Abstract
The dorsomedial striatum (DMS) has been strongly implicated in flexible, outcome-based decision making, including the outcome-specific Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer effect (PIT), which measures the tendency for a reward-predictive cue to preferentially motivate actions that have been associated with the predicted reward over actions associated with different rewards. Although the neurochemical underpinnings of this effect are not well understood, there is growing evidence that striatal acetylcholine signaling may play an important role. The current study investigated this hypothesis by assessing the effects of intra-DMS infusions of the nicotinic antagonist mecamylamine or the muscarinic antagonist scopolamine on expression of specific PIT in rats. These treatments produced dissociable behavioral effects. Mecamylamine infusions enhanced rats’ tendency to use specific cue-elicited outcome expectations to select whichever action was trained with the predicted outcome, relative to their performance when tested after vehicle infusions. In contrast, scopolamine infusions appeared to render instrumental performance insensitive to this motivational influence of reward-paired cues. These drug treatments had no detectable effect on conditioned food-cup approach behavior, indicating that they selectively perturbed cue-guided action selection without producing more wide-ranging alterations in behavioral control. Our findings reveal an important role for DMS acetylcholine signaling in modulating the impact of cue-evoked reward expectations on instrumental action selection.
Keywords: acetylcholine, Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer, decision making, goal-directed behavior, basal ganglia
The dorsomedial striatum (DMS) is strongly implicated in the acquisition and flexible control of reward-seeking behavior (Ragozzino, 2007; Yin et al., 2008; Hart et al., 2014; Goodman & Packard, 2016), particularly when this involves the use of detailed expectations of behavioral goals or outcomes (Yin et al., 2005a; Yin et al., 2005b; Corbit & Janak, 2007b; Lex & Hauber, 2010; Shiflett et al., 2010; Corbit et al., 2013; Li et al., 2016). For instance, disrupting normal DMS function in rats impairs their ability to learn about new action-outcome contingencies or use previously encoded associations when selecting actions based on expected outcome value (Yin et al., 2005a; Yin et al., 2008). The DMS also appears to mediate flexible action selection based on cue-evoked reward expectations (Corbit & Janak, 2007b), an aspect of motivated behavior which can be selectively assayed using the outcome-specific Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT) task (Kruse et al., 1983). Outcome-specific PIT studies typically involve training rats during separate experimental phases with two different stimulus-outcome contingencies (S1—O1 & S2—O2) and two different action-outcome contingencies (A1—O1 & A2—O2). Subsequent test sessions are then used to assess the impact of noncontingent cue presentations on ongoing instrumental performance. Under normal conditions, presenting such a cue will selectively bias performance towards whichever action was trained with the same outcome as that cue (i.e., S1 will increase performance of A1 relative to A2). However, transiently inactivating the DMS during PIT testing has been shown to disrupt the outcome selectivity of this effect, resulting in a nonspecific increase in instrumental performance during cue presentations (Corbit & Janak, 2007b). Although studies such as these are typically conducted in rodents, recent findings suggest that expression of PIT in humans engages a homologous neural circuitry (Bray et al., 2008; Prevost et al., 2012). Interestingly, there is growing evidence that the processes underlying PIT contribute to drug seeking (Corbit & Janak, 2007a; Hogarth et al., 2007; LeBlanc et al., 2012; Garbusow et al., 2016) and may be compromised in certain maladaptive states such as heightened stress (Quail et al., 2016) and schizophrenia (Morris et al., 2015).
Recent findings also indicate that cholinergic activity within the striatum plays a crucial role in the expression of flexible reward-seeking behavior (Ragozzino, 2007). For instance, selective lesions of DMS cholinergic interneurons, the primary source of striatal acetylcholine, disrupt the expression of outcome-specific reinstatement (Matamales et al., 2016), which involves selecting actions based on the noncontingent presentation of actual rewards – as opposed to cue-evoked reward expectations (Ostlund & Balline, 2007). Although it was recently shown that systemic blockade of either muscarinic (mAChR) or nicotinic (nAChR) acetylcholine receptors prior to specific PIT testing disrupts the expression of this behavioral effect (Ostlund et al., 2014a), it remains unknown how acetylcholine signaling within the DMS contributes to action selection based on cue-evoked reward expectations. The current study investigated this issue by determining the effects of intra-DMS infusions of mecamylamine (selective mAChR antagonist) and scopolamine (selective nAChR antagonist) on expression of outcome-specific PIT.
Methods
Subjects
15 adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (300 – 425 g; Charles River Laboratories) were housed in a climate-controlled vivarium and tested during the light phase of a 12:12 h light:dark cycle. Rats were pair-housed up until surgery, after which they were individually housed for the remainder of the experiment. A food-restriction schedule (~10–14g/rat/day) was in place during training and testing to maintain rats at approximately 85% their free-feeding body weight. Ad libitum water was continuously provided in their home cages. All procedures were approved by the University of California, Los Angeles Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, and were performed in accordance with the National Research Council’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Apparatus
Behavioral testing was conducted in eight identical Med Associates (East Fairfield, VT) chambers, housed within light- and sound-attenuating cubicles. Each chamber contained two retractable levers, located on either side of a recessed food cup, into which 45-mg grain-based food pellets (Bioserv, Frenchtown, NJ) or 20% sucrose solution (0.1ml) could be delivered. A photobeam sensor detected head entries into the food cup. A house light (24V, 3W) provided continuous illumination during all sessions. White noise and clicker (10Hz) generators were used to deliver auditory stimuli (~70 dB).
Pavlovian Conditioning
Rats received eight once-daily sessions of Pavlovian conditioning in which each of two auditory-conditioned stimuli (CSs; noise or clicker; 2min each) was paired with a different food outcome (grain pellets or sucrose solution). Pavlovian stimulus-outcome contingencies were counterbalanced, such that half of the rats were given clicker-pellet and noise-sucrose pairings, and half were given clicker-sucrose and noise-pellet pairings. Each session lasted approximately 40min and consisted of four clicker and four noise trials, separated by a variable interval (mean=3.125min; range=2.25–4min). During each stimulus, the appropriate outcome was delivered on a random time 30-s schedule, resulting in an average of four outcome deliveries per trial.
Instrumental Conditioning
Rats then received 11 days of instrumental training. Each response (left vs. right lever press) was reinforced with a different outcome (pellet or sucrose). Responses were trained in separate sessions, such that rats received two sessions per day, with session order alternating over days. Action-outcome contingencies were counterbalanced with Pavlovian contingencies. Thus, half of the rats in each Pavlovian training condition were trained with left press-pellet and right press-sucrose contingencies, whereas the remaining half were trained with left press-sucrose and right press-pellet contingencies. Each session lasted until 30 rewards were earned or 30min had elapsed, whichever came first. Over days, the reinforcement schedule was gradually shifted from continuous reinforcement (2 days) to increasingly more effortful random ratio (RR) schedules (3 days each with RR-5, -10, and -20) to establish robust instrumental performance.
Surgery
After initial training, rats underwent asceptic stereotaxic surgery for bilateral guide cannula implantation under isoflurane anesthesia. Stainless steel guide cannulae (22-gauge, Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) were positioned such that their tips would be 1 mm above the intended infusion site in the DMS (AP: −0.4 mm from Bregma, ML: ± 2.6 mm from Bregma, DV: −4.2 mm from skull surface), following previous studies (Yin et al., 2005b; Shiflett et al., 2010). Rats were given 7 d to recover from surgery before further testing. They were given ad libitum home chow for 5 d after surgery, at which point they were returned to the food restriction regimen for the remainder of the experiment.
Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer testing
After recovering from surgery, rats were given 1 d of Pavlovian retraining followed by 2 d of instrumental retraining (RR-10, then RR-20), as described above. Rats were then administered a 1 h extinction session consisting of continuous nonreinforced access to both levers prior to PIT testing. Each test session involved 30 min of non-reinforced access to both levers with intermittent presentations of the reward-paired cues (also nonreinforced). Each cue (clicker and noise) was presented twice in a noncontingent manner for 2 min using an alternating trial order (clicker-noise-clicker-noise), with the first trial beginning 4 min into the session and trials separated by a fixed 4-min interval. Before each test, rats were given bilateral intra-DMS injections (0.5 µl/site) of vehicle (artificial cerebrospinal fluid), scopolamine hydrochloride (10 µg/site; Tocris Bioscience), or mecamylamine hydrochloride (10 µg/site; Tocris Bioscience) using a 0.5 µl/min flow rate. Injectors were left in place for an additional 1 min to facilitate drug diffusion. Rats were then placed in the chamber and the test session was initiated 1 min later. Drug doses and injection-to-test intervals were based on previous studies to maximize potential to alter reward-motivated behavior and minimize gross motor effects (Pratt & Kelley, 2004; Tzavos et al., 2004; Collins et al., 2016). Each rat was administered 4 PIT tests to provide a fully within-subjects assessment of each drug’s effect. During the first pair of tests, half of the rats were tested on scopolamine and half were tested on mecamylamine. This drug treatment occurred prior to Test 1 or Test 2 (counterbalanced with drug type and training contingencies), with the alternate test serving as a vehicle control. The same procedure was repeated for the second pair of tests except that the drug conditions were reversed (e.g., a rat given mecamylamine in Test 1 and vehicle in Test 2 would be given scopolamine in Test 3 and vehicle in Test 4). Retraining began 48 h after each test to allow for drug clearance. Prior to Tests 2–4, rats were given 1 d of Pavlovian conditioning and 3 d of instrumental conditioning (RR5, RR10, RR20), followed by 1d of response extinction (60 min), as described above.
Histology
After testing, rats were given an overdose of sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p.). Their brains were removed, postfixed, and cryoprotected in a 30% sucrose–formalin solution, and cut into 50 µm coronal sections across the DMS. Sections were mounted on glass slides, stained with cresyl violet, and analyzed under a light microscope to determine injector placements. All injector placements were confirmed to be within the DMS (see Figure 1 Paxinos and Watson (2005)).
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVAs or paired t-tests, as appropriate. We assessed the development of conditioned approach behavior by comparing the mean rate (responses/min) of food cup beam breaks during pre-CS and CS (prior to first reward delivery) periods across Pavlovian conditioning sessions (CS period × Session ANOVA). The acquisition of instrumental performance was assessed as the mean rate of lever pressing across conditioning sessions. Analysis of lever press rates during PIT testing focused on difference scores reflecting CS-induced changes in response rate (CS – pre-CS), which were calculated separately for each action based its relationship to the CS (Δ Same vs. Δ Different). Because rats were tested with concurrent access to both actions, a change in performance in one action may have impacted performance of the alternate action through response competition. Therefore, our response-specific difference scores are appropriate for evaluating how CS presentations influence action selection, as opposed to their ability to generally invigorate reward-seeking behavior (cf. Ostlund & Maidment, 2012). Analysis of these data included factors for Action and Drug treatment (Action × Drug ANOVA). Data were collapsed across the two vehicle tests after preliminary analysis confirmed that the specific PIT score was unaffected by this counterbalancing condition (F1,14 = .0.71, p = 0.41). The rate of food cup entry during PIT testing was analyzed as a function of Cue period and Drug condition (CS period × Drug ANOVA). The source of significant omnibus interactions in 2 × 3 ANOVAs was determined through assessment of partial interactions involving each combination of Drug treatment (2 × 2 ANOVAs). Significant interactions in 2 × 2 ANOVAs were assessed further through multiple pairwise comparisons (paired t-test, two-tailed) as advised by Levin et al. (1994) based on a logical extension of Fisher’s protected least significant difference (PLSD) procedure for controlling familywise Type I error rates. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 for all analyses.
Results
Pre-training
Rats were first given Pavlovian conditioning with two distinct stimulus-outcome relationships (S1-O1 & S2-O2). During these sessions, the rats acquired conditioned anticipatory food-cup approach behavior (Figure 2a), indicating that the training regimen was effective. Analysis of these data detected a significant main effect of Session (F7,105 = 29.71, p < 0.001) and CS period (F1,15 = 51.00, p < 0.001), as well as a significant Session × CS period interaction (F7,105 = 17.32, p < 0.001). In the next phase of the study, rats were trained to perform two different lever-press actions for different food rewards (R1-O1 & R2-O2). They readily acquired this behavior, steadily increasing their rate of lever pressing over sessions as the schedule of reinforcement increased from FR1 to RR20 (Figure 2b), as indicated by a significant main effect of Session (F10,140 = 248.07, p < 0.001).
Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer
Rats underwent a series of PIT tests to determine the effect of intra-DMS administration of scopolamine and mecamylamine on the outcome-specific influence of reward-paired cues on reward-seeking behavior. The results of PIT testing are presented in Figure 3. Inspection of these data indicate that rats showed a typical outcome-specific bias in lever pressing during CS presentations, choosing to perform whichever action was trained with the same outcome as the current CS over the alternate action (Different). This cue-evoked shift in performance appeared to be augmented when DMS nAChRs were blocked with mecamylamine and attenuated when DMS mAChRs were blocked with scopolamine (Figure 3a). Initial analysis of these data revealed that there was no effect of drug treatment on pre-CS (baseline) response rates (F2,28 = 0.221, p = 0.80; see Table 1 for full ANOVA table). To focus more directly on the influence of CS presentations on responding, we calculated the change in response rate for each action (e.g., Δ Same), relative to response-specific baseline values (CS – Pre-CS). Analysis of these data (Figure 3b) detected a significant main effect of Drug (F2,28 = 5.44, p = 0.01) and a significant Drug × Action interaction (F2,28 = 4.77, p = 0.017). To identify the source of this interaction, separate Drug × Action ANOVAs were run for the 3 combinations of drug treatments. A significant Drug × Action (partial) interaction was detected for ANOVA comparing tests vehicle and mecamylamine (F1,14 = 4.93, p = 0.043). Pairwise comparisons of this interaction based on Fisher’s PLSD (see Methods) found a significant simple effect of Action for both the vehicle (t14 = 2.22, p = 0.044) and mecamylamine (t14 = 7.83, p = 0.004) tests. There was a marginal effect of Drug for action Same (t14 = −2.07, p = 0.058) but not for action Different (t14 = 0.49, p = 0.633). Together with the significant interaction, these results suggest that intra-DMS mecamylamine enhanced the magnitude of the outcome-specific PIT effect (i.e., the difference between actions Same and Different). A significant Drug × Action (partial) interaction was also detected for the ANOVA comparing tests mecamylamine and scopolamine (F1,14 = 7.05, p = 0.019). Pairwise comparisons found no effect of Action for the scopolamine test (t14 = −0.94, p = 0.36), demonstrating that the influence of the CS over action selection was abolished by mAChR blockade. Consistent with this, a significant effect of Drug was detected for action Same (t14 = 3.67, p = 0.002) but not Different (t14 = −0.64, p = 0.54). The final Drug × Action ANOVA comparing tests vehicle and scopolamine did not result in a significant interaction (F1,14 = 2.23, p = 0.16).
Table 1.
Factors (levels) | d.f. | F-value | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Drug (3) | 2,28 | 0.899 | 0.419 |
Action (2) | 1,14 | 8.274 | 0.012* |
CS period (2) | 1,14 | 0.392 | 0.541 |
Drug × Action | 2,28 | 0.274 | 0.762 |
Drug × CS period | 2,28 | 5.439 | 0.010* |
Action × CS period | 1,14 | 3.104 | 0.100 |
Drug × Action × CS period | 2,28 | 4.766 | 0.017* |
A 3-way repeated-measures ANOVA (Drug × Action × CS period) was performed on the average rate of lever pressing during specific PIT testing. See Figure 3a for means and SEMs and see text for main analyses of cue-related changes in press rate.
P < 0.05.
Although our manipulations of DMS acetylcholine transmission produced opposing effects on cue-evoked instrumental reward seeking, they had little impact on cue-evoked anticipatory food cup approach behavior (Figure 3c). Analysis of these data revealed that food cup approach rates were elevated during CS trials, relative to the pre-CS period (main effect of CS period: F1,14 = 72.94, p < 0.001). Although there was a significant main effect of Drug (F2,28 = 3.43, p = 0.047), there was no interaction between Drug and CS period (F2,28 = 1.48, p = 0.99), indicating that drug effects were not specific to responding triggered by the CS. Interestingly, this main effect of Drug appeared to be driven by an increase in food cup approach responses (collapsing across CS period) during the scopolamine test (Fisher’s PLSD, effect of Drug comparing saline and scopolamine, F14 = 7.96, p = 0.014). Such findings strongly suggest that the scopolamine-induced attenuation in PIT performance was not the result of a nonspecific motor impairment.
Discussion
The current study tested the dependence of outcome-specific PIT performance on cholinergic signaling at mAChRs and nAChRs within the DMS. We found that muscarinic acetylcholine receptor blockade disrupted the use of cue-evoked reward expectations when selecting between reward-seeking actions. Blocking nicotinic acetylcholine receptors had the opposite effect, enhancing this aspect of cue-motivated reward seeking. These findings provide evidence that DMS acetylcholine plays an important role in mediating the influence of reward-paired cues on instrumental reward seeking.
While the current study demonstrates that DMS acetylcholine makes important contributions to specific PIT, the complexity of the striatal cholinergic system and its interactions with other neurochemical systems (Calabresi et al., 2000; Goldberg et al., 2012) will make it difficult to determine the specific mechanisms underlying behavioral effects described here. However, several possibilities are worth noting. For instance, it is well established that antagonizing M1 mAChRs decreases the excitability of striatal medium spiny projection neurons (Calabresi et al., 2000). The resulting disruption of DMS output would readily account for the attenuated PIT performance that we observed following intra-DMS injections of scopolamine. Likewise, nAChRs expressed by striatal inhibitory interneurons are well positioned to regulate striatal output (English et al., 2012; Lim et al., 2014). Blocking these nAChRs should weaken inhibitory tone on DMS projection neurons. It is therefore possible that the resulting facilitation of DMS output contributes to the augmentation of PIT performance produced by mecamylamine infusions. Our scopolamine injections may have also attenuated excitatory drive in the DMS via activation of mAChRs on glutamatergic terminals (Calabresi et al., 2000; Goldberg et al., 2012).
The current findings could also be explained by a more complex interaction between striatal acetylcholine and dopamine systems. For instance, a recent study (Collins et al., 2016) on the role of nucleus accumbens core acetylcholine in nonspecific PIT performance found that intra-core injections of scopolamine and mecamylamine had distinct behavioral effects, attenuating and augmenting expression of PIT, respectively, which is strikingly similar to the present findings. Interestingly, this earlier study found that these pharmacological manipulations of cholinergic signaling also altered cue-related dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core, with scopolamine blunting and mecamylamine enhancing dopamine release. Given the well-established role of dopamine in the nonspecific component of PIT (Dickinson et al., 2000; Lex & Hauber, 2008; Wassum et al., 2011; Ostlund & Maidment, 2012; Pecina & Berridge, 2013; Wassum et al., 2013; Ostlund et al., 2014b; Hebart & Glascher, 2015; Aitken et al., 2016; Collins et al., 2016), such findings suggest that acetylcholine activity in the nucleus accumbens core may modulate cue-motived behavior through its known regulation of dopamine release at striatal terminals. This hypothesis is bolstered by slice voltammetry studies showing that nAChRs and mAChRs play opposing roles in regulating terminal dopamine release in both the ventral and dorsal striatum (Sulzer et al., 2016). Although a similar mechanism may underlie the current results, the finding that dopamine-depleting lesions of the DMS produce only modest, nonsignificant effects on specific PIT (Pielock et al., 2011) raises questions about the importance of DMS dopamine in this aspect of cue-motivated behavior.
Although the current findings and previous results (Collins et al. 2016) indicate that striatal nAChRs exert a net suppressive influence over cue-motivated behavior, our previous finding that systemic blockade of nAChRs disrupts specific PIT suggests that nAChRs at extra-striatal sites facilitate expression of this behavioral effect (Ostlund et al., 2014a). Potential targets for future studies include regions rich in nAChRs which have been implicated in specific PIT, such as the orbitofrontal cortex, mediodorsal striatum, basolateral amygdala, and ventral tegmental area (Blundell et al., 2001; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Corbit et al., 2007; Ostlund & Balleine, 2007; 2008; Shiflett & Balleine, 2010; Prevost et al., 2012; Leung & Balleine, 2015; Malvaez et al., 2015; Parnaudeau et al., 2015).
While reward-predictive cues normally provide an adaptive influence over action selection and initiation, there is great interest in the possibility that aberrant Pavlovian learning contributes to the development of pathological drug seeking, overeating and other disorders of behavioral control (Everitt et al., 2001; Robinson & Berridge, 2008). For individuals attempting to quit using drugs, drug cues can promote intense drug craving and trigger relapse (O'Brien et al., 1992; Epstein et al., 2009; Tiffany & Wray, 2012). Interestingly, it has been shown that rats given repeated exposure to cocaine or amphetamine are more sensitive to the response-invigorating effects of food-paired cues during PIT testing (Wyvell & Berridge, 2001; Saddoris et al., 2011; LeBlanc et al., 2013; Shiflett et al., 2013; LeBlanc et al., 2014; Ostlund et al., 2014b), suggesting that such drugs are capable of producing long-lasting adaptations in the neural circuitry underlying Pavlovian incentive motivation. Most studies on this topic have applied relatively simple PIT tasks that do not assay the influence of outcome expectations on response selection. However, one recent study using a specific PIT task (Shiflett, 2012) found that repeated amphetamine exposure disrupts the outcome specificity of this effect, which is generally in line with a wider body of research showing that chronic drug exposure can impair certain aspects of outcome encoding (Stalnaker et al., 2009). Such findings may be relevant to understanding self-reports of generalized craving for palatable foods and other non-drug rewards by illicit drug users (Picozzi et al., 1972; Gambera & Clarke, 1976; Weiss, 1982; Nolan & Scagnelli, 2007), smokers (Spring et al., 2003; Mahler & de Wit, 2005), alcoholics (Moorhouse et al., 2000), and Parkinson’s disease patients undergoing dopamine agonist treatment (Giovannoni et al., 2000). The current results identify DMS acetylcholine as a neurochemical target for future studies investigating the maladaptive influence of reward-predictive cues on decision making.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by National Institutes of Health grants AG045380 and DK098709 (SBO and NTM), DA029035 (SBO), and MH106972 (SBO and KMW). Funding sources were not involved in study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of data, or in manuscript preparation.
References
- Aitken TJ, Greenfield VY, Wassum KM. Nucleus accumbens core dopamine signaling tracks the need-based motivational value of food-paired cues. J Neurochem. 2016;136:1026–1036. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blundell P, Hall G, Killcross S. Lesions of the basolateral amygdala disrupt selective aspects of reinforcer representation in rats. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9018–9026. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-09018.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bray S, Rangel A, Shimojo S, Balleine B, O'Doherty JP. The neural mechanisms underlying the influence of pavlovian cues on human decision making. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5861–5866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0897-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calabresi P, Centonze D, Gubellini P, Pisani A, Bernardi G. Acetylcholine-mediated modulation of striatal function. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:120–126. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01501-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AL, Aitken TJ, Greenfield VY, Ostlund SB, Wassum KM. Nucleus Accumbens Acetylcholine Receptors Modulate Dopamine and Motivation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016 doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Balleine BW. Double dissociation of basolateral and central amygdala lesions on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. J Neurosci. 2005;25:962–970. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4507-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Janak PH. Ethanol-associated cues produce general pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007a;31:766–774. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00359.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Janak PH. Inactivation of the lateral but not medial dorsal striatum eliminates the excitatory impact of Pavlovian stimuli on instrumental responding. J Neurosci. 2007b;27:13977–13981. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4097-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Janak PH, Balleine BW. General and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer: the effect of shifts in motivational state and inactivation of the ventral tegmental area. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:3141–3149. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05934.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Leung BK, Balleine BW. The role of the amygdala-striatal pathway in the acquisition and performance of goal-directed instrumental actions. J Neurosci. 2013;33:17682–17690. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3271-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Smith J, Mirenowicz J. Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114:468–483. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.3.468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- English DF, Ibanez-Sandoval O, Stark E, Tecuapetla F, Buzsaki G, Deisseroth K, Tepper JM, Koos T. GABAergic circuits mediate the reinforcement-related signals of striatal cholinergic interneurons. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:123–130. doi: 10.1038/nn.2984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein DH, Willner-Reid J, Vahabzadeh M, Mezghanni M, Lin JL, Preston KL. Real-time electronic diary reports of cue exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin craving and use. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:88–94. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Dickinson A, Robbins TW. The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001;36:129–138. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00088-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gambera SE, Clarke JA. Comments on dietary intake of drug-dependent persons. J Am Diet Assoc. 1976;68:155–157. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garbusow M, Schad DJ, Sebold M, Friedel E, Bernhardt N, Koch SP, Steinacher B, Kathmann N, Geurts DE, Sommer C, Muller DK, Nebe S, Paul S, Wittchen HU, Zimmermann US, Walter H, Smolka MN, Sterzer P, Rapp MA, Huys QJ, Schlagenhauf F, Heinz A. Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer effects in the nucleus accumbens relate to relapse in alcohol dependence. Addict Biol. 2016;21:719–731. doi: 10.1111/adb.12243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giovannoni G, O'Sullivan JD, Turner K, Manson AJ, Lees AJ. Hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation in patients with Parkinson's disease on dopamine replacement therapies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68:423–428. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.68.4.423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg JA, Ding JB, Surmeier DJ. Muscarinic modulation of striatal function and circuitry. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2012:223–241. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-23274-9_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman J, Packard MG. Memory Systems and the Addicted Brain. Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:24. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart G, Leung BK, Balleine BW. Dorsal and ventral streams: the distinct role of striatal subregions in the acquisition and performance of goal-directed actions. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;108:104–118. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebart MN, Glascher J. Serotonin and dopamine differentially affect appetitive and aversive general Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015;232:437–451. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3682-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Wright A, Kouvaraki M, Duka T. The role of drug expectancy in the control of human drug seeking. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2007;33:484–496. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.33.4.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruse JM, Overmier JB, Konz WA, Rokke E. Pavlovian Conditioned-Stimulus Effects Upon Instrumental Choice Behavior Are Reinforcer Specific. Learn Motiv. 1983;14:165–181. [Google Scholar]
- LeBlanc KH, Maidment NT, Ostlund SB. Repeated cocaine exposure facilitates the expression of incentive motivation and induces habitual control in rats. PLoS One. 2013;8:e61355. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeBlanc KH, Maidment NT, Ostlund SB. Impact of repeated intravenous cocaine administration on incentive motivation depends on mode of drug delivery. Addict Biol. 2014;19:965–971. doi: 10.1111/adb.12063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeBlanc KH, Ostlund SB, Maidment NT. Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in cocaine seeking rats. Behav Neurosci. 2012;126:681–689. doi: 10.1037/a0029534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung BK, Balleine BW. Ventral pallidal projections to mediodorsal thalamus and ventral tegmental area play distinct roles in outcome-specific Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. J Neurosci. 2015;35:4953–4964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4837-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin JR, Serlin RC, Seaman MA. A Controlled, Powerful Multiple-Comparison Strategy for Several Situations. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:153–159. [Google Scholar]
- Lex A, Hauber W. Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens core and shell mediate Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Learn Mem. 2008;15:483–491. doi: 10.1101/lm.978708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lex B, Hauber W. Disconnection of the entorhinal cortex and dorsomedial striatum impairs the sensitivity to instrumental contingency degradation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1788–1796. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, He Y, Chen M, Pu Z, Chen L, Li P, Li B, Li H, Huang ZL, Li Z, Chen JF. Optogenetic Activation of Adenosine A2A Receptor Signaling in the Dorsomedial Striatopallidal Neurons Suppresses Goal-Directed Behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:1003–1013. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim SA, Kang UJ, McGehee DS. Striatal cholinergic interneuron regulation and circuit effects. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2014;6:22. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2014.00022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahler SV, de Wit H. Effects of haloperidol on reactions to smoking cues in humans. Behav Pharmacol. 2005;16:123–126. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200503000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malvaez M, Greenfield VY, Wang AS, Yorita AM, Feng L, Linker KE, Monbouquette HG, Wassum KM. Basolateral amygdala rapid glutamate release encodes an outcome-specific representation vital for reward-predictive cues to selectively invigorate reward-seeking actions. Sci Rep. 2015;5:12511. doi: 10.1038/srep12511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matamales M, Skrbis Z, Hatch RJ, Balleine BW, Gotz J, Bertran-Gonzalez J. Aging-Related Dysfunction of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons Produces Conflict in Action Selection. Neuron. 2016;90:362–373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moorhouse M, Loh E, Lockett D, Grymala J, Chudzik G, Wilson A. Carbohydrate craving by alcohol-dependent men during sobriety: relationship to nutrition and serotonergic function. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:635–643. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RW, Quail S, Griffiths KR, Green MJ, Balleine BW. Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolan LJ, Scagnelli LM. Preference for sweet foods and higher body mass index in patients being treated in long-term methadone maintenance. Subst Use Misuse. 2007;42:1555–1566. doi: 10.1080/10826080701517727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R. Classical conditioning in drug-dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992;654:400–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25984.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in Pavlovian but not instrumental conditioning. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4819–4825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5443-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Differential involvement of the basolateral amygdala and mediodorsal thalamus in instrumental action selection. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4398–4405. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5472-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Balline BW. Selective reinstatement of instrumental performance depends on the discriminative stimulus properties of the mediating outcome. Learn Behav. 2007;35:43–52. doi: 10.3758/bf03196073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Kosheleff AR, Maidment NT. Differential effects of systemic cholinergic receptor blockade on Pavlovian incentive motivation and goal-directed action selection. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014a;39:1490–1497. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, LeBlanc KH, Kosheleff AR, Wassum KM, Maidment NT. Phasic mesolimbic dopamine signaling encodes the facilitation of incentive motivation produced by repeated cocaine exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014b;39:2441–2449. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Maidment NT. Dopamine receptor blockade attenuates the general incentive motivational effects of noncontingently delivered rewards and reward-paired cues without affecting their ability to bias action selection. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:508–519. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnaudeau S, Taylor K, Bolkan SS, Ward RD, Balsam PD, Kellendonk C. Mediodorsal thalamus hypofunction impairs flexible goal-directed behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. New York: Academic Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Pecina S, Berridge KC. Dopamine or opioid stimulation of nucleus accumbens similarly amplify cue-triggered 'wanting' for reward: entire core and medial shell mapped as substrates for PIT enhancement. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:1529–1540. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picozzi A, Dworkin SF, Leeds JG, Nash J. Dental and associated attitudinal aspects of heroin addiction: a pilot study. J Dent Res. 1972;51:869. doi: 10.1177/00220345720510032901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pielock SM, Lex B, Hauber W. The role of dopamine in the dorsomedial striatum in general and outcome-selective Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:717–725. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratt WE, Kelley AE. Nucleus accumbens acetylcholine regulates appetitive learning and motivation for food via activation of muscarinic receptors. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:730–739. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prevost C, Liljeholm M, Tyszka JM, O'Doherty JP. Neural correlates of specific and general Pavlovian-to-Instrumental Transfer within human amygdalar subregions: a high-resolution fMRI study. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8383–8390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6237-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quail SL, Morris RW, Balleine BW. Stress associated changes in Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in humans. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016:1–11. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1149198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragozzino ME. The contribution of the medial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and dorsomedial striatum to behavioral flexibility. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:355–375. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093. Review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saddoris MP, Stamatakis A, Carelli RM. Neural correlates of Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in the nucleus accumbens shell are selectively potentiated following cocaine self-administration. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:2274–2287. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07683.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiflett MW. The effects of amphetamine exposure on outcome-selective Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;223:361–370. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2724-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiflett MW, Balleine BW. At the limbic-motor interface: disconnection of basolateral amygdala from nucleus accumbens core and shell reveals dissociable components of incentive motivation. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;32:1735–1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07439.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiflett MW, Brown RA, Balleine BW. Acquisition and performance of goal-directed instrumental actions depends on ERK signaling in distinct regions of dorsal striatum in rats. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2951–2959. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1778-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiflett MW, Riccie M, DiMatteo R. The effects of amphetamine sensitization on conditioned inhibition during a Pavlovian-instrumental transfer task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013;230:137–147. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3144-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spring B, Pagoto S, McChargue D, Hedeker D, Werth J. Altered reward value of carbohydrate snacks for female smokers withdrawn from nicotine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;76:351–360. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2003.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Takahashi Y, Roesch MR, Schoenbaum G. Neural substrates of cognitive inflexibility after chronic cocaine exposure. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56(Suppl 1):63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulzer D, Cragg SJ, Rice ME. Striatal dopamine neurotransmission: regulation of release and uptake. Basal Ganglia. 2016;6:123–148. doi: 10.1016/j.baga.2016.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST, Wray JM. The clinical significance of drug craving. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1248:1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06298.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzavos A, Jih J, Ragozzino ME. Differential effects of M1 muscarinic receptor blockade and nicotinic receptor blockade in the dorsomedial striatum on response reversal learning. Behav Brain Res. 2004;154:245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wassum KM, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW, Maidment NT. Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling. Learn Mem. 2011;18:475–483. doi: 10.1101/lm.2229311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wassum KM, Ostlund SB, Loewinger GC, Maidment NT. Phasic mesolimbic dopamine release tracks reward seeking during expression of Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:747–755. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss G. Food fantasies of incarcerated drug users. Int J Addict. 1982;17:905–912. doi: 10.3109/10826088209056337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyvell CL, Berridge KC. Incentive sensitization by previous amphetamine exposure: increased cue-triggered "wanting" for sucrose reward. J Neurosci. 2001;21:7831–7840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07831.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action-outcome learning in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005a;22:505–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:1437–1448. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06422.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005b;22:513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]