Abstract
Objectives
Higher or lower blood pressure may relate to cognitive impairment, while the relationship between blood pressure and cognitive impairment among the elderly is not well-studied. The study objective was to determine whether blood pressure is associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly, and, if so, to accurately describe the association.
Design
Cross-sectional data from the sixth wave of the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) conducted in 2011
Setting
Community-based setting in longevity areas in China
Participants
7,144 Chinese elderly aged 65 years and older were included in the sample
Measures
Systolic blood pressures (SBP) and diastolic blood pressures (DBP) were measured, pulse pressure (PP) was calculated as (SBP)-(DBP) and mean arterial pressures (MAP) was calculated as 1/3(SBP) + 2/3(DBP). Cognitive function was assessed via a validated Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE).
Results
Based on the results of generalized additive models (GAMs), U-shaped associations were identified between cognitive impairment and SBP, DBP, PP and MAP. The cut-points at which risk for cognitive impairment (MMSE<24) was minimized were determined by quadratic models as 141 mmHg, 85 mmHg, 62 mmHg and 103 mmHg, respectively. In the logistic models, U-shaped associations remained for SBP, DBP, and MAP but not PP. Below the identified cut-points, each 1mmHg decrease in blood pressure corresponded to 0.7%, 1.1%, and 1.1% greater risk in the risk of cognitive impairment, respectively. Above the cut-points, each 1mmHg increase in blood pressure corresponded to 1.2%, 1.8%, and 2.1% greater risk of cognitive impairment for SBP, DBP and MAP, respectively.
Conclusion
A U-shaped association between blood pressure and cognitive function in an elderly Chinese population was found. Recognition of these instances is important identifying the high-risk population for cognitive impairment and to individualize blood pressure management for cognitive impairment prevention.
Keywords: lood pressure, hypertension, hypotension, cognitive impairment, curvilinear association, elderly
Introduction
Hypertension and hypotension are important risk factors for vascular diseases, which may lead to cognitive impairment due to stroke or chronic cerebral hypoperfusion in the elderly [1]. This suggests that hypertension and hypotension may both be associated with cognitive impairment, a common neurological disorder in the elderly.
In recent years, the link between blood pressure, including systolic blood pressures (SBP) and diastolic blood pressures (DBP), to cognitive function has attracted much attention, but the role of blood pressure in cognitive impairment is still unclear [2–14]. Several studies reported a linear association between elevated blood pressure and increased risk of cognitive impairment or dementia [2, 3], and low blood pressure was related to higher risk of dementia in the elderly [4, 5]. Additionally, antihypertensive medicines have a valuable role in the prevention of dementia or cognitive impairment [6, 7], while some studies failed to confirm this association [8, 9].
One proposed explanation for inconsistencies among these studies is that the relationship between blood pressure and cognitive function is nonlinear. Several investigators have demonstrated a U-shaped relationship [10–14] or a J-shaped association between cognitive function and blood pressure [4]. Most related researches use linear model including multivariate regression model [12,15], hierarchical linear model [14,16], mixed-effects regression analyses[11] to examine the possible relations. Few studies use splines to determine presence and type of the curvilinear relationships [17]. None of the curvilinear associations were demonstrated using generalized additive models (GAMs) and quadratic models, whose strengths include the ability to determine the natural shape of the relationship, sensitivity to detect non-linear relationships and identifying the optimal cut-points between dependent and predictor variable(s).
Combined measures such as pulse pressure (PP) or mean arterial pressure (MAP) may be a salient blood pressure index in the elderly[18] as SBP continuously increases and DBP continuously decreases after age 60[19]. Notably, PP and cognitive function have not been extensively studied, with positive associations with cognitive impairment being reported in some [20–23] but not all [24] studies. Some studies suggest that MAP may be a more accurate predictor than other blood pressure indices [25, 26].
It is important to clarify whether blood pressure is associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly, and, if so, to accurately describe the association. In the present study, we investigated the relationship between blood pressure (as measured by SBP, DBP, PP and MAP) and cognitive impairment among Chinese elderly, and the possibility of a linear or curvilinear relationship using GAMs and quadratic models.
Methods
Study population
A sample of 7144 elderly aged 65 years and older was included in the sixth wave of the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) conducted in 2011. The details of the CLHLS and sample design have been described elsewhere[27,28]. Informed consent was obtained from all participants and/or their relatives, and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Peking University.
Assessment of cognitive function
Cognitive function was assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [29], which has been widely applied in epidemiological studies. Five domains of cognitive function were included, namely orientation, registration, attention, memory, language and visual construction skills [30]. The scores ranged from 0 to 30 points, and cognitive impairment was identified if the MMSE score was less than 24 [31].
Assessment of blood pressure
After having rested for 5 minutes under supervision of trained research assistants, arterial blood pressure was measured with a mercury sphygmomanometer on the right arm at heart level of the seated subject. In our analysis, the mean value of two blood pressure measurements was calculated and used for further analyses. PP was calculated as (SBP)-(DBP) and MAP was calculated as 1/3(SBP) + 2/3(DBP). Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥140mm Hg and/or diastolic blood pressure ≥90mm Hg.
Assessment of covariates
Covariates were collected by a structured questionnaire, one part is sociodemographic characteristics including age, sex, educational background and residence; the other part is health characteristics including physical activity, measured waist circumference, and self-reported diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular diseases and stroke and other cerebrovascular diseases. Age and education were two of the most important confounders in the analysis of association between cognitive impairment and blood pressure, and were modeled as continuous variables in years. Residence was categorized as urban or rural. Physical activity was classified into “yes” or “no” by the question “Do you often participate in physical activities, including walking, playing ball, running, and Qigong?”. Non-stretchable tape was used to measure waist circumference at a level between the lowest rib and iliac crest. Central obesity was identified if the participant’s waist circumference was ≥85cm in men or ≥80cm in women.
Statistical analyses
Generalized additive models were performed to explore the linear or curvilinear association between SBP, DBP, PP, MAP and odds of having cognitive impairment. GAMs establish a relationship between the mean of a dependent variable and a smoothed (non-parametric) function of the predictor variable(s) via a link function. As GAM analysis suggested U-shaped associations between SBP, DBP, PP, MAP and risk of cognitive impairment on the basis of smoothed curve (Figure 1), quadratic regression was performed predicting risk of cognitive impairment: Pr(Cognitive impairment|Blood pressure) = {1+exp[-(α+β1(Blood pressure)+ β2(Blood pressure)2]}−1, with models parameters α, β1 and β2 estimated using maximum likelihood methods. If β1 and β2 are both non-zero and statistically significant, then the estimated level of blood pressure at which risk of cognitive impairment is minimized is -(β1/2β2). These values are used as cut-points of blood pressure for further analysis. For SBP, DBP, PP and MAP, the β1 were estimated as −0.07908, −0.10114, −0.03585 and −0.13279 respectively, and the β2 were estimated as 0.00028, 0.00059, 0.00029 and 0.00064 respectively; all p values were less than 0.01. The blood pressure values associated with lowest risk were identified as 141 mm Hg, 85 mm Hg, 62 mm Hg and 103 mm Hg for SBP, DBP, PP and MAP, respectively. For each blood pressure measure, subjects were divided into 4 groups, first based on whether they were above or below the cut-point for that measure, and within these 2 groups whether they were above or below the mean value of that measure. Thus, subjects were first stratified into two groups by cut-points of each blood pressure index (values noted above); subjects in the low group were then divided into two sub-groups (denoted low and medium-low) using medians of each index (127mm Hg, 77 mm Hg, 49 mm Hg, 93 mm Hg for SBP, DBP, PP and MAP, respectively) and subjects in the high group were divided into two sub-groups (denoted high and medium-high) using the medians of each index (155mm Hg, 90 mm Hg, 73 mm Hg, 110 mm Hg for SBP, DBP, PP and MAP respectively).
Then logistic regression models were performed to assess the risk of cognitive impairment with SBP, DBP, PP and MAP as continuous or binary predictor variables below and above the cut-points. In a basic model, we adjusted for demographic variables including age, gender, years of education, and residence, and further adjusted for physical activity, central obesity, diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular diseases, stroke and cerebrovascular diseases in the final model. Odds ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) in logistic regression models were presented. SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) was used for all analyses.
Results
Description of population
The age of 7,144 participants ranged from 65 to 115 years, with an average of 87 years; 55% of participants were female. The mean SBP, DBP, PP and MAP were 135.5, 80.1, 55.4 and 98.6 mm Hg, respectively; 42.3% of participants had hypertension (Table 1). The average MMSE score was 21.1, and prevalence of cognitive impairment was 38.5% (27.8% for men and 47.3% for women). The participants who were men, living in urban areas, participating in physical activity, and with cardiovascular diseases were less likely to be cognitively impaired. The participants with type 2 diabetes or stroke and cerebrovascular diseases were more likely to be cognitively impaired (Table 1). The prevalence of hypertension or blood pressure measures was not significantly different in those with and without cognitive impairment.
Table 1.
Factors* | Normal cognition n=4393 |
Cognitive impairment n=2751 |
Total n=7144 |
P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
MMSE | 26.8(2.3) | 9.2(6.7) | 21.1(10.9) | <0.0001 |
Age (year) | 83.3(9.7) | 93.2(9.7) | 87.1(10.9) | <0.0001 |
Gender | ||||
Male | 2335(46.9) | 900(32.7) | 3235(45.3) | <0.0001 |
Female | 2058(53.1) | 1851(67.3) | 3909(54.7) | |
Residence | ||||
Urban | 2226(59.8) | 1464(53.2) | 4430(57.2) | <0.0001 |
Rural | 1787(40.2) | 1287(46.8) | 3054(42.8) | |
Education (year) | 3.2(3.0) | 1.1(2.5) | 2.4(3.6) | <0.0001 |
Physically active | 2100(48.0) | 671(24.5) | 2771(39.0) | <0.0001 |
Good visual function | 3749(85.3) | 1577(57.3) | 5326(74.6) | <0.0001 |
Central obesity | 2198(50.0) | 1102(40.1) | 3300(46.2) | <0.0001 |
Diabetes mellitus | 248(5.9) | 92(3.7) | 340(5.1) | <0.0001 |
Cardiovascular diseases | 653(15.5) | 324(12.8) | 977(14.5) | 0.0024 |
Stroke and cerebrovascular diseases | 332(7.9) | 265(10.4) | 597(8.8) | 0.0004 |
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) | 135.7(19.4) | 135.2(22.0) | 135.5(20.4) | 0.3055 |
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) | 80.3(11.2) | 79.8(12.9) | 80.1(11.9) | 0.1302 |
Pulse pressure (mmHg) | 55.4(15.9) | 55.4(17.5) | 55.4(16.6) | 0.8600 |
Mean arterial pressure(mmHg) | 98.8(12.3) | 98.3(14.3) | 98.6(13.1) | 0.1494 |
Hypertension | 1859(42.3) | 1162(42.2) | 3021(42.3) | 0.9481 |
Dichotomous variables listed as n (%), including gender, current smoking practice, alcohol drinking habits, central obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, stroke and cerebrovascular diseases, hypertension, were compared between two cognitive status groups using Chi-square tests; Continuous variables listed as mean (standard deviation), including age, education, SBP, DBP, PP, MAP, were comparted between two groups using Student’s T tests or Cochran-Cox tests. MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.
Associations between blood pressure and cognitive impairment
The results of GAMs and quadratic models suggested a U-shaped association between SBP, DBP, PP, and MAP and cognitive impairment. Logistic models were consistent with the U-shaped associations between cognitive impairment and SBP, DBP, and MAP; however, such an association was not apparent for PP, regardless of considering PP as a continuous or categorical variable.
SBP and cognitive impairment
Among the participants whose SBP levels were below the cut-point (141 mm Hg), there was a trend towards significantly decreasing risk of cognitive impairment with increasing levels of SBP; each 1mm Hg lower SBP corresponded to a 1.4% higher risk of cognitive impairment before adjustment (OR, 1.014; 95% CI, 1.009–1.019). The risk attenuation remained significant after adjusting for covariates in the basic and final models, which decreased to 0.8% (OR, 1.008; 95% CI, 1.003–1.014) and 0.7% (OR, 1.007; 95% CI, 1.002–1.013) respectively (Table 2). Participants whose SBP levels were below the cut-point were stratified into two equal-sized groups, the univariate analysis showed that the low SBP group had higher risk of cognitive impairment compared to medium-low SBP group (OR, 1.25; 95% CI, 1.12–1.41). The multivariate OR (95% CI) reported in the basic model was 1.15 (1.00–1.32) and in the final model was 1.18 (1.02–1.35) (Table 3).
Table 2.
Variables | Unadjusted | Basic model‡ | Final model§ |
---|---|---|---|
Each 1mmHg lower in participants with SBP <141mmHg (n=4733) |
1.014(1.009–1.019) † | 1.008(1.003–1.014) † | 1.007(1.002–1.013) † |
Each 1mmHg higher in participants with SBP ≥141mmHg ( n=2411) |
1.012(1.006–1.017) † | 1.010(1.004–1.017) † | 1.012(1.005–1.019) † |
Each 1mmHg lowerin participants with DBP<85mmHg (n=5180) |
1.023(1.015–1.030) † | 1.011(1.003–1.019) † | 1.011(1.003–1.020)* |
Each 1mmHg higherin participants with DBP ≥85mmHg (n=1964) |
1.018(1.007–1.028) † | 1.021(1.009–1.033) † | 1.018(1.006–1.030) † |
Each 1mmHg lower in participants with PP<62mmHg (n=5111) |
1.013(1.007–1.20) † | 1.008(1.001–1.015)* | 1.008(1.000–1.015)* |
Each 1mmHg higherin participants with PP≥62mmHg (n=2033) |
1.009(1.002–1.017)* | 1.002(0.994–1.011) | 1.005(0.996–1.013) |
Each 1mmHg lower in participants with MAP<103mmHg (n=4792) |
1.022(1.014–1.029) † | 1.012(1.004–1.021) † | 1.011(1.003–1.020)* |
Each 1mmHg higher in participants with MAP≥103mmHg (n=2352) |
1.021(1.012–1.030) † | 1.022(1.012–1.033) † | 1.021(1.011–1.032) † |
p<0.05
p<0.01.
Adjusted for demographic variables including age (year), gender, education level (year), residence in basic model,
Further adjusted physical activity, vision, waist circumference, type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular diseases and stroke and cerebrovascular diseases in final model.
DBP diastolic blood pressure; MAP mean arterial pressure, PP pulse pressure, SBP systolic blood pressure.
Table 3.
Variables | Unadjusted | Basic model‡ | Final model§ |
---|---|---|---|
SBP | |||
Low(SBP<127mmHg, n=2324) | 1.25(1.12–1.41)† | 1.15(1.00–1.32)* | 1.18(1.02–1.35)* |
Medium-low (127≤ SBP<141mmHg, n=2409) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
Medium-high (141≤ SBP<155mmHg, n=1284) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
High(SBP≥155mmHg, n=1127) | 1.21(1.02–1.42)* | 1.18(0.99–1.42) | 1.23(1.02–1.49)* |
DBP | |||
Low(DBP<77mmHg, n=2536) | 1.33(1.19–1.49)† | 1.20(1.05–1.36)† | 1.20(1.05–1.37)† |
Medium-low (77≤ DBP<85mmHg, n=2644) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
Medium-High (85≤ DBP<90mmHg, n=989) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
High(DBP≥90mmHg, n=975) | 1.26(1.07–1.50)† | 1.26(1.04–1.52)* | 1.24(1.02–1.52)* |
PP | |||
Low(PP<49mmHg, n=2523) | 1.18(1.05–1.32)† | 1.08(0.95–1.23) | 1.09(0.95–1.25) |
Medium-low (49≤ PP<62mmHg, n=2588) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
Medium-high (62≤ PP<73mmHg, n=1035) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
High (PP≥73mmHg, n=998) | 1.04(0.87–1.24) | 0.90(0.74–1.10) | 1.04(0.84–1.28) |
MAP | |||
Low(MAP<93mmHg, n=2560) | 1.31(1.17–1.48)† | 1.15(1.00–1.33) | 1.15(1.00–1.34)* |
Medium-low (93≤ MAP<103mmHg, n=2232) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
Medium-high (103≤ MAP<110mmHg, n=1 186) | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference | 1.00 reference |
High (MAP≥1 10mmHg, n=1 166 ) | 1.21(0.99–1.33) | 1.20(0.99–1.44) | 1.22(1.01–1.48)* |
p<0.05
p<0.01.
Adjusted for demographic variables including age (year), gender, education level (year), residence in basic model, §Further adjusted physical activity, vision, waist circumference, type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular diseases and stroke and cerebrovascular diseases in final model.
DBP diastolic blood pressure; MAP mean arterial pressure, PP pulse pressure, SBP systolic blood pressure.
In contrast with findings below the cut-points, the risk of cognitive impairment was greater with each higher unit of SBP among the participants whose SBP levels were above the cut-points. Each 1mm Hg higher SBP corresponded to a 1.2% greater risk of cognitive impairment in the univariate and multivariate models (Table 2). Compared with medium-high SBP group, participants with high SBP level had a 23% higher risk of cognitive impairment in the final model (OR, 1.23; 95% CI, 1.02–1.49) (Table 3).
DBP and cognitive impairment
Similar to the relationship seen for SBP, for DBP below the cut-points of 85 mm Hg the increased risk of cognitive impairment was also found with lower DBP, there was a 1.1% greater risk of cognitive impairment with each 1mm Hg lower DBP after full adjustment (OR, 1.011; 95% CI, 1.003–1.020). Compared with medium-low DBP group, participants with low DBP levels had a 33% higher risk of cognitive impairment before adjustment (OR, 1.33; 95% CI, 1.19–1.49), and the association remained after the adjustment.
Above the cut-point of 85 mm Hg, when DBP was taken as a continuous variable, there was a 1.8% greater risk of cognitive impairment with each 1mm Hg increment of DBP before adjustment and full adjustment (Table 2). When higher DBP was stratified into two equal groups, participants with high DBP levels had a 24% higher risk of cognitive impairment compared with medium-high DBP group in the final model (OR, 1.24; 95% CI, 1.02–1.52).
PP and cognitive impairment
Although we found a U-shaped association between cognitive impairment and PP in the GAMs models (Figure 1), quadratic models and as a continuous variable in the univariate logistic model, it was non-significant based on multivariate logistic regression (Table 2, Table 3).
MAP and cognitive impairment
As shown in GAMs and in quadratic models, the U-shaped association was confirmed in the logistic regression model, that is, each 1 mm Hg lower MAP corresponded to a 1.1% greater risk in cognitive impairment below the cut-points of 103 mm Hg, and each 1 mm Hg higher MAP corresponded to a 2.1% greater risk in cognitive impairment above the cut-points of 103 mm Hg (Table 2). Poorer cognitive performance was found in those with low MAP level compared to those with medium-low MAP level, and in those with high MAP level compared to those with medium-high MAP level (Table 3)
Discussion
U-shaped association between blood pressure and cognitive impairment
In this cross-sectional study of 7144 Chinese elderly aged 65 years and older, we observed U-shaped associations between blood pressure (SBP, DBP, and MAP) and cognitive impairment, with extremes of blood pressures associated with poorer cognitive performance after controlling for demographic, lifestyle and biological confounding factors. This association was less evident for PP as the U-shaped relationship identified in the GAMs, quadratic models and univariate logistic models, Previous studies have provided inconsistent results, indicating linear, J-shaped or no association [2–9]. Notably, one proposed explanation was that linear function based on experience rather than the natural shape between blood pressure and cognitive impairment may not adequately describe the association.
Several previous reports are consistent with our finding of a U-shaped association of blood pressure with cognitive impairment or dementia [10–14]. In a cross-sectional study conducted in a biracial community, the highest cognitive function scores were found in the elderly aged 65 and older whose SBP was at the mean of 140 mm Hg, or in the persons whose DBP was at the mean of 77 mm Hg [10]. Similarly, this phenomenon was also confirmed in a 9-year longitudinal study in which a U-shaped association with higher risk of cognitive impairment among individuals with higher (≥160 mm Hg) and lower SBP (<130 mm Hg), as well as higher DBP (≥80mm Hg) and lower DBP (<70mm Hg) [12]. This was somewhat consistent for the very elderly, aged 75 and older, where SBP was significantly associated with MMSE in a U-shaped fashion and there was also a linear relationship with PP but no association with DBP [13]. What is distinctive in the current study is that it includes a large sample of the very elderly, and applies GAMs and the quadratic models identify cut-points that more naturally describe the shape of the relationship between blood pressure and cognitive impairment.
High PP in older adults is recognized as a marker of increased artery stiffness and widespread atherosclerosis [32], and may lead to decreased perfusion of the cerebral white matter and ultimately increase the risk for cognitive impairment as seen in some previous studies [21–23]. A U-shaped association between PP and cognitive impairment found in a previous study [20], was also found in GAMs, quadratic models and univariate logistic models, but not confirmed when logistic models were further performed in our study.
It has been speculated that low or high blood pressure may be an early manifestation of the same pathological changes that lead to cognitive impairment, that is, damage to the brain regions that regulate blood pressure [33]. The association between blood pressure and cognitive impairment is problematic, and may be an effect of intermediate confounding factors affecting both blood pressure and cognitive performance such as weight loss and dietary change [34,35], or an effect of shared risk factors such as psychological stress and oxidative stress [36–38]. Another hypothesis is the healthy survivor bias. Probably, some people whose blood pressure was too low or too high had died before aged 65 years due to a window of susceptibility to related diseases (such as hemorrhagic or ischemic stroke) in middle age, thus, the elderly in our study were a selected middle blood pressure population whose health status was better, including a better cognitive performance.
Notably, the explanations and mechanisms of how both higher blood pressure and lower blood pressure impairs cognitive function are likely to be different.
High blood pressure was associated with cognitive impairment
The association between hypertension and cognitive performance may be atherosclerosis, cerebral hypoperfusion and white matter lesions [39–43]. Hypertension is a powerful risk factor for atherosclerosis throughout the arterial tree [40], which leads to redistribution of blood flow and further decrease in cerebral perfusion [41]. This phenomenon has been shown to be exceedingly important for cerebral metabolism and cognition [42]. Additionally, studies have indicated relationships between abnormalities of the white matter and hypertension [43], and such white matter lesions are frequently associated with cognitive impairment [42].
Abnormalities of the renin angiotensin system have also been involved in the pathogenesis of both hypertension and cognitive impairment. Also, in recent years antihypertensive drugs have been used in the treatment and prevention of Alzheimer’s disease via the renin-angiotensin system [44]. It is also hypothesized that hypoperfusion-hypoxia might accelerate cognitive impairment through inducing expression of vasoactive mediators such as nitric oxide, hypoxia-inducible-factor-1α, and vascular endothelial growth factor [45].
Low blood pressure was associated with cognitive impairment
For obvious reasons, low blood pressure can cause cerebral hypoperfusion [46]. It had also been argued that decline in blood pressure may reduce the velocity of cerebral blood flow, which can further cause damage to neural tissue and add to the pathological mechanisms of cognitive impairment. Reduced cerebral blood flow has been found both in individuals with dementia and in those showing early signs of dementia [46, 47]. Furthermore, it has been proposed that severe atherosclerosis resulting from long-standing low blood pressure in midlife and later-life may lead to cerebral hypoperfusion, which may be two biological pathways [48]. The review by Duschek and colleagues suggested the mechanisms of hypotension for reducing cognitive performance include diminished cortical activity, deficient regulation of cerebral blood flow in persons with low blood pressure, and insufficient adjustment of blood flow to cognitive requirements [49]. However, most studies focus on possible mechanisms of cognitive impairment caused by low blood pressure; we haven’t found related researches on the mechanism of lower blood pressure within the normotensive range.
Limitations
Our study has several limitations. First, it is not possible to assess the direction of the association since both exposure and outcome are assessed simultaneously. Second, the blood pressure measurement took place at a single visit and missing data on use of antihypertensive medications may diminish the data accuracy. Third, our findings may not be applicable to younger populations because CLHLS over-sampled the oldest-old and dynamics of blood pressure changes with age. Finally, weight loss and dietary change, psychological stress and an oxidative stress may be confounding factors, but they were not measured and controlled in this study.
Conclusions
Our results suggest a U-shaped association between blood pressure (including SBP, DBP, MAP) and cognitive functions, that is, both higher and lower levels of blood pressure were associated with higher risk of cognitive impairment, which may have several implications for clinical practice and cognitive impairment prevention. First, both lower and higher blood pressure may be used to identify the high-risk population for cognitive impairment. Furthermore, it may point to the need to individualize blood pressure management for cognitive impairment prevention, that is, we should not only address blood pressure-lowering therapy in elderly with hypertension, but may also focus on raising blood pressure in elderly with relatively lower blood pressure. Future prospective longitudinal studies with blood pressure monitoring and extensive cognitive testing are needed to confirm the association we found in this study.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the staff of local county Centers for Disease control and Prevention in the study sites for collecting study data, and thank study participants.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grants 71110107025, 81273160 and 81573247) the National Institute of Health/National Institute of Aging (grant number RO1AG023627), the Claude D. Pepper Older Americans Independence Centers (grant number 5P30 AG028716 from NIA), and Singapore Translational Research Investigator Award grant from the National Medical Research Council.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of Interest: None.
Ethical Approval: Informed consent was obtained from all participants and/or their relatives, and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Peking University.
References
- 1.Abete P, Della-Morte D, Gargiulo G, et al. Cognitive impairment and cardiovascular diseases in the elderly. A heart-brain continuum hypothesis. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;18:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Albert M, et al. Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:1218–1227. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Skoog I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, et al. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and dementia. Lancet. 1996;347:1141–1145. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90608-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Paran E, Anson O, Reuveni H. Blood pressure and cognitive functioning among independent elderly. Am J Hypertens. 2003;16:818–826. doi: 10.1016/s0895-7061(03)01005-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Molander L, Gustafson Y, Lövheim H. Longitudinal associations between blood pressure and dementia in the very old. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:269–276. doi: 10.1159/000320252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gelber RP, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, Masaki KH, Launer LJ, White LR. Antihypertensive medication use and risk of cognitive impairment: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurology. 2013;81:888–895. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a351d4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Anderson C, Teo K, Gao P, et al. Renin-angiotensin system blockade and cognitive function in patients at high risk of cardiovascular disease: Analysis of data from the ONTARGET and TRANSCEND studies. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:43–53. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70250-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, Glynn RJ, Bennett DA, Evans DA. Association of incident Alzheimer disease and blood pressure measured from 13 years before to 2 years after diagnosis in a large community study. JAMA Neurol. 2001;58:1640–1646. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.10.1640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Posner HB, Tang MX, Luchsinger J, Lantigua R, Stern Y, Mayeux R. The relationship of hypertension in the elderly to AD, vascular dementia, and cognitive function. Neurology. 2002;58:1175–1181. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.8.1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, et al. Association between Blood Pressure and Cognitive Function in a Biracial Community Population of Older Persons. Neuroepidemiology. 2002;21:123–130. doi: 10.1159/000054809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Waldstein SR, Giggey PP, Thayer JF, Zonderman AB. Nonlinear relations of blood pressure to cognitive function: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension. 2005;45:374–379. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000156744.44218.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Glynn RJ, Beckett LA, Hebert LE, Morris MC, Scherr PA, Evans DA. Current and remote blood pressure and cognitive decline. JAMA. 1999;281:438–445. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.5.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Molander L, Gustafson Y, Lövheim H. Low blood pressure is associated with cognitive impairment in very old people. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;29:335–341. doi: 10.1159/000289821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Thorvaldsson V, Skoog I, Hofer SM, et al. Nonlinear Blood Pressure Effects on Cognition in Old Age: Separating Between-Person and Within-Person Associations. Psychol Aging. 2011;27:375–383. doi: 10.1037/a0025631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Waldstein SR, Katzel LI. Interactive relations of central versus total obesity and blood pressure to cognitive function. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30:201–207. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gunstad J, Keary TA, Spitznagel MB, et al. Blood pressure and cognitive function in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Int J Neurosci. 2009;119:2228–2242. doi: 10.3109/00207450903139713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bohannon AD, Fillenbaum GG, Pieper CF, Hanlon JT, Blazer DG. Relationship of race/ethnicity and blood pressure to change in cognitive function. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:424–429. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Franklin SS, Khan SA, Wong ND, Larson MG, Levy D. Is pulse pressure useful in predicting risk for coronary heart Disease? The Framingham heart study. Circulation. 1999;100:354–360. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.100.4.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Franklin SS, Gustin W, Wong ND, et al. Hemodynamic patterns of age-related changes in blood pressure. The Framingham Heart Study. Circulation. 1997;96:308–315. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.96.1.308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Qiu C, Winblad B, Viitanen M, Fratiglioni L. Pulse pressure and risk of Alzheimer disease in persons aged 75 years and older: a community-based, longitudinal study. Stroke. 2003;34:594–599. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000060127.96986.F4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Obisesan TO, Obisesan OA, Martins S, et al. High Blood Pressure, Hypertension, and High Pulse Pressure Are Associated with Poorer Cognitive Function in Persons Aged 60 and Older: The Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:501–509. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01592.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Waldstein SR, Rice SC, Thayer JF, Najjar SS, Scuteri A, Zonderman AB. Pulse pressure and pulse wave velocity are related to cognitive decline in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension. 2008;51:99–104. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.093674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Leea AY, Jeonga SH, Choib BH, Sohna EH, Chuic H. Pulse pressure correlates with leukoaraiosis in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gerontol. Geriatr. 2006;42:157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2005.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Freitag MH, Peila R, Masaki K, et al. Midlife pulse pressure and incidence of dementia: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Stroke. 2006;37:33–37. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000196941.58869.2d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sesso HD, Stampfer MJ, Rosner B, et al. Systolic and diastolic blood pressure, pulse pressure, and mean arterial pressure as predictors of cardiovascular disease risk in Men. Hypertension. 2000;36:801–807. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.36.5.801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Avanzini F, Alli C, Boccanelli A, et al. High pulse pressure and low mean arterial pressure: two predictors of death after a myocardial infarction. J Hypertens. 2006;24:2377–2385. doi: 10.1097/01.hjh.0000251897.40002.bf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zeng Y. Towards deeper research and better policy for healthy aging --using the unique data of Chinese longitudinal healthy longevity survey. China Economic J. 2012;5:131–149. doi: 10.1080/17538963.2013.764677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Yin Z, Shi X, Kraus VB, et al. Gender-dependent association of body mass index and waist circumference with disability in the chinese oldest old. Obesity. 2014;22:1918–1925. doi: 10.1002/oby.20775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Katzman R, Zhang M, Ouang-Ya-Qu, et al. A Chinese version of the mini-mental state examination; Impact of illiteracy in a Shanghai dementia survey. J Clin. Epidemiol. 1988;41:971–978. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(88)90034-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Moore DJ, Palmer BW, Jeste DV. Use of the Mini-Mental State Exam in Middle-Aged and Older Outpatients With Schizophrenia : Cognitive Impairment and Its Associations. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;12:412–419. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.12.4.412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tombaugh TN, Mcintyre NJ. The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:922–935. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mitchell GF, Vasan RS, Keyes MJ, et al. Pulse pressure and risk of new-onset atrial fibrillation. JAMA. 2007;297:709–715. doi: 10.1001/jama.297.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Burke WJ, Coronado PG, Schmitt CA, Gillespie KM, Chung HD. Blood pressure regulation in alzheimer’s disease. J Auton Nerv Syst. 1994;48:65–71. doi: 10.1016/0165-1838(94)90160-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gale CR, Martyn CN, Cooper C. Cognitive impairment and mortality in a cohort of elderly people. BMJ. 1996;312:608–611. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7031.608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.White H, Pieper C, Schmader K, Fillenbaum G. Weight Change in Alzheimer’s Disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44:265–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb00912.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Carroll D, Phillips AC, Der G, Hunt K, Benzeval M. Blood pressure reactions to acute mental stress and future blood pressure status: data from the 12-year follow-up of the West of Scotland Study. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:737–742. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182359808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Marin MF, Lord C, Andrews J, et al. Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;96:583–595. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kim HA, Miller AA, Drummond GR, et al. Vascular cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: role of cerebral hypoperfusion and oxidative stress. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol. 2012;385:953–959. doi: 10.1007/s00210-012-0790-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bos D, Vernooij MW, de Bruijn RF, et al. Atherosclerotic calcification is related to a higher risk of dementia and cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;11:639–647. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.05.1758. e631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Arnett DK, Boland LL, Evans GW, et al. Hypertension and arterial stiffness: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Hypertens. 2000;13:317–323. doi: 10.1016/s0895-7061(99)00281-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.de la Torre JC, Stefano GB. Evidence that Alzheimer’s disease is a microvascular disorder: the role of constitutive nitric oxide. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;34:119–136. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(00)00043-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gurol ME. Cerebral hypoperfusion and white matter disease in healthy elderly and patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurol. 2013;20:214–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03865.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Verhaaren BF, Vernooij MW, Renske B, et al. High Blood Pressure and Cerebral White Matter Lesion Progression in the General Population. Hypertension. 2013;61:1354–1359. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.00430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kehoe PG, Wilcock GK. Is inhibition of the renin-angiotensin system a new treatment option for Alzheimer’s disease? Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:373–378. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70077-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Takeda S, Sato N, Ogihara T, Morishita R. The renin-angiotensin system, hypertension and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: new therapeutic potential. Front Biosci. 2008;13:2253–2265. doi: 10.2741/2839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kalback W, Esh C, Castaño EM, et al. Atherosclerosis, vascular amyloidosis and brain hypoperfusion in the pathogenesis of sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol Res. 2004;26(5):525–539. doi: 10.1179/016164104225017668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, Foley D, Havlik RJ. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA. 1995;274:1846–1851. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:487–499. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70141-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Duschek S, Schandry R. Reduced brain perfusion and cognitive performance due to constitutional hypotension. Clin Auton Res. 2007;17:69–76. doi: 10.1007/s10286-006-0379-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]