phenotypes including a gene-environment interaction

Andrew T M Bagshaw 1,, L John Horwood 2, David M Fergusson 2, Neil J Gemmell 3,4, Martin A Kennedy 1
PMCID: PMC5291968  PMID: 28158988

Abstract

Background

The genetic and environmental influences on human personality and behaviour are a complex matter of ongoing debate. Accumulating evidence indicates that short tandem repeats (STRs) in regulatory regions are good candidates to explain heritability not accessed by genome-wide association studies.

Methods

We tested for associations between the genotypes of four selected repeats and 18 traits relating to personality, behaviour, cognitive ability and mental health in a well-studied longitudinal birth cohort (n = 458-589) using one way analysis of variance. The repeats were a highly conserved poly-AC microsatellite in the upstream promoter region of the T-box brain 1 (TBR1) gene and three previously studied STRs in the activating enhancer-binding protein 2-beta (AP2-β) and androgen receptor (AR) genes. Where significance was found we used multiple regression to assess the influence of confounding factors.

Results

Carriers of the shorter, most common, allele of the AR gene’s GGN microsatellite polymorphism had fewer anxiety-related symptoms, which was consistent with previous studies, but in our study this was not significant following Bonferroni correction. No associations with two repeats in the AP2-β gene withstood this correction. A novel finding was that carriers of the minor allele of the TBR1 AC microsatellite were at higher risk of conduct problems in childhood at age 7-9 (p = 0.0007, which did pass Bonferroni correction). Including maternal smoking during pregnancy (MSDP) in models controlling for potentially confounding influences showed that an interaction between TBR1 genotype and MSDP was a significant predictor of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence (p < 0.001), and of self-reported criminal behaviour up to age 25 years (p ≤ 0.02). This interaction remained significant after controlling for possible confounders including maternal age at birth, socio-economic status and education, and offspring birth weight.

Conclusions

The potential functional importance of the TBR1 gene’s promoter microsatellite deserves further investigation. Our results suggest that it participates in a gene-environment interaction with MDSP and antisocial behaviour. However, previous evidence that mothers who smoke during pregnancy carry genes for antisocial behaviour suggests that epistasis may influence the interaction.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s12881-017-0374-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: TBR1, Androgen receptor, AP2-β, Microsatellite, STR, Antisocial, Behaviour, Pregnancy, Smoking, Gene-environment interaction

Background

Twin studies indicate that personality and behavioural traits including antisocial behaviour have a strong genetic component [14]. However, similarly to many other complex traits, the sources of their heritability have not been uncovered to a substantial degree by genome-wide association studies (GWAS) [3, 59]. Accumulating evidence suggests that heritability not so far accessed by GWAS resides partly in under-studied forms of genetic variation including length polymorphism in arrays of short tandem repeat (STR) sequences, otherwise known as microsatellites [1013]. This is not adequately represented in GWAS because the frequency and diversity of microsatellite polymorphism are much higher than those of single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) [11, 14].

Many species including bacteria and yeasts are known to have harnessed the high mutability of microsatellites for regulatory purposes, and there is evidence that microsatellite functionality is far more widespread in the human genome than has traditionally been appreciated [1113, 1518]. For example a recent study of expression quantitative trait loci estimated that at least 10–15% of the heritability in human gene expression levels attributable to common variants in cis is due to microsatellite polymorphism [12]. Candidate-gene association studies for a growing number of human regulatory microsatellites have been well replicated [1924], and mechanisms by which microsatellites function, including modification of spacing between adjacent transcription factors and other promoter elements [2527], regulation of splicing [22, 28, 29], adoption of structural variants such as Z-DNA [3032], and modification of epigenetic signals [12] are increasingly well understood. Despite this body of evidence, examples of phenotypic associations with microsatellites remain isolated. This is mainly due to difficulties genotyping large numbers of loci, and while methods have recently been developed that will facilitate microsatellite-based association studies comparable in scale to contemporary SNP-based GWAS, these still require extensive sequencing and/or capture probe synthesis [11, 33].

Previous work by our group has attempted to identify likely functional microsatellites by investigating their conservation among mammalian species [34, 35], and links between microsatellites and several neurological disorders and behavioural phenotypes suggest that they are particularly likely to be functionally important when associated with brain-related genes [13, 36]. These considerations motivated us to investigate associations between human behavioural traits and a previously unstudied AC12-13 microsatellite in the upstream promoter region of the T-Box Brain 1 (TBR1) gene, which is in the top 2.5% of human microsatellites by level of conservation [34]. TBR1 is a member of the set of regulatory genes involved in the genesis and fate determination of glutamatergic neurons [3739]. As a transcription factor it regulates as many as 124 other genes [40, 41] including GRIN2B, which has been implicated in attention deficits in children [42], cognitive performance [43], neuroticism [44] and smoking [45] among other psychiatric and behavioural phenotypes [46]. TBR1 haploinsufficiency in mice results in defective axonal projections of amygdalar neurons and the impairment of social interaction, ultrasonic vocalization, associative memory and cognitive flexibility [40]. In humans, deletion of chromosomal regions including TBR1 is associated with intellectual disability [47], and mutations of the gene have been linked to autism spectrum disorder [48, 49]. TBR1 was also recently distinguished as the highest scoring locus associated with educational attainment in a study of more than 300,000 individuals [50].

We also sought to replicate and/or expand on previous studies showing associations between psychological and/or behavioural phenotypes and three other tandem repeats. The GGN microsatellite in exon 1 of the androgen receptor (AR) gene encodes a poly-glycine stretch associated with receptor responsiveness [51, 52]. It has been less studied than the CAG microsatellite in the same exon, which is associated with diseases including spinobulbar muscular atrophy [53], but its common length variants have been linked to externalizing behaviours including conduct disorder [54] and the personality traits self-transcendence [24, 55], aggression and impulsivity [56]. Two intronic repeats in the AP-2β transcription factor gene have also been studied relatively little. A minisatellite in its first intron has been associated with the impulsivity-linked phenotypes alcohol dependence [57] and late auditory-evoked potentials [58]. A CAAA microsatellite in the second intron of the same gene has been linked to reduced MAOB activity, which is also related to impulsivity [59], late auditory-evoked potentials [58], anxiety-related measures [60] and the personality traits self-transcendence and spiritual acceptance [61]. The AP-2β transcription factor functions in neural development and influences brain monoaminergic systems by regulating target genes [62, 63].

The initial aim of this study was to assess the effects on human traits relating to personality, behaviour, cognitive ability and mental health of our selected repeat polymorphisms. The most significant associations we found were between the TBR1 microsatellite and antisocial behaviour. Following this, recent interest by two of our authors (LJH and DMF) in maternal smoking during pregnancy (MSDP) and its effects on offspring behaviour [64, 65], led us, in conjunction with evidence showing expression of TBR1 in the human embryonic neocortex [37], and linking the gene to responses to prenatal cannabis exposure [66], and maternal illness [67], to the additional hypothesis that MSDP may modify the effects of TBR1 microsatellite genotype.

Our test subjects were from a well-studied birth cohort, the Christchurch Health and Development Study (CHDS). The CHDS is a longitudinal study of 1265 children born in Christchurch, New Zealand who have been studied on 22 occasions from birth to age 30 [68, 69], 570 of whom were included in the work presented here. As part of this study data were gathered on: a) MSDP; b) measures of antisocial behaviour assessed from early childhood to mature adulthood by a wide range of methods; c) potentially confounding social and contextual factors.

Methods

Sample

The sample group was from the CHDS birth cohort of 1265 children born in the Christchurch (New Zealand) urban region in mid-1977 who have been studied at birth, 4 months, 1 year, annually to age 16 years, and again at ages 18, 21, 25 and 30 years [68, 69]. At age 28–30 surviving cohort members were approached to provide a blood sample for genetic analysis. Those unwilling or unable to provide a blood sample were asked for a saliva sample instead. Of those approached 916 (91%) agreed to provide a DNA sample (90% bloods, 10% saliva), and 679 (74%), 537 (59%), 702 (77%) and 684 (75%) were successfully genotyped for TBR1, AR, AP2-β AAAC and AP2-β minisatellite respectively. The remainder could not be genotyped despite repeated attempts, suggesting DNA sample degradation. Since preliminary analysis suggested the presence of ethnic stratification with respect to the distribution of both TBR1 and AP2-β CAAA genotypes, the primary analysis was restricted to those of sole European ancestry defined on the basis of parental reports of ethnic ancestry; the non-European sample comprised 14.1% of the cohort. The final analysis samples with data on both genotype and at least one outcome were respectively 570, 459, 589 and 571 for the four genes. Actual sample sizes vary slightly from these Ns depending on the pattern of missing data for each outcome. The process of DNA collection and preparation has been described elsewhere [70].

Genotyping

PCR primers and genomic locations of each of the four repeats are listed in Table 1. All forward primers were 5′-labelled with compatible fluorophores (Applied Biosystems Foster City, CA, USA). Polymerase chain reaction conditions were as follows: initial 2-min denaturing step at 95 °C, followed by 35 cycles of 94 °C for 45 s, 60 °C for 45 s and 72 °C for 45 s, and a final extension phase of 72 °C for 5 min. Reactions were performed in 10 μl volumes using PCR buffer with 1.75 mM MgCl2, ∼50 ng of genomic DNA, 500 nM of each primer, 200 μM of each dNTP and 0.5 units of Taq polymerase (Fisher Biotech Wembley, WA, Australia). Polymerase chain reaction products were assayed on an Applied Biosystems 3130xl genetic analyser, using GeneScan 500 LIZ (Applied Biosystems) as size standard. Results were analysed with GeneMapper v 4.0 software for Windows (Applied Biosystems). Analyses were restricted to homozygous major allele carriers vs other genotypes (explained below), and the copy numbers and frequencies of the most common alleles are shown in Table 1. All of these alleles were confirmed by sequencing five homozygotes of each genotype using an Applied Biosystems 3130xl genetic analyser (data not shown).

Table 1.

Location, repeat motif, copy number, frequency of most common alleles, and PCR primers for each of the four repeats studied

Gene Motif Copies (freq) GRCh38 coordinates Primers (fwd/rev)
TBR1 AC 12 (0.82), 13 (0.18) chr2:161,415,158-161,415,183 GCTTTTTCTCCCCTCCAGAT
GGGAGACAGAGTGGAAGTGG
AR GGN 23 (0.59), 24 (0.41) chrX:67,546,497-67,546,572 CTCTTCACAGCCGAAGAAGG
AGGCGACATTTCTGGAAGGA
AP-2β GCGGACGAG 9 (0.22), 10 (0.61) chr6:50,819,803-50,819,893 CTCCTGTGTGGGCATCTTTC
CCAGACCATTCCGCTTAAAAG
AP-2β CAAA 4 (0.4), 5 (0.59) chr6:50,823,873-50,823,896 GGCAGCGGCAAGAAGTGGGT
AGACAGAGCTGCCCTCTCCTG

Phenotyping

An initial series of 18 measures was selected from the study database to examine for associations with the repeat variants. These measures spanned the following domains: child behaviour (conduct problems, attentional problems, anxiety/withdrawal); cognitive ability (child IQ, scholastic ability); adolescent personality (neuroticism, extroversion, novelty seeking) and self-esteem; and mental health symptoms in adolescence (somatisation, anxiety, phobic anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, depression, interpersonal sensitivity, psychoticism, paranoid ideation, hostility). A detailed description of these measures is provided in the Additional file 1. However, the principal results reported in this paper relate to exploration of a specific association between TBR1 and childhood conduct problems at age 7–9, and the identification of a possible gene by environment interaction between TBR1, MSDP and the development of broader antisocial behaviours. The specific measures used in this component of the analysis are described in detail below.

Measures of antisocial behaviour

Four measures of antisocial behaviour were selected from the database of the study to span the period from childhood to young adulthood.

Childhood conduct problems (7–9 years)

When sample members were aged 7, 8 and 9 years parental and teacher reports of the child’s tendencies to disruptive, oppositional and conduct disordered behaviours were obtained using an instrument that combined items from the Rutter [71] and Conners [72] parent and teacher questionnaires. The selected items spanned a range of behaviours relating to disobedience and defiance of authority, fits of temper and irritability, aggression or cruelty towards others, destruction of property, lying, stealing and other similar behaviours. Confirmatory factor analysis of the selected items for each source (parents, teachers) suggested that, in each case, the items could be scaled as unidimensional scales representing the extent of child conduct problems as reported by parents and teachers [73]. Scale scores representing the extent of disruptive, oppositional or conduct disordered behaviour at age 7, 8 and 9 years were created by summing parental and teacher item scores for each child at each age. These scales were then averaged over the 3-year period to provide an overall measure of the extent of conduct problems in middle childhood. The reliability of this scale, assessed using coefficient alpha, was 0.97. To simplify presentation in the present analysis the scale scores have been standardised to a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 10.

Adolescent conduct problems (15–16 years)

When participants were aged 15 and 16 years parental and self-report measures of the child’s tendencies to oppositional and conduct disordered behaviours were obtained. Parental reports were based on a selected series of items from the Revised Behaviour Problems Checklist [74]. Children were asked a series of questions relating to conduct/oppositional defiant behaviors derived from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children [75]. In a similar fashion to the behavior reports in middle childhood, a total symptom score was constructed for each child at each age by summing the parent and self-report items. These symptom scores were then averaged over the 2 years to provide an overall measure of the child’s tendencies to conduct disordered and oppositional behaviours in adolescence. The reliability of this scale, assessed using coefficient alpha, was 0.87. In the present analysis the scale scores have been standardized to a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 10.

Self-reported property/violent offences (14–25 years)

At each assessment from age 15 to 25 years participants were questioned about their involvement in criminal offending since the previous assessment. At ages 15 and 16 questioning was conducted using the Self-Report Early Delinquency Scale [76]. At ages 18, 21 and 25 questioning was based on the Self-Report Delinquency Inventory [SDRI; 45], supplemented by additional custom-written survey items. This information was used to derive count measures of the number of self-reported property and/or violent offences committed in each year from age 15 to age 25. Property offences were defined to include theft, burglary, breaking and entering, vandalism, fire setting, and related offences; violent offences included assault, fighting, use of a weapon, threats of violence against a person, cruelty to animals and related offences. For the purposes of the present analyses, the number of offences committed in each year was summed over the period 15–25 years to create an overall score reflecting the total number of property or violent offences reported over the period from adolescence to young adulthood. To avoid problems with extreme outliers, the number of reported offences was truncated to a maximum of 500.

Self-reported arrests/convictions (16–25 years)

At ages 18, 21 and 25 years participants were questioned about their contacts with the police and criminal justice system for each 12 month period since the previous assessment. This information included details of any arrests or court convictions received in each interval. To provide an overall measure of the level of involvement with the criminal justice system in young adulthood, these report data were used to construct a count of the total number of arrests/convictions reported over the interval from age 16–25 years.

Maternal smoking during pregnancy

At the time of the survey child’s birth, mothers were questioned about cigarette smoking during pregnancy, and specifically how much they smoked (cigs/day) in each trimester. These reports were averaged over the three trimesters to provide a measure of the average number of cigarettes smoked per day during pregnancy. In the analysis below this variable is used in two ways: (a) to define a dichotomous (smoker/non-smoker) measure of smoking during pregnancy for use in the main analysis; and (b) to classify mothers on an ordinal measure reflecting the extent of smoking during pregnancy (non-smoker, 1–9 cigs/day, 10+ cigs/day) for use in the supplementary analysis.

Confounding Factors

To adjust the observed associations between MSDP, TBR1 and behaviour outcomes for possible confounding by social, family and related factors correlated with MSDP or TBR1 genotype the following measures were included in the analysis: maternal age at the birth of the child; maternal education at the time of the birth classified in three levels (no formal qualifications, high school qualifications, tertiary qualifications); family SES at the time of the birth classified using the 6-level Elley-Irving scale for New Zealand occupations [77]; the type of family (single parent/two parent) the child entered at birth; a measure of family income based on the average of the family’s gross annual income in each of the first 5 years of the study; a measure of family living standards based on an average of interviewer ratings of the quality of family living standards assessed over the first 5 years of the study; the child’s birthweight.

Statistical methods

Allele grouping

Preliminary examination of the distribution of TBR1 genotype showed that only 5 participants were homozygous for the minor allele (13-copy repeat). Given the very small number of homozygotes, all carriers of the TBR1 minor allele were combined into a single group for analysis purposes, referred to as ‘other’. We also used a division of homozygous major allele vs other genotypes for the other three repeats, which replicated published studies on the AP2-β loci and was similar to previous work on the AR microsatellite, which categorized alleles as “long” vs “short” [24, 58, 60].

Associations with microsatellite variants

Associations between microsatellite variants (classified as homozygous major allele vs other) and the measures of child behaviour, cognitive ability, personality/self-esteem and mental health were tested for statistical significance using one way analysis of variance. The strength of each association was summarised by the point biserial correlation between the microsatellite and the outcome. Two way ANOVA models with sex and microsatellite as factors were used to test for sex by microsatellite interactions. A Bonferroni correction was applied to adjust for multiple comparisons. The Bonferroni corrected p-value calculated to take into account the average correlations between the 18 outcome measures (r = 0.23) across the four microsatellite variants was p = 0.0019.

Modelling the TBR1 by MSDP interaction

The joint associations between TBR1 genotype (homozygous major allele/other) MSDP (no/yes) and antisocial behaviour outcomes (Table 2) were modelled by fitting a series of saturated regression models in which each behavioural outcome was modelled as a function of TBR1 genotype, MSDP and the multiplicative interaction of TBR1 and MSDP. For the measures of childhood and adolescent conduct problems multiple linear regression models were fitted; for the count measures of violent/property offences and arrests/convictions, Poisson regression models were fitted with a deviance correction for over-dispersion. These models were then extended to control for potential confounding by social, family and related factors (Table 3). To control for the possibility of Type 1 errors due to multiple significance testing both sets of analyses were supplemented by fitting a multivariate regression model to conduct simultaneous tests of significance of the effects of TBR1, MSDP and their interaction. Analyses were also extended to examine the effect of smoking frequency during pregnancy (0, 1–9, 10+ cigs/day) to test for multiplicative interactions with gender and to examine the possible implications of ethnic stratification.

Table 2.

Associations between microsatellite variants and measures of child behaviour, cognitive ability, personality/self-esteem and mental health (European sample only). The AP-2β minisatellite showed no significant association so was excluded from the table

AR-GGN homozygous major vs rest AP-2β-CAAA homozygous major vs rest TBR1
homozygous major vs rest
Measure r p r p r p
Childhood behaviour (7–9 years) (N = 454–459) (N = 582–589) (N = 563–570)
Conduct problems 0.056 0.23 −0.055 0.18 0.141 0.0007**
Attention problems 0.049 0.29 −0.061 0.14 0.045 0.29
Anxiety/withdrawal 0.019 0.68 −0.039 0.34 0.103 0.015*
Cognitive abilitya (N = 348–371) (N = 451–482) (N = 442–468)
WISC-R IQ (8–9 years) −0.104 0.046* 0.085 0.062 −0.015 0.75
Scholastic ability (13 years) −0.083 0.12 0.136 0.004* −0.042 0.38
Personality/self-esteem (N = 428–433) (N = 554–557) (N = 542–544)
EPI neuroticism (14 years) 0.039 0.41 −0.006 0.88 0.068 0.11
EPI extroversion (14 years) −0.040 0.41 −0.072 0.08 0.061 0.16
TPI novelty seeking (16 years) 0.017 0.72 −0.045 0.29 0.034 0.43
Self-esteem (15 years) −0.077 0.11 0.011 0.79 −0.051 0.23
Mental health (SCL-90, 18 years) (N = 453) (N = 588) (N = 568)
Somatisation 0.060 0.15 −0.024 0.56 0.011 0.80
Anxiety 0.096 0.040* 0.020 0.63 0.022 0.60
Phobic anxiety 0.078 0.10 0.036 0.38 −0.022 0.61
Obsessive compulsive 0.037 0.44 −0.042 0.31 0.036 0.39
Depression 0.037 0.43 −0.009 0.83 0.037 0.38
Interpersonal sensitivity 0.060 0.21 0.012 0.78 0.054 0.20
Psychoticism 0.061 0.19 −0.110 0.008* 0.001 0.97
Paranoid ideation 0.081 0.086 −0.064 0.12 0.046 0.28
Hostility −0.024 0.60 −0.028 0.49 0.087 0.039*

aCognitive outcomes assessed only for cohort members resident in Christchurch urban region (approximately 80% of total cohort at each age)

*Nominally significant (p < 0.05) but didn't withstand Bonferroni correction

**Significant following Bonferroni correction

Table 3.

Associations between TBR1 genotype, MSDP and measures of offspring antisocial behaviour

TBR1 genotype MSDP
No Yes
(a) Childhood conduct problems (7–9 years)
Homozygous major allele Mean (SD) 98.5 (8.4) 99.4 (8.6)
N 312 140
Other Mean (SD) 99.2 (9.0) 107.6 (13.2)
N 79 39
Fitted regression model: main effect TBR1 (B = 0.71, SE = 1.12, p = 0.53); main effect MSDP (B = 0.93, SE = 0.91, p = 0.31); TBR1 x MSDP interaction (B = 7.51, SE = 1.97, p < 0.001)
(b) Adolescent conduct problems (15–16 years)
Homozygous major allele Mean (SD) 98.6 (8.8) 100.7 (10.5)
N 300 136
Other Mean (SD) 97.8 (6.8) 107.5 (14.1)
N 75 39
Fitted regression model: main effect TBR1 (B = -0.85, SE = 1.22, p = 0.49); main effect MSDP (B = 2.04, SE = 0.98, p = 0.04); TBR1 x MSDP interaction (B = 7.70, SE = 2.11, p < 0.001)
(c) Self-reported property/violent offences (14–25 years)
Homozygous major allele Mean (SD) 9.2 (35.0) 15.1 (52.6)
N 303 139
Other Mean (SD) 5.8 (15.3) 26.9 (83.6)
N 78 36
Fitted regression model: main effect TBR1 (B =−0.46, SE = 0.34, p = 0.18); main effect MSDP (B = 0.50, SE = 0.20, p = 0.01); TBR1 x MSDP interaction (B = 1.03, SE = 0.43, p = 0.02)
(d) Self-reported arrests or convictions (16–25 years)
Homozygous major allele Mean (SD) 0.6 (2.1) 1.3 (4.5)
N 303 139
Other Mean (SD) 0.3 (0.8) 2.0 (5.4)
N 78 36
Fitted regression model: main effect TBR1 (B = −0.83, SE = 0.44, p = 0.06); main effect MSDP (B = 0.78, SE = 0.20, p < 0.001); TBR1 x MSDP interaction (B = 1.27, SE = 0.51, p = 0.01)

Supplementary analyses

To address the issue that the study findings may have been influenced by selection bias attributable to the processes of sample loss and failure of genotyping, a data weighting strategy was used in which the data were first stratified by socio-demographic characteristics at birth to estimate the probability of inclusion in the analysis sample for each association. Typically this showed the presence of modest but statistically significant (p < 0.05) tendencies for some analysis samples to under-represent participants from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds (low parental education, low SES families, single parent families). All data were then re-analysed with each participant weighted by the inverse probability of sample selection. The results for the weighted analyses were negligibly different from results for the unweighted analyses reported in this paper, suggesting that the study findings were unlikely to be affected by selection bias. Finally, the robustness of study findings was checked against re-analysis on the full sample including non-European participants.

Results

Association analyses

We examined associations between four short tandem repeats in the TBR1, AR and AP-2β genes (Table 1) and a series of 18 measures of personal characteristics, temperament and behaviours in the CHDS cohort (Table 2). The GGN microsatellite in exon 1 of the AR gene showed modest nominally significant associations between a dichotomous (homozygous major allele GGN23 carried by 70% of individuals vs rest) measure and two outcomes: child IQ (r = −0.104, p = 0.046) and anxiety symptoms (r = 0.096, p = 0.040). Those homozygous for the major allele scored higher on IQ and lower on anxiety symptoms. Neither association withstood correction for multiple comparisons. There was no apparent association with neuroticism or extroversion. The CAAA4-5 repeat polymorphism in intron 2 of the AP-2β gene showed nominally significant correlations with two measures: child scholastic ability (r = 0.136, p = 0.004) and a measure of psychotic symptoms (r = -0.110, p = 0.008). Carriers of the minor allele scored higher on scholastic ability at age 13 and lower on psychotic symptoms at age 18. We found no clear associations for the minisatellite in intron 1 of the AP-2β gene. The AC12-13 microsatellite in the promoter region of the TBR1 gene was associated with three outcomes. The 22% of individuals who carried the minor allele (AC13), scored higher on the measures of childhood conduct problems (r = 0.141, p = 0.0007), childhood anxiety/withdrawal (r = 0.102, p = 0.015), and hostility symptoms in adolescence (r = 0.087, p = 0.039). Only the association with childhood conduct problems withstood correction for multiple comparisons.

Reanalysis of the data including those of non-European ancestry identified a small number of additional nominally significant (p < 0.05) associations between the GGN microsatellite and the mental health measures of somatisation, phobic anxiety and psychoticism (r = 0.088–0.094), indicative of lower scores on these measures for those who were homozygous for the major allele. The lowest P value for these tests was 0.006, for an association between the homozygous major allele genotype and lower levels of anxiety, and there was nothing significant following Bonferroni correction. Otherwise essentially the same pattern of findings applied to the full sample.

Tests of sex by genotype interaction identified four nominally significant interactions: for AR GGN on childhood conduct problems (p = 0.013); and for AP2-β CAAA on childhood conduct problems (p = 0.043), childhood attention problems (p = 0.035) and interpersonal sensitivity in adolescence (p = 0.035). The associations with the measures of childhood behaviour problems were stronger for males, and the association with interpersonal sensitivity stronger for females. However, none of these interactions withstood Bonferroni correction. No sex interactions were observed for the minisatellite in intron 1 of the AP-2β gene or for the TBR1 microsatellite.

The most significant association we found overall was for the TBR1 microsatellite minor allele (AC13), carried by 22% of the sample, with childhood conduct problems at age 7–9. This was statistically significant after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (r = 0.141, p = 0.0007), and we performed no further analyses on the other three repeats. For the reasons outlined above we examined one additional hypothesis: that there may be a joint effect of TBR1 and MSDP on childhood conduct problems, and we discovered an interaction. We then replicated the analysis by widening the outcomes to other measures of antisocial behaviour. These included measures of adolescent conduct problems (15–16 years), self-reported property/violent offences (14–25 years) and self-reported arrests or convictions (16–25 years).

Associations between smoking during pregnancy, TBR1 and subsequent antisocial behaviour

Table 3 shows the sample cross-classified according to history of MSDP (no/yes) and TBR1 microsatellite genotype (homozygous major allele/other). For each classification the table reports the mean scores on a series of dependent variables representing measures of antisocial behaviour over the life course. The table also reports the results of regression models fitted to the data for each outcome including tests of significance of: a) the main effect of TBR1; b) the main effect of MSDP; c) the TBR1 by MSDP interaction. The table shows that:

  1. In three out of four cases there was a significant (p < 0.05) main effect of MSDP, reflecting a pervasive association between MSDP and higher rates of subsequent anti-social behaviour assessed up to the age of 25.

  2. There were no significant main effects for TBR1 reflecting the fact that in the absence of MSDP overall rates of antisocial behaviour did not vary with TBR1.

  3. In all analyses there was a significant (p < 0.05) MSDP by TBR1 interaction, reflecting the fact that the effects of MSDP on antisocial behaviour were more marked for the ‘other’ strata who carried at least one TBR1 minor allele than for the TBR1 homozygous major allele group.

As noted in Methods, the analysis in Table 3 is based on a classification of TBR1 genotype in which all carriers of the TBR1 minor allele have been classified into a single group. This was done because of the very small number (n = 5) who were homozygous for the minor allele. At the same time it is of interest to note that elaboration of the data in Table 3 according to the number of TBR1 minor alleles showed a pattern of data for three of the four outcomes consistent with an increasing effect of MSDP on antisocial behaviour with an increasing number of TBR1 minor alleles (see Table S1 in Additional file 1).

A limitation of the analysis in Table 3 is that it reports multiple tests of significance, thus increasing risks of type 1 statistical errors. To address this issue the data in the table were re-analysed using a multivariate regression modelling approach to test the joint significance of effects across all outcomes (see Methods). This analysis confirmed the presence of a just significant main effect of MSDP (F(4,520) = 2.38, p = 0.05), the absence of a significant main effect for TBR1 (F(4,520) = 0.57, p = 0.59) and a significant MSDP x TBR1 interaction (F(4,520) = 3.95, p < 0.005).

Adjustment for confounding factors

To examine the possible effects of confounding social, economic and related factors, the analyses in Table 3 were extended to include a range of covariate factors correlated with MSDP. These factors included measures of: maternal age, maternal education, family socio-economic status, family income, family living standards, family type (single parent/two parent) and birthweight. Table 4 reports tests of the main effects and the TBR1 by MSDP interaction after covariate adjustment. This table shows that in all cases the significant TBR1 x MSDP interaction persisted. A multivariate regression model also confirmed the presence of a significant TBR1 x MSDP interaction (F(4,501) = 3.3, p = 0.01). However, the overall main effect of MSDP was no longer significant after covariate adjustment (F(4,501) = 1.64, p = 0.16).

Table 4.

Estimated regression coefficients for the effects of TBR1 genotype, MSDPand TBR1 by MSDP interaction after adjustment for confounding

TBR1 genotype MSDP TBR1 by MSDP interaction
Outcome B(SE) p B(SE) p B(SE) P
Conduct problems (7–9 years) 0.84 (1.07) 0.43 −0.26 (0.88) 0.77 6.47 (1.86) <0.001
Conduct problems (15–16 years) −0.61 (1.19) 0.61 0.83 (0.97) 0.40 6.23 (2.04) < 0.005
Property/violent offences (14–25 years) −0.91 (0.47) 0.05 0.65 (0.21) < 0.005 1.24 (0.54) 0.02
Arrests/convictions (16–25 years) −0.32 (0.42) 0.45 0.76 (0.24) < 0.005 2.21 (0.49) < 0.001

The effect of frequency of smoking

To take account of variations in MSDP the data were re-analysed using an ordered categorical measure of MSDP: non-smoker; 1–9 cigarettes per day; 10+ cigarettes per day. This analysis produced the same overall pattern of findings as those reported in Tables 3 and 4. After covariate adjustment the multivariate regression model again showed a significant main effect for MSDP (F(4,501) = 2.51, p = 0.04) and a significant TBR1 by MSDP interaction (F(4,501) = 2.85, p = 0.02).

The effect of gender

To examine the extent to which the findings may be moderated by gender, the analyses were extended to include gender as a further factor and tests of gender x TBR1 x MSDP interaction were conducted. No significant three-way interactions were found, suggesting that the TBR1 x MSDP interaction was evident for both males and females.

The effect of ethnic stratification

To examine the implications of ethnic stratification, the data were re-analysed including an additional 91 participants with data on TBR1, MSDP and behaviour outcomes who were of non-European (Maori or Pacific Island, by self-definition) ethnic origin. The fitted regression models were extended to incorporate ethnicity as a factor and to test for ethnicity by TBR1 by MSDP interactions. While the analysis lacked statistical power to draw strong conclusions, for all outcomes the TBR1 by MSDP interaction appeared to be somewhat weaker for non-European sample members. This was reflected in the multivariate regression model which showed that, after covariate adjustment, there was a significant ethnicity by TBR1 by MSDP interaction (F(4,577) = 2.62, p = 0.034), in addition to the existing TBR1 by MSDP interaction (F(4,577) = 3.03, p = 0.017).

Collectively the results in Tables 3 and 4 and the additional analyses reported above confirm the presence of a robust and persistent TBR1 by MSDP interaction in which MSDP had greater effects on the risks of subsequent antisocial behaviours for those with the “other” genotype who carried at least one copy of the TBR1 minor allele when compared with those who were homozygous for the major allele. These findings held after: a) control for covariate factors; b) variation in the measurement of MSDP; c) tests of gender interaction. This interaction appeared to be stronger amongst those of European ancestry.

Linkage disequilibrium with nearby single nucleotide polymorphisms

Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in our cohort had been genotyped as part of a genome wide association study [78]. We examined all SNPs within 150 k bases (kb) of TBR1 to see if any of them could explain the effects of the microsatellite. Three SNPs located 37-64 kb upstream of the gene, rs3769963, rs1116173 and rs6727917, are in at least partial linkage disequilibrium with the microsatellite polymorphism. We analysed these SNPs individually and jointly with microsatellite genotype. Individually all three SNPs showed evidence of a gene x MSDP interaction on childhood conduct problems (data not shown). Of the three, rs3769963 showed the strongest evidence of interaction and produced results that were closest to those for the microsatellite. However, in all cases the R squared values for these models (0.032-0.042) were substantially less than the R-squared value for the microsatellite model (0.058). Also, rs3769963 is located 65 kb upstream of the TBR1 gene’s transcriptional start site, and while this is not inconsistent with a regulatory role, its level of LD with the promoter microsatellite polymorphism was 2.6 × higher than that of any other SNP within 150 kb of TBR1. In contrast, the levels of LD between the microsatellite and its neighbouring SNPs were very low (R-squared < 0.0005 and D’ < 0.2).

Discussion

Our main finding was that the minor allele of the AC12-13 microsatellite in the upstream promoter region of the TBR1 gene interacts with MSDP to increase risk of antisocial behaviour, a putative gene by environment (G x E) interaction which remained significant after adjustment for potentially confounding social and contextual factors, gender and frequency of smoking. We found some evidence that the interaction may be stronger in individuals of European descent, but the number of non-Europeans in our cohort was too small to rule out a stochastic effect.

We also report results for two intronic repeats in the AP-2β transcription factor gene, and a GGN microsatellite in the AR gene’s first exon. All of these have previously been associated with personality or behaviour-related phenotypes [24, 5461]. Our finding that the most common allele of the AR microsatellite, GGN23, was associated with fewer anxiety-related symptoms was consistent with previous work [24], though in our study this wasn’t significant following correction for multiple hypotheses. GGN23, elsewhere referred to as GGC16 [54], is the shorter of the two most common alleles and has been associated with reduced [51] and increased [52] activation of androgen receptor protein in different systems. Our only nominally significant results for the two previously studied AP-2β repeats were that carriers of the minor allele of the CAAA4-5 microsatellite polymorphism scored lower on psychotic symptoms at age 18 and higher on scholastic ability at age 13. The former was unexpected in view of a previous study reporting higher rates of anxiety and related traits in carriers of this allele among 137 Caucasians living in Sweden [60]. However, none of our findings in relation to this repeat were significant following Bonferroni correction.

We note that evidence for common genetic risk factors for many psychiatric, substance abuse and conduct disorder phenotypes [79] suggests that the lack of consistent associations in our data may indicate false positives. This argument does not apply to the TBR1 interaction, which did replicate across different ages and measures, but because this study is the first to link the TBR1 gene with antisocial behaviour, or with the effects of MSDP, the results should be treated with caution until replication can be attempted in another cohort. This point has been highlighted by a meta-analysis showing that first reports of gene-environment interactions are often not confirmed by studies attempting replication, presumably due to under-reporting of negative results [80]. Several considerations do, however, support the validity of our results at this stage. Firstly, our interaction hypothesis emerged from a coincidence of rare interests in conserved microsatellites, their influence on the genetics of behaviour, and the effects of MSDP. Previous attempts at finding the interaction are therefore very unlikely, and we have presented all of our negative results here. Furthermore, our sample size is substantial by the standards of G x E interaction studies [80], and the P-value of the interaction we observed also compares favourably with previous reports of G x E interaction involving MSDP and behavioural or cognitive phenotypes [8188]. In this field, high quality, multi-method phenotype measurement similar to that used for this study is usually performed. Along with adjustment for confounding factors this provides some compensation for sample sizes being smaller than most contemporary complex trait GWAS, which are thought to be limited by insufficiently detailed phenotyping [89].

The association between TBR1, MSDP and antisocial behaviour may be due to modification of the effects of tobacco exposure on embryonic development by TBR1 genotype. G x E interaction involving MSDP has been demonstrated directly in knockout mice [90], and both MSDP and microsatellite polymorphism have been linked to epigenetic modifications in offspring [12, 91, 92], However, the Genotype-Tissue Expression Project has so far not identified expression quantitative trait loci for TBR1 [93]. An alternative to the G x E interaction explanation is suggested by the likely existence of genetic factors which predispose mothers to both pregnancy smoking and antisocial behaviour [94]. An important role for gene-gene interaction (epistasis) would be consistent with large scale studies showing that siblings who are differentially exposed to prenatal tobacco are similarly prone to antisocial behaviour [95, 96]. Indeed, it has been suggested that promoter-associated and exonic microsatellites may be particularly likely to participate in epistasis due to their interactions with trans-acting factors [11].

SNPs associated with TBR1 have not been identified by GWAS of antisocial behaviour or related phenotypes [58]. Nevertheless, the possibility that effects of the microsatellite polymorphism were not detectable by these GWAS is supported by its very low level of LD with neighbouring SNPs, and by the absence of its effect on antisocial behaviour in individuals whose mothers didn’t smoke during pregnancy. The association we report, and the extremely high level of conservation of the TBR1 microsatellite [34], suggest that its putative functional importance deserves further testing. One possible approach would be to investigate the effect on transcriptional frequency of each allele using reporter plasmids in cultured neuronal cells [97]. Such assays have been used in previous studies to show that changes in repeat number of promoter-associated poly-AC microsatellites can affect frequency of transcription [98, 99]. However it is notable that they don’t necessarily provide an accurate model of gene expression in developing brain, and functional studies have shown cell type dependent microsatellite length effects, in one case with opposite effects in two different cell types [100].

Conclusions

We found limited support for the previously observed association between the AR gene’s exonic GGN microsatellite polymorphism and anxiety-related symptoms, but we found no significant results for two previously studied repeats in the AP2-β transcription factor gene. Our findings in relation to the AC microsatellite in the TBR1 promoter region suggest that it deserves further investigation, including replication in a separate cohort of its associations with antisocial behaviour and MSDP. They also demonstrate the potential value of investigating the functional importance of conserved microsatellites in general.

Acknowledgements

We thank John Pearson (University of Otago, Christchurch, NZ) for help with SNP analysis and also Sterling Sawaya (University of Colorado, USA), who led a study of conservation of microsatellites among mammalian species [40], and suggested the TBR1 microsatellite as a candidate genetic factor for psychological traits.

Funding

ATMB and MAK were supported by the Marsden Fund Council from Government funding, administered by the Royal Society of New Zealand (contract 08-UOO-114 BMS). This research was also supported by grants from the Health Research Council of New Zealand, the National Child Health Research Foundation, the Canterbury Medical Research Foundation, the New Zealand Lottery Grants Board, the University of Otago and the James Hume Bequest Fund.

Availability of data and materials

To protect the confidentiality of the study participants, raw data availability is limited to that contained in the manuscript and additional file.

Authors’ contributions

The project was conceived by ATMB, who carried out the genotyping and LD analyses and wrote the Abstract, Introduction and Discussion sections and some of the Results and Methods sections. LJH performed the remaining data analyses and wrote most of the Methods and Results sections. DMF, MAK and NJG provided funding and/or equipment and co-wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All phases of the CHDS were subject to ethical approval from the Canterbury (NZ) Regional Health and Disability Ethics Committee and all aspects of data collection were with the written consent of research participants.

Additional file

Additional file 1: (24.1KB, docx)

Supplementary methods and Table S1. (DOCX 20 kb)

Contributor Information

Andrew T. M. Bagshaw, Email: andrew.bagshaw@gmail.com

L. John Horwood, Email: john.horwood@otago.ac.nz.

David M. Fergusson, Email: david.fergusson@otago.ac.nz

Neil J. Gemmell, Email: neil.gemmell@otago.ac.nz

Martin A. Kennedy, Email: martin.kennedy@otago.ac.nz

References

  • 1.Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002;128:490–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hudziak JJ, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Bartels M, Rietveld MJH, Rettew DC, Derks EM, et al. Individual differences in aggression: genetic analyses by age, gender, and informant in 3-, 7-, and 10-year-old Dutch twins. Behav Genet. 2003;33:575–89. doi: 10.1023/A:1025782918793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Davies MN, Verdi S, Burri A, Trzaskowski M, Lee M, Hettema JM, et al. Generalised anxiety disorder--a twin study of genetic architecture, genome-wide association and differential gene expression. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0134865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vukasovic T, Bratko D. Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:769–85. doi: 10.1037/bul0000017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Pappa I, St Pourcain B, Benke K, Cavadino A, Hakulinen C, Nivard MG, et al. A genome-wide approach to children’s aggressive behavior: the EAGLE consortium. Genet: Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr; 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Salvatore JE, Edwards AC, McClintick JN, Bigdeli TB, Adkins A, Aliev F, et al. Genome-wide association data suggest ABCB1 and immune-related gene sets may be involved in adult antisocial behavior. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e558. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tielbeek JJ, Medland SE, Benyamin B, Byrne EM, Heath AC, Madden PAF, et al. Unraveling the genetic etiology of adult antisocial behavior: a genome-wide association study. PLoS One. 2012;7:e45086. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dick DM, Aliev F, Krueger RF, Edwards A, Agrawal A, Lynskey M, et al. Genome-wide association study of conduct disorder symptomatology. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:800–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Balestri M, Calati R, Serretti A, De Ronchi D. Genetic modulation of personality traits: a systematic review of the literature. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;29:1–15. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0b013e328364590b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hannan AJ. Tandem repeat polymorphisms: mediators of genetic plasticity, modulators of biological diversity and dynamic sources of disease susceptibility. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2012;769:1–9. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5434-2_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Press MO, Carlson KD, Queitsch C. The overdue promise of short tandem repeat variation for heritability. Trends Genet. 2014;30:504–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2014.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gymrek M, Willems T, Guilmatre A, Zeng H, Markus B, Georgiev S, et al. Abundant contribution of short tandem repeats to gene expression variation in humans. Nat Genet. 2016;48:22–9. doi: 10.1038/ng.3461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sawaya SM, Bagshaw AT, Buschiazzo E, Gemmell NJ. Promoter microsatellites as modulators of human gene expression. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2012;769:41–54. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5434-2_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Willems T, Gymrek M, Highnam G, Mittelman D, Erlich Y. The landscape of human STR variation. Genome Res. 2014;24:1894–904. doi: 10.1101/gr.177774.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vinces MD, Legendre M, Caldara M, Hagihara M, Verstrepen KJ. Unstable tandem repeats in promoters confer transcriptional evolvability. Science. 2009;324:1213–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1170097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Martin P, Makepeace K, Hill SA, Hood DW, Moxon ER. Microsatellite instability regulates transcription factor binding and gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:3800–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406805102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Quilez J, Guilmatre A, Garg P, Highnam G, Gymrek M, Erlich Y, et al. Polymorphic tandem repeats within gene promoters act as modifiers of gene expression and DNA methylation in humans. Nucleic Acids Res. 2016;44:3750–62. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bilgin Sonay T, Carvalho T, Robinson MD, Greminger MP, Krutzen M, Comas D, et al. Tandem repeat variation in human and great ape populations and its impact on gene expression divergence. Genome Res. 2015;25:1591–9. doi: 10.1101/gr.190868.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. The relative contribution of proximal 5’ flanking sequence and microsatellite variation on brain vasopressin 1a receptor (Avpr1a) gene expression and behavior. PLoS Genet. 2013;9:e1003729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Costa BM, Viana-Pereira M, Fernandes R, Costa S, Linhares P, Vaz R, et al. Impact of EGFR genetic variants on glioma risk and patient outcome. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2011;20:2610–7. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-11-0340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gau B-H, Chen T-M, Shih Y-HJ, Sun HS. FUBP3 interacts with FGF9 3’ microsatellite and positively regulates FGF9 translation. Nucleic Acids Res. 2011;39:3582–93. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq1295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kramer M, Sponholz C, Slaba M, Wissuwa B, Claus RA, Menzel U, et al. Alternative 5’ untranslated regions are involved in expression regulation of human heme oxygenase-1. PLoS One. 2013;8:e77224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Retz W, Reif A, Freitag CM, Retz-Junginger P, Rosler M. Association of a functional variant of neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene with self-reported impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in male offenders. J Neural Transm. 2010;117:321–4. doi: 10.1007/s00702-009-0352-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Westberg L, Henningsson S, Landen M, Annerbrink K, Melke J, Nilsson S, et al. Influence of androgen receptor repeat polymorphisms on personality traits in men. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:205–13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Spek CA, Bertina RM, Reitsma PH. Unique distance- and DNA-turn-dependent interactions in the human protein C gene promoter confer submaximal transcriptional activity. Biochem J. 1999;340(Pt 2):513–8. doi: 10.1042/bj3400513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Uhlemann A-C, Szlezak NA, Vonthein R, Tomiuk J, Emmer SA, Lell B, et al. DNA phasing by TA dinucleotide microsatellite length determines in vitro and in vivo expression of the gp91phox subunit of NADPH oxidase and mediates protection against severe malaria. J Infect Dis. 2004;189:2227–34. doi: 10.1086/421242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Willems R, Paul A, van der Heide HG, ter Avest AR, Mooi FR. Fimbrial phase variation in Bordetella pertussis: a novel mechanism for transcriptional regulation. EMBO J. 1990;9:2803–9. doi: 10.1002/j.1460-2075.1990.tb07468.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hefferon TW, Groman JD, Yurk CE, Cutting GR. A variable dinucleotide repeat in the CFTR gene contributes to phenotype diversity by forming RNA secondary structures that alter splicing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:3504–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400182101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hui J, Hung L-H, Heiner M, Schreiner S, Neumuller N, Reither G, et al. Intronic CA-repeat and CA-rich elements: a new class of regulators of mammalian alternative splicing. EMBO J. 2005;24:1988–98. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7600677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Liu H, Mulholland N, Fu H, Zhao K. Cooperative activity of BRG1 and Z-DNA formation in chromatin remodeling. Mol Cell Biol. 2006;26:2550–9. doi: 10.1128/MCB.26.7.2550-2559.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rothenburg S, Koch-Nolte F, Rich A, Haag F. A polymorphic dinucleotide repeat in the rat nucleolin gene forms Z-DNA and inhibits promoter activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:8985–90. doi: 10.1073/pnas.121176998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kouzine F, Levens D. Supercoil-driven DNA structures regulate genetic transactions. Front Biosci. 2007;12:4409–23. doi: 10.2741/2398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Carlson KD, Sudmant PH, Press MO, Eichler EE, Shendure J, Queitsch C. MIPSTR: a method for multiplex genotyping of germline and somatic STR variation across many individuals. Genome Res. 2015;25:750–61. doi: 10.1101/gr.182212.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sawaya SM, Lennon D, Buschiazzo E, Gemmell N, Minin VN. Measuring microsatellite conservation in mammalian evolution with a phylogenetic birth-death model. Genome Biol Evol. 2012;4:636–47. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evs050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Buschiazzo E, Gemmell NJ. Conservation of human microsatellites across 450 million years of evolution. Genome Biol Evol. 2010;2:153–65. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evq007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Dynamic mutations as digital genetic modulators of brain development, function and dysfunction. Bioessays. 2007;29:525–35. doi: 10.1002/bies.20589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bayatti N, Moss JA, Sun L, Ambrose P, Ward JFH, Lindsay S, et al. A molecular neuroanatomical study of the developing human neocortex from 8 to 17 postconceptional weeks revealing the early differentiation of the subplate and subventricular zone. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1536–48. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hevner RF, Hodge RD, Daza RAM, Englund C. Transcription factors in glutamatergic neurogenesis: conserved programs in neocortex, cerebellum, and adult hippocampus. Neurosci Res. 2006;55:223–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kwan KY, Sestan N, Anton ES. Transcriptional co-regulation of neuronal migration and laminar identity in the neocortex. Development. 2012;139:1535–46. doi: 10.1242/dev.069963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Huang T-N, Chuang H-C, Chou W-H, Chen C-Y, Wang H-F, Chou S-J, et al. Tbr1 haploinsufficiency impairs amygdalar axonal projections and results in cognitive abnormality. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:240–7. doi: 10.1038/nn.3626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chuang H-C, Huang T-N, Hsueh Y-P. T-brain-1--a potential master regulator in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2015;8:412–26. doi: 10.1002/aur.1456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Riva V, Battaglia M, Nobile M, Cattaneo F, Lazazzera C, Mascheretti S, et al. GRIN2B predicts attention problems among disadvantaged children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24:827–36. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0627-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Need AC, Attix DK, McEvoy JM, Cirulli ET, Linney KL, Hunt P, et al. A genome-wide study of common SNPs and CNVs in cognitive performance in the CANTAB. Hum Mol Genet. 2009;18:4650–61. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddp413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Aragam N, Wang K-S, Anderson JL, Liu X. TMPRSS9 and GRIN2B are associated with neuroticism: a genome-wide association study in a European sample. J Mol Neurosci. 2013;50:250–6. doi: 10.1007/s12031-012-9931-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Vink JM, Smit AB, de Geus EJC, Sullivan P, Willemsen G, Hottenga J-J, et al. Genome-wide association study of smoking initiation and current smoking. Am J Hum Genet. 2009;84:367–79. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Chuang H-C, Huang T-N, Hsueh Y-P. Neuronal excitation upregulates Tbr1, a high-confidence risk gene of autism, mediating Grin2b expression in the adult brain. Front Cell Neurosci. 2014;8:280. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Palumbo O, Fichera M, Palumbo P, Rizzo R, Mazzolla E, Cocuzza DM, et al. TBR1 is the candidate gene for intellectual disability in patients with a 2q24.2 interstitial deletion. Am J Med Genet A. 2014;164A:828–33. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.36363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.O’Roak BJ, Vives L, Girirajan S, Karakoc E, Krumm N, Coe BP, et al. Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature. 2012;485:246–50. doi: 10.1038/nature10989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Notwell JH, Heavner WE, Darbandi SF, Katzman S, McKenna WL, Ortiz-Londono CF, et al. TBR1 regulates autism risk genes in the developing neocortex. Genome Res. 2016;26:1013–22. doi: 10.1101/gr.203612.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Okbay A, Beauchamp JP, Fontana MA, Lee JJ, Pers TH, Rietveld CA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature. 2016;533:539–42. doi: 10.1038/nature17671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Brockschmidt FF, Nothen MM, Hillmer AM. The two most common alleles of the coding GGN repeat in the androgen receptor gene cause differences in protein function. J Mol Endocrinol. 2007;39:1–8. doi: 10.1677/JME-06-0072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lundin KB, Giwercman A, Dizeyi N, Giwercman YL. Functional in vitro characterisation of the androgen receptor GGN polymorphism. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2007;264:184–7. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2006.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Zajac JD, Fui MNT. Kennedy’s disease: clinical significance of tandem repeats in the androgen receptor. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2012;769:153–68. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5434-2_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Comings DE, Chen C, Wu S, Muhleman D. Association of the androgen receptor gene (AR) with ADHD and conduct disorder. Neuroreport. 1999;10:1589–92. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199905140-00036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, Blake H, et al. A multivariate analysis of 59 candidate genes in personality traits: the temperament and character inventory. Clin Genet. 2000;58:375–85. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2000.580508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Comings DE, Muhleman D, Johnson JP, MacMurray JP. Parent-daughter transmission of the androgen receptor gene as an explanation of the effect of father absence on age of menarche. Child Dev. 2002;73:1046–51. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nordquist N, Gokturk C, Comasco E, Nilsson KW, Oreland L, Hallman J. Transcription factor AP2 beta involved in severe female alcoholism. Brain Res. 2009;1305:S20–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hensch T, Wargelius H-L, Herold U, Strobel A, Oreland L, Brocke B. Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of polymorphisms in the transcription factor AP-2beta coding gene. Neurosci Lett. 2008;436:67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Damberg M, Garpenstrand H, Berggard C, Asberg M, Hallman J, Oreland L. The genotype of human transcription factor AP-2beta is associated with platelet monoamine oxidase B activity. Neurosci Lett. 2000;291:204–6. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01405-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Damberg M, Garpenstrand H, Alfredsson J, Ekblom J, Forslund K, Rylander G, et al. A polymorphic region in the human transcription factor AP-2beta gene is associated with specific personality traits. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:220–4. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Nilsson KW, Damberg M, Ohrvik J, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, Anckarsater H, et al. Genes encoding for AP-2beta and the Serotonin Transporter are associated with the Personality Character Spiritual Acceptance. Neurosci Lett. 2007;411:233–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.10.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hong SJ, Lardaro T, Oh MS, Huh Y, Ding Y, Kang UJ, et al. Regulation of the noradrenaline neurotransmitter phenotype by the transcription factor AP-2beta. J Biol Chem. 2008;283:16860–7. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M709106200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Takeuchi S, Imafuku I, Waragai M, Roth C, Kanazawa I, Buettner R, et al. AP-2beta represses D(1A) dopamine receptor gene transcription in neuro2a cells. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1999;74:208–16. doi: 10.1016/S0169-328X(99)00298-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ, Horwood LJ. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psychiatric adjustment in late adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:721–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.8.721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Gaysina D, Fergusson DM, Leve LD, Horwood J, Reiss D, Shaw DS, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems: evidence from 3 independent genetically sensitive research designs. JAMA psychiatry. 2013;70:956–63. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Saez TMM, Aronne MP, Caltana L, Brusco AH. Prenatal exposure to the CB1 and CB2 cannabinoid receptor agonist WIN 55,212-2 alters migration of early-born glutamatergic neurons and GABAergic interneurons in the rat cerebral cortex. J Neurochem. 2014;129:637–48. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Carpentier PA, Haditsch U, Braun AE, Cantu AV, Moon HM, Price RO, et al. Stereotypical alterations in cortical patterning are associated with maternal illness-induced placental dysfunction. J Neurosci. 2013;33:16874–88. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4654-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. The Christchurch Health and Development Study: review of findings on child and adolescent mental health. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35:287–96. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00902.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Shannon FT, Lawton JM. The Christchurch Child Development Study: a review of epidemiological findings. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1989;3:302–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.1989.tb00382.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Miller AL, Kennedy MA. Life stress, 5-HTTLPR and mental disorder: findings from a 30-year longitudinal study. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198:129–35. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Rutter M, Tizard J, Whitmore K. Education, Health and Behaviour. London: Longmans; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Conners CK. A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. Am J Psychiatry. 1969;126:884–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.6.884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lloyd M. Confirmatory factor models of attention deficit and conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32:257–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00305.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Quay HC PD. Manual for the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Miami: Authors; 1987.
  • 75.Costello A, Edelbrock C, Kalas R, Kessler MKS. Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC) Bethesda: National Institute of Mental Health; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Self-reported delinquency: Results from an instrument for New Zealand. Aust New Zeal J Criminol. 1988;21:227–40. doi: 10.1177/000486588802100405. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Elley WBIJ. Revised socio-economic index for New Zealand. New Zeal J Educ Stud. 1976;11:25–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Costello EJ, Eaves L, Sullivan P, Kennedy M, Conway K, Adkins DE, et al. Genes, environments, and developmental research: methods for a multi-site study of early substance abuse. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16:505–15. doi: 10.1017/thg.2013.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:929–37. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Duncan LE, Keller MC. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1041–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Brennan PA, Hammen C, Sylvers P, Bor W, Najman J, Lind P, et al. Interactions between the COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism and maternal prenatal smoking predict aggressive behavior outcomes. Biol Psychol. 2011;87:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Paus T, Bernard M, Chakravarty MM, Davey Smith G, Gillis J, Lourdusamy A, et al. KCTD8 gene and brain growth in adverse intrauterine environment: a genome-wide association study. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:2634–42. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Massey SH, Estabrook R, O’Brien TC, Pine DS, Burns JL, Jacob S, et al. Preliminary evidence for the interaction of the oxytocin receptor gene (oxtr) and face processing in differentiating prenatal smoking patterns. Neurosci Lett. 2015;584:259–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.10.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cents RAM, Tiemeier H, Velders FP, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child emotional problems: the relevance of maternal and child 5-HTTLPR genotype. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B:289–97. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Wakschlag LS, Kistner EO, Pine DS, Biesecker G, Pickett KE, Skol AD, et al. Interaction of prenatal exposure to cigarettes and MAOA genotype in pathways to youth antisocial behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:928–37. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, et al. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002;297:851–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.O’Brien TC, Mustanski BS, Skol A, Cook EHJ, Wakschlag LS. Do dopamine gene variants and prenatal smoking interactively predict youth externalizing behavior? Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;40:67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Morales E, Sunyer J, Julvez J, Castro-Giner F, Estivill X, Torrent M, et al. GSTM1 polymorphisms modify the effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on cognitive functioning in preschoolers. Int J Epidemiol. 2009;38:690–7. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Robinson MR, Wray NR, Visscher PM. Explaining additional genetic variation in complex traits. Trends Genet. 2014;30:124–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2014.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Baca M, Allan AM, Partridge LD, Wilson MC. Gene-environment interactions affect long-term depression (LTD) through changes in dopamine receptor affinity in Snap25 deficient mice. Brain Res. 2013;1532:85–98. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Breton CV, Siegmund KD, Joubert BR, Wang X, Qui W, Carey V, et al. Prenatal tobacco smoke exposure is associated with childhood DNA CpG methylation. PLoS One. 2014;9:e99716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Joubert BR, Haberg SE, Bell DA, Nilsen RM, Vollset SE, Midttun O, et al. Maternal smoking and DNA methylation in newborns: in utero effect or epigenetic inheritance? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2014;23:1007–17. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-13-1256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.The GTEx Portal. Available from: http://www.gtexportal.org. Accessed 7 Sep 2016.
  • 94.Ellingson JM, Rickert ME, Lichtenstein P, Langstrom N, D’Onofrio BM. Disentangling the relationships between maternal smoking during pregnancy and co-occurring risk factors. Psychol Med. 2012;42:1547–57. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.D’Onofrio BM, Van Hulle CA, Goodnight JA, Rathouz PJ, Lahey BB. Is maternal smoking during pregnancy a causal environmental risk factor for adolescent antisocial behavior? Testing etiological theories and assumptions. Psychol Med. 2012;42:1535–45. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.D’Onofrio BM, Singh AL, Iliadou A, Lambe M, Hultman CM, Grann M, et al. Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring criminality: a population-based study in Sweden. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:529–38. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Miraglia LJ, King FJ, Damoiseaux R. Seeing the light: luminescent reporter gene assays. Comb Chem High Throughput Screen. 2011;14:648–57. doi: 10.2174/138620711796504389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Rife T, Rasoul B, Pullen N, Mitchell D, Grathwol K, Kurth J. The effect of a promoter polymorphism on the transcription of nitric oxide synthase 1 and its relevance to Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci Res. 2009;87:2319–25. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Okladnova O, Syagailo YV, Tranitz M, Stober G, Riederer P, Mossner R, et al. A promoter-associated polymorphic repeat modulates PAX-6 expression in human brain. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 1998;248:402–5. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.1998.8972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Chen T-M, Kuo P-L, Hsu C-H, Tsai S-J, Chen M-J, Lin C-W, et al. Microsatellite in the 3’ untranslated region of human fibroblast growth factor 9 (FGF9) gene exhibits pleiotropic effect on modulating FGF9 protein expression. Hum Mutat. 2007;28:98. doi: 10.1002/humu.9471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

To protect the confidentiality of the study participants, raw data availability is limited to that contained in the manuscript and additional file.


Articles from BMC Medical Genetics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES