Abstract
Background
Little is known about patient factors that might influence outcomes of tinnitus interventions. Determining such factors would offer insights into why some individuals benefit from tinnitus intervention whereas others do not.
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to evaluate selected patient factors that may be associated with outcomes of tinnitus intervention. Factors studied include demographics, tinnitus characteristics, psychoacoustic tinnitus measures, audiometric data, and overall physical/emotional health status.
Research Design
A retrospective analysis was performed on data obtained from a controlled clinical study that compared factors associated with tinnitus relief after tinnitus masking and tinnitus retraining therapy.
Study Sample
A total of 126 military veterans participated in this controlled clinical study. Of these, 89 completed outcome measures at both baseline and 12 mo and were included in the present analysis.
Data Collection and Analysis
A “responder” to intervention was identified as having a decrease (improvement) of 20 or more points on the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory between baseline and 12 mo. A “nonresponder” did not achieve a 20-point improvement on the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory. Individual patient factors were examined using independent t-tests or χ2 analysis. A logistic regression model was used to determine how well each factor predicted treatment outcome (responder or nonresponder) while controlling for each of the other factors.
Results
Five patient factors were significantly different (p ≤ 0.05) between responders and nonresponders. Responders tended to (1) be younger in age; (2) have better low-frequency hearing sensitivity; (3) have greater problems with overall hearing; (4) be more likely to have tinnitus for shorter durations; and (5) perceive their tinnitus to be located “in the head” versus “in the ears.” A logistic regression was then performed to determine how well each factor predicted the treatment outcome (responder versus nonresponder) while controlling for each of the other factors. Results from the logistic regression revealed two of the five factors, localization of tinnitus and self-report of hearing problems, to be statistically significant.
Conclusions
Examining the association of individual patient factors to a specific tinnitus intervention yielded several significant findings. Although these findings are not definitive, they reveal the capability that exists to perform these kinds of analyses to investigate relationships between individual patient characteristics and outcomes of intervention for tinnitus. Prospective research using systematic approaches is needed to identify these relationships that would contribute toward the ability to differentially predict outcomes of various tinnitus interventions. Obtaining this information would lead to more targeted therapy and ultimately more effective intervention.
Keywords: Tinnitus, logistic regression, psychoacoustics, treatment
INTRODUCTION
Tinnitus is the perception of sound in the absence of external acoustic stimuli and is experienced by 10–15% of the adult population (Hoffman and Reed, 2004). Only approximately 20% of those who experience tinnitus consider it to be a significant problem (Jastreboff and Hazell, 1998; Davis and Refaie, 2000). A great variety of tinnitus interventions are available for clinicians to use; however, most of the methods have not been verified by controlled clinical trials (Dobie, 2004a; Hobson et al, 2010; Martinez-Devesa et al, 2010). It is likely that no one form of intervention is effective for all patients with tinnitus, i.e., individual characteristics and circumstances may predispose patients to respond differentially to the available interventions (Tyler et al, 2008). The present study was conducted to examine patient variables as possible predictors of outcomes of intervention for tinnitus.
Identifying factors associated with clinical outcomes falls under the category of prognostic factor research (Riley et al, 2013). One aspect of such research is to inform clinical treatment recommendations. Randomized clinical trials (RCTs) are the “gold standard” for the evaluation of different therapies (Gallin and Ognibene, 2007). Numerous RCTs have been conducted to evaluate different tinnitus interventions (Tyler et al, 2007; Hesser et al, 2011; Hoare et al, 2011). If factors that predict prognosis are known, then participants in an RCT for tinnitus can be stratified accordingly (Dobie, 2004a). Because such factors are not known, RCTs for tinnitus may not achieve prognostic balance among treatment groups, which can result in both false-positive and false-negative errors. The lack of known prognostic factors also confounds efforts to compare results of RCTs. For example, participants in one study might be amenable to treatment, whereas those in another study might be more resistant to treatment. Outcomes of such studies cannot be directly compared because of the different prognostic categories. Because of these concerns, Dobie (2004a) recommended, “Until we know more about prognostic variables, tinnitus RCTs should be limited to patients who are unlikely to improve spontaneously: those with severe, long-standing tinnitus.” (p. 270)
In an attempt to predict outcomes of tinnitus therapies, some researchers have examined aspects of the tinnitus (e.g., psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus characteristics) and of individuals presenting with tinnitus (e.g., psychological factors) to better understand what factors might lead to increased tinnitus-related distress. Results have varied, but offer some insight into possible predictors of tinnitus-related distress and treatment outcomes. Jastreboff et al (1994) investigated possible predictive capabilities of tinnitus psychoacoustic testing for treatment outcomes. Results were consistent with previous research in that none of the initial psychoacoustic measures correlated with outcomes. Measures of tinnitus maskability (minimal amount of noise required to render the tinnitus perception inaudible), however, were seen to decrease for patients whose tinnitus condition improved, and to increase for patients whose condition either worsened or remained the same. The authors concluded that measures of tinnitus maskability could be useful for monitoring the effectiveness of tinnitus treatment, but that initial psychoacoustic measures were not useful for predicting outcomes. Andersson et al (2001) examined whether baseline tinnitus psychoacoustic testing could be used to predict long-term tinnitus distress, finding that tinnitus maskability was predictive of later distress. In a review of psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus, Henry and Meikle (2000) noted that studies have consistently shown poor correlations between these measures and subjective ratings of tinnitus severity. Further research is needed to determine if tinnitus maskability is predictive of outcomes or if it correlates with outcomes.
In addition to the study by Jastreboff et al (1994), other studies have focused on prognostic factors for predicting outcome with tinnitus retraining therapy (TRT). Studies by both Herraiz et al (2007) and Koizumi et al (2009) found that patients had better results with TRT if, before receiving treatment, they reported higher levels of tinnitus loudness and greater distress levels. Ariizumi et al (2010) reported that lower tinnitus loudness and a positive attitude were predictive of favorable results with TRT.
Kleinjung et al (2007) reported tinnitus duration and degree of hearing loss to be factors associated with responsiveness to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Specifically, individuals with normal hearing and shorter duration of tinnitus were more likely to respond to TMS as a treatment of tinnitus.
Kröner-Herwig et al (2006) examined whether the presence of specific patient variables, including mental disorders, dysfunctional cognitions relating to tinnitus, and other variables were associated with reduced benefit in one of two tinnitus interventions: cognitive-behavioral tinnitus coping training, or habituation-based training. None of these variables had predictive capabilities with respect to the two interventions.
Kaldo-Sandström et al (2004) evaluated predictors of outcome for patients who participated in Internet-based cognitive-behavioral therapy. The only patient factor associated with outcome was the number of earlier treatments of tinnitus. Graul et al (2008) studied patient factors for “responders” and “nonresponders” to a multimodal cognitive-behavioral inpatient treatment. Factors that were different between these two groups were age and the extent of psychosocial stress.
Although limited in their scope, all of these studies contributed to the available knowledge as to why some individuals respond better to tinnitus interventions than others. The present study further explored this avenue of research, hypothesizing that multiple relationships exist between patient factors (i.e., demographics, tinnitus characteristics, psychoacoustic tinnitus measures, hearing ability, and overall physical and emotional health) and tinnitus treatment outcomes. The goal of this study was to retrospectively identify patient factors associated with differential responsiveness to tinnitus intervention. A secondary goal was to bring awareness regarding the diversity of approaches used to examine patient factors and outcomes of tinnitus treatment.
METHODS
Data were analyzed retrospectively from a controlled clinical study that was conducted at the National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research to evaluate the clinical efficacy of tinnitus masking (TM) and TRT (Henry et al, 2006). TM and TRT both provide “sound therapy,” but they differ in their approach (Henry et al, 2002). TM primarily uses ear-level sound generators (“maskers”) to induce an immediate sense of relief from tinnitus (Vernon and Meikle, 2000). TRT is a counseling-based technique that also makes use of ear-level sound generators for patients with more severe tinnitus (Jastreboff and Jastreboff, 2004). Sound therapy with TRT is intended to promote habituation to the tinnitus, and not to induce a sense of relief. The counseling component of TRT is fairly extensive, and uses the “neurophysiologic model” as a guide to describe the underlying causes of tinnitus distress and therefore to “demystify” the tinnitus. TM also involves counseling but in a less structured manner and mainly focuses on how to use maskers to achieve relief. Differences between TM and TRT have been described in detail (Henry et al, 2002; Henry et al, 2006).
The study from which the present data were analyzed (Henry et al, 2006) used the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI) (Newman et al, 1996) as the primary instrument to assess outcomes. THI data were collected at 0, 3, 6, 12, and 18 mo. Psychoacoustic and audiometric testing were performed at baseline and then every 6 mo (baseline, 6, 12, and 18 mo). Both groups showed significant reduction (improvement) in THI scores with TRT showing relatively greater effects than TM at 12 and 18 mo.
Participants
Of the 126 participants enrolled in the controlled study, 89 completed the THI at both baseline and at 12 mo and were included in the present analysis. These included 44 males (mean age = 59.0 yr; standard deviation [SD] =10.7) in the TRT group, and 41 males and four females (mean age = 61.0 yr; SD = 9.9) in the TM group. Mean age was not significantly different between groups (p = 0.37), nor was the duration of tinnitus (p = 0.92).
Primary Outcome Measure
The THI is a 25-item outcome measure that determines the level of self-perceived handicap caused by tinnitus, based on a 0–100 increasing handicap scale (with 100 being total handicap and 0 being no handicap) (Newman et al, 1996). The THI aids the clinician in identifying patients who would benefit from tinnitus-specific intervention. Regarding demographic data, the THI shows no statistically significant differences for age or gender. The THI has been validated psychometrically, showing high internal consistency (α= 0.93) and high test-retest reliability for each subscale: Functional, Emotional, Catastrophic (r =0.94, 0.88, and 0.84, respectively) (Newman et al, 1998).
Determining Responder versus Nonresponder
Different methods exist to determine whether a clinically significant change has occurred after treatment (suggestive of treatment benefit or responsiveness). For example, Norman et al (2003) established the threshold to detect changes in health-related quality of life for chronic diseases to be half of an SD. For the current study, the main outcome measure was the THI. Newman et al (1998) evaluated the test-retest reliability of the THI and determined that a reduction (between test and retest) of at least 20 points was necessary for tinnitus therapy to be considered effective based on the 95% confidence intervals associated with a significant change in perceived handicap. The current study used this criterion (change of at least 20 points on the THI) to establish “responders” to tinnitus treatment (either TM or TRT). Individuals who did not reach that criterion were classified as “nonresponders.”
The outcome time point used for this analysis was 12 mo. It was important to use a time point that would allow for responsiveness to treatment to occur. Using a time point earlier than 12 mo might have been too soon for any change—improvement or otherwise—to be detected. Selecting a time point later than 12 mo, such as the 18 mo time point, risked reducing the number of subject data available because of loss of follow-up, a common concern in longitudinal studies.
Individual Patient Factors
The variables most often examined in previous related studies include sociodemographic information, self-reported hearing loss, localization of tinnitus (individual ear versus in the head), and tinnitus loudness (Andersson et al, 2001; Langenbach et al, 2005; Kröner-Herwig et al, 2006; Wallhäusser-Franke et al, 2012). Each of these variables was considered when determining which patient factors to select for analysis. Audiometric and psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus have also been examined for their potential to predict treatment responsiveness and therefore were included in the present study. Selected factors were grouped in the following categories: demographics, self-reported tinnitus characteristics, psychoacoustic tinnitus measures, audiometric measures, self-reported hearing and sound tolerance concerns, physical health status, and emotional health status.
Demographics
Four demographic factors were examined: age, marital status, employment, and education. For all factors but age, the distribution of response categories was skewed with few participants in some of the categories; those factors were dichotomized to increase the likelihood of detecting significant differences: (1) Age was analyzed as continuous data. (2) Marital status was collected categorically with respect to married, single, divorced, widowed, or not married (these groups were collapsed into dichotomous categories, married and not married). (3) Participants reported their employment in terms of retired, employed, unemployed, or part-time (these were collapsed into employed and unemployed). (4) Participants were asked to report their education in the following categories: less than 9th grade, 9th–12th grade, high school graduate, some college, or college graduate (these categories were collapsed into two groups: high school or less and post high school education).
Self-Reported Tinnitus Characteristics
Eight self-reported tinnitus characteristics were examined: tinnitus loudness; tinnitus duration; perceived localization of tinnitus; number of tinnitus sounds; tinnitus onset sudden or gradual; fluctuation in tinnitus loudness; which is worse—hearing loss, tinnitus, or both; and strength of problem associated with tinnitus. Participants were asked to (1) rate the loudness of their tinnitus on a scale of 1–10, with 1 being very soft and 10 being very loud; (2) describe their tinnitus duration as occurring for less than 1 yr; 1–2 yr; 2–5 yr; 5–10 yr; 10–20 yr and more than 20 yr (these categories were collapsed into two groups: ≤20 and >20 yr); (3) characterize the localization of their tinnitus as being in the ears (right, left, or both) or in the head (right side, left side, or fills head); (4) report the number of tinnitus sounds as 1, 2, or 3 or more (these three categories were collapsed into two: one sound and two or more sounds); (5) report their tinnitus as having a sudden or gradual onset; (6) report the amount of fluctuation in tinnitus loudness as daily, weekly, monthly, rarely, or never (these were collapsed into two categories: daily or less than daily); (7) rate which is more of a problem—tinnitus, hearing, or both (only two participants reported hearing to be a bigger problem than tinnitus; therefore, categories were collapsed into tinnitus the worse problem, and tinnitus and hearing equally problematic); (8) rate the severity of their tinnitus as being a small, moderate, big, or very big problem (these were collapsed into a small to moderate problem and a big to very big problem).
Psychoacoustic Tinnitus Measures
Three psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus were selected: tinnitus loudness matching, minimal masking level (MML), and residual inhibition (RI); these measures are frequently used as part of a tinnitus assessment (Henry et al, 2005). For loudness matching, participants were asked to match the loudness of their tinnitus to the loudness of pure tones at audiometric frequencies. Loudness matching has been shown to be a reliable measure (Henry et al, 1999). MML is the lowest level at which broadband noise renders the tinnitus perception inaudible. MML was measured to the closest 1 dB and reported in dB sensation level (Henry et al, 2006). As mentioned in the Introduction section, MMLs have been shown to correlate with treatment outcomes; specifically, a reduction in the MML has correlated with successful treatment (Jastreboff et al, 1994). Residual inhibition (RI) is defined as the temporary suppression (partial RI) or elimination (complete RI) of tinnitus after an auditory stimulus (Vernon, 1982; Vernon and Meikle, 1988). For this study, RI was measured immediately after MML testing (Henry et al, 2006). A broadband noise was presented bilaterally for 1 min at 10 dB above the MML. When the stimulus was terminated, participants were asked to report if their tinnitus had changed. RI was analyzed as the number of seconds that either partial or complete RI occurred.
Audiometric Measures
Several audiometric measures were examined: low-frequency pure-tone average (LF-PTA; 0.5, 1, or 2 kHz); high-frequency PTA (HF-PTA; 3, 4, 6, or 8 kHz); and speech reception threshold (SRT). Audiometric testing was performed using calibrated equipment in a conventional sound-treated booth meeting established standards (American National Standards Institute, 1991). The LF-PTA and HF-PTA were computed separately for the left and right ears. SRTs were obtained in dB hearing level separately for the right and left ears.
Self-Report Hearing and Sound Tolerance Concerns
Factors related to participants’ self-reported hearing ability included problem with sound tolerance (yes/no); use of hearing aids (yes/no); and self-reported hearing problem (none, mild, moderate, or severe). Categories for self-reported hearing problem were collapsed into two categories: mild to moderate problem and big to very big problem.
Physical Health Status
Four health status factors were examined: (1) number of coexisting health conditions; (2) self-rated health (compared with peers as “worse than people my age,” “same as people my age,” or “better than people my age”); (3) admitted as a patient in a hospital during the past 6 mo; and (4) received treatment at an emergency room or urgent care center during the past 6 mo. Responses for the last two factors were never, one time only, two to three times, or more than three times (these categories were collapsed into two groups: never and one or more times).
Emotional Health Status
Participants were administered the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, 1983) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al, 1961). The raw scores from the STAI and BDI were collected and analyzed to determine if there was any association between these two measures and tinnitus treatment outcomes.
Data Analysis
Bivariate Analysis
Comparisons were initially made between responders and nonresponders for each treatment group separately (TM versus TRT), using χ2 analysis for categorical factors and analysis of variance for factors with continuous data. Because of small sample sizes, the two treatment groups were then pooled for a secondary analysis to improve statistical power.
Some factors such as ethnicity and gender were excluded from these analyses because of a lack of variability within the participant population. All of the participants were military veterans, mostly male and Caucasian.
Multivariate Analyses
Factors exhibiting or approaching a statistically significant difference (p ≤ 0.05) between responders and nonresponders and demonstrating similar patterns across each treatment group, were further analyzed using multiple logistic regression. A logistic regression model was used to determine how well each factor predicted the treatment outcome (responder or nonresponder) while controlling for each of the other factors. Such a model is conceptually similar to multiple linear regressions inasmuch as statistical relationships between one dependent variable and several independent variables are evaluated. In multiple logistic regression, the dependent variable is binary (in this case, responder or nonresponder), and the actual probability of a participant responding is statistically modeled as an odds ratio.
The statistical analysis was conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences v17.
RESULTS
Comparisons were initially made between responders and nonresponders for each treatment group separately (TM versus TRT). The mean THI score at baseline for the TRT group was 51.0 (SD = 20.0; range = 14–98); for the TM group, it was 52.0 (SD = 25.0; range = 10–94). Mean THI scores at 12 mo were 29.1 (SD = 17.8; range = 0–86) for the TRT group and 43.8 (SD = 26.6; range = 8–98) for the TM group. Tables 1 and 2 show comparisons of responders and nonresponders within each treatment group for continuous and categorical data, respectively. In general, distribution patterns across the two treatments groups were similar, with few exceptions. One exception was that depression and anxiety scores showed opposite results for TRT participants compared with TM participants with respect to treatment outcome. Again, because of the small sample sizes, observed differences between the two intervention groups should be interpreted with caution.
Table 1.
Masking | TRT | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|||||||
N | Mean | SD | p-value | N | Mean | SD | p-value | |
Demographics | ||||||||
Age of Participant (yr) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 32 | 62.41 | 10.16 | 0.143 | 21 | 63.00 | 9.46 | 0.017 |
Responder | 13 | 57.62 | 8.61 | 23 | 55.44 | 10.63 | ||
Self-Reported Tinnitus Characteristics | ||||||||
Tinnitus Loudness | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 31 | 7.66 | 1.37 | 0.334 | 18 | 7.47 | 1.14 | 0.256 |
Responder | 11 | 8.14 | 1.42 | 20 | 8.03 | 1.77 | ||
Psychoacoustic Tinnitus Measurements | ||||||||
Loudness-Match | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 26 | 8.58 | 9.80 | 0.366 | 21 | 11.24 | 10.78 | 0.165 |
Responder | 13 | 12.85 | 19.50 | 21 | 7.38 | 6.34 | ||
RE MML (SL) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 24 | 22.50 | 19.00 | 0.897 | 18 | 23.61 | 22.48 | 0.936 |
Responder | 10 | 23.50 | 23.69 | 17 | 24.12 | 13.14 | ||
LE MML (SL) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 24 | 22.92 | 18.41 | 0.398 | 16 | 24.38 | 17.59 | 0.918 |
Responder | 10 | 29.50 | 24.88 | 17 | 23.82 | 12.81 | ||
RI duration | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 30 | 61.43 | 62.96 | 0.890 | 17 | 71.94 | 45.18 | 0.912 |
Responder | 12 | 64.50 | 69.24 | 21 | 73.86 | 58.10 | ||
Audiometric Measurements/Self-Reported Hearing Ability | ||||||||
RE SRT (dB HL) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 31 | 21.39 | 12.77 | 0.807 | 21 | 24.52 | 14.13 | 0.013 |
Responder | 12 | 20.17 | 18.77 | 21 | 15.19 | 8.29 | ||
LE SRT (dB HL) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 32 | 20.78 | 11.46 | 0.994 | 21 | 22.62 | 13.84 | 0.124 |
Responder | 12 | 20.75 | 15.94 | 21 | 17.00 | 8.80 | ||
RE PTA Low | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 32 | 27.45 | 23.03 | 0.175 | 21 | 26.90 | 13.69 | 0.008 |
Responder | 13 | 17.82 | 15.64 | 23 | 16.38 | 11.38 | ||
LE PTA Low | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 32 | 23.65 | 14.84 | 0.651 | 21 | 27.30 | 16.55 | 0.078 |
Responder | 13 | 21.28 | 18.03 | 23 | 19.49 | 11.94 | ||
RE PTA High | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 27 | 57.92 | 28.99 | 0.379 | 20 | 54.88 | 22.62 | 0.226 |
Responder | 12 | 49.69 | 19.95 | 23 | 47.12 | 18.79 | ||
LE PTA High | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 26 | 59.38 | 23.08 | 0.453 | 20 | 59.94 | 22.82 | 0.212 |
Responder | 12 | 53.44 | 20.89 | 23 | 51.85 | 19.01 | ||
Physical Health Status | ||||||||
No. of Health Problems | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 32 | 2.78 | 1.79 | 0.792 | 21 | 2.14 | 1.80 | 0.095 |
Responder | 13 | 2.62 | 2.14 | 23 | 3.26 | 2.51 | ||
Emotional Health Status | ||||||||
BDI | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 31 | 7.45 | 8.14 | 0.011 | 18 | 3.67 | 4.35 | 0.004 |
Responder | 10 | 3.10 | 2.28 | 21 | 9.38 | 7.05 | ||
STAI (Trait) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 30 | 43.95 | 14.01 | 0.059 | 18 | 35.67 | 11.96 | 0.008 |
Responder | 10 | 34.70 | 9.21 | 21 | 46.88 | 12.82 | ||
STAI (State) | ||||||||
Nonresponder | 30 | 43.51 | 17.67 | 0.063 | 18 | 34.62 | 12.94 | 0.009 |
Responder | 10 | 32.00 | 11.70 | 21 | 46.38 | 13.73 |
Table 2.
Masking | TRT | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|||||||
χ2 | p-value | χ2 | p-value | |||||
Demographics | ||||||||
Marital Status | Married | Not Married | Married | Not Married | ||||
Nonresponder | 23 (72%) | 9 (28%) | 0.27 | 0.608 | 16 (76%) | 5 (24%) | 0.01 | 0.933 |
Responder | 7 (64%) | 4 (36%) | 17 (77%) | 5 (23%) | ||||
Employment Status | Employed | Not Employed | Employed | Not Employed | ||||
Nonresponder | 12 (38%) | 20 (62%) | 0.61 | 0.436 | 7 (35%) | 13 (65%) | 0.96 | 0.327 |
Responder | 3 (25%) | 9 (75%) | 11 (50%) | 11 (50%) | ||||
Level of Education | HS or less | Post HS | HS or less | Post HS | ||||
Nonresponder | 5 (17%) | 25 (83%) | 0.06 | 0.810 | 6 (35%) | 11 (65%) | 0.60 | 0.438 |
Responder | 2 (20%) | 8 (80%) | 5 (24%) | 16 (76%) | ||||
Self-Reported Tinnitus Characteristics | ||||||||
Duration of Tinnitus | ≤20 Years | >20 Years | ≤20 Years | >20 Years | ||||
Nonresponder | 12 (38%) | 20 (62%) | 2.20 | 0.141 | 7 (33%) | 14 (67%) | 2.40 | 0.123 |
Responder | 8 (62%) | 5 (38%) | 13 (57%) | 10 (43%) | ||||
Perceived Localization of Tinnitus | In the Ears | In the Head | In the Ears | In The Head | ||||
Nonresponder | 20 (71%) | 8 (29%) | 2.20 | 0.141 | 14 (67%) | 7 (33%) | 3.30 | 0.068 |
Responder | 5 (19%) | 22 (81%) | 9 (39%) | 14 (61%) | ||||
Number of Tinnitus Sounds | One Sound | Multiple Sounds | One Sound | Multiple Sounds | ||||
Nonresponder | 14 (48%) | 15 (52%) | 1.90 | 0.167 | 13 (68%) | 6 (32%) | 2.20 | 0.141 |
Responder | 2 (22%) | 7 (78%) | 8 (38%) | 10 (63%) | ||||
Tinnitus Onset: Sudden or Gradual | Sudden | Gradual | Sudden | Gradual | ||||
Nonresponder | 7 (26%) | 20 (74%) | 0.69 | 0.406 | 7 (35%) | 13 (65%) | 0.00 | 0.985 |
Responder | 4 (40%) | 6 (60%) | 6 (35%) | 11 (65%) | ||||
Tinnitus Varies in Loudness | Not Daily | Daily | Not Daily | Daily | ||||
Nonresponder | 21 (66%) | 11 (34%) | 1.50 | 0.227 | 10 (50%) | 10 (50%) | 3.40 | 0.065 |
Responder | 6 (46%) | 7 (54%) | 17 (77%) | 5 (23%) | ||||
Degree of Tinnitus Problem | Small-Moderate | Big-Very Big | Small-Moderate | Big-Very Big | ||||
Nonresponder | 11 (35%) | 20 (65%) | 1.80 | 0.182 | 5 (25%) | 15 (75%) | 0.37 | 0.541 |
Responder | 2 (15%) | 11 (85%) | 4 (17%) | 19 (83%) | ||||
Which is worse: | ||||||||
Tinnitus/Hearing | Tinnitus | Tinnitus & Hearing | Tinnitus | Tinnitus & Hearing | ||||
Nonresponder | 10 (37%) | 17 (63%) | 2.40 | 0.121 | 10 (63%) | 6 (37%) | 0.80 | 0.371 |
Responder | 6 (67%) | 3 (33%) | 9 (47%) | 10 (53%) | ||||
Physical Health Status | ||||||||
Self-Rated Health Status | Excellent-Good | Fair-Poor | Excellent-Good | Fair-Poor | ||||
Nonresponder | 15 (47%) | 17 (53%) | 0.18 | 0.672 | 15 (75%) | 5 (25%) | 0.49 | 0.486 |
Responder | 7 (54%) | 6 (46%) | 15 (65%) | 8 (35%) | ||||
Admitted to Hospital Past 6 mo | No | Yes | No | Yes | ||||
Nonresponder | 26 (84%) | 5 (16%) | 0.80 | 0.777 | 14 (74%) | 5 (26%) | 0.90 | 0.342 |
Responder | 8 (80%) | 2 (20%) | 18 (86%) | 3 (14%) | ||||
Admitted to ER Past 6 mo | No | Yes | No | Yes | ||||
Nonresponder | 19 (61%) | 12 (39%) | 0.01 | 0.942 | 13 (68%) | 6 (32%) | 0.01 | 0.915 |
Responder | 6 (60%) | 4 (40%) | 14 (70%) | 6 (30%) | ||||
Self-Reported Hearing & Sound Tolerance Concerns | ||||||||
Decreased Sound Tolerance | No | Yes | No | Yes | ||||
Nonresponder | 9 (28%) | 23 (72%) | 0.43 | 0.514 | 9 (43%) | 12 (57%) | 0.06 | 0.802 |
Responder | 2 (18%) | 9 (82%) | 9 (39%) | 14 (61%) | ||||
Hearing Problems | Mild-Moderate | Big-Very Big | Mild-Moderate | Big-Very Big | ||||
Nonresponder | 18 (60%) | 12 (40%) | 0.06 | 0.812 | 13 (72%) | 5 (28%) | 5.27 | 0.022 |
Responder | 5 (56%) | 4 (44%) | 7 (35%) | 13 (65%) | ||||
Hearing Aid Use | No | Yes | No | Yes | ||||
Nonresponder | 25 (81%) | 6 (19%) | 0.50 | 0.479 | 14 (74%) | 5 (26%) | 0.30 | 0.583 |
Responder | 7 (70%) | 3 (30%) | 17 (81%) | 4 (19%) |
For the pooled treatment group, five patient factors were found to be significantly different (p ≤ 0.05) between responder and nonresponder groups (see Tables 3 and 4). Compared with nonresponders, responders tended to be younger in age (mean age = 56.2 yr, SD = 9.9 yr for responders versus 62.6 yr, SD = 9.8 yr for nonresponders), with better hearing thresholds in the low frequency range (mean = 16.9 dB HL, SD = 12.9 dB HL for responders versus 27.2 dB HL, SD = 19.7 dB HL for nonresponders) but greater problems with overall hearing (59% of responders versus 35% of nonresponders reporting big to very big problems with hearing). Responders also were more likely to have their tinnitus for a shorter duration (58% responders versus 35% of nonresponders with duration of tinnitus ≤20 yr) and to perceive the tinnitus to be in their head versus in their ears (61% of responders versus 36% of nonresponders reported localization of tinnitus to be “in the head”). To determine which of these factors or set of factors best predicted response to treatment, we performed a logistic regression analysis.
Table 3.
N | Mean | SD | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
Age | ||||
Nonresponder | 53 | 62.6 | 9.8 | 0.003* |
Responder | 36 | 56.2 | 9.9 | |
Self-Reported Tinnitus Characteristics | ||||
Self-report of tinnitus loudness | ||||
Nonresponder | 49 | 7.6 | 1.3 | 0.15 |
Responder | 31 | 8.1 | 1.6 | |
Psychoacoustic Tinnitus Measurements | ||||
Tinnitus loudness (loudness match) | ||||
Nonresponder | 47 | 60.9 | 21.6 | 0.79 |
Responder | 34 | 62.3 | 25.6 | |
RE MML (SL) | ||||
Nonresponder | 42 | 23.0 | 20.3 | 0.85 |
Responder | 27 | 24.0 | 17.3 | |
LE MML (SL) | ||||
Nonresponder | 40 | 23.5 | 17.9 | 0.59 |
Responder | 27 | 26.0 | 18.0 | |
RI Duration | ||||
Nonresponder | 36 | 64.2 | 43.6 | 0.15 |
Responder | 26 | 82.5 | 54.8 | |
Audiometric Measurements/Self-Report Hearing Ability | ||||
RE SRT (dB HL) | ||||
Nonresponder | 52 | 22.7 | 13.3 | 0.06 |
Responder | 33 | 17.0 | 13.0 | |
LE SRT (dB HL) | ||||
Nonresponder | 53 | 21.5 | 12.4 | 0.25 |
Responder | 33 | 18.4 | 11.8 | |
RE PTA Low (0.5, 1, 2 kHz) | ||||
Nonresponder | 53 | 27.2 | 19.7 | 0.007* |
Responder | 36 | 16.9 | 12.9 | |
LE PTA Low (0.5, 1, 2 kHz) | ||||
Nonresponder | 53 | 25.1 | 15.5 | 0.13 |
Responder | 36 | 20.1 | 14.2 | |
RE PTA High (3, 4, 6, 8 kHz) | ||||
Nonresponder | 47 | 56.6 | 26.2 | 0.10 |
Responder | 35 | 48.0 | 18.9 | |
LE PTA High (3, 4, 6, 8 kHz) | ||||
Nonresponder | 46 | 59.6 | 22.7 | 0.14 |
Responder | 35 | 52.4 | 19.4 | |
Physical Health Status | ||||
Number of Health Problems | ||||
Nonresponder | 53 | 2.5 | 1.8 | 0.29 |
Responder | 36 | 3 | 2.4 | |
Emotional Health Status | ||||
BDI | ||||
Nonresponder | 49 | 6.1 | 7.2 | 0.42 |
Responder | 31 | 7.4 | 6.6 | |
STAI (Trait) | ||||
Nonresponder | 48 | 40.8 | 13.8 | 0.50 |
Responder | 31 | 42.9 | 13.0 | |
STAI (State) | ||||
Nonresponder | 48 | 40.2 | 16.5 | 0.67 |
Responder | 31 | 41.7 | 14.6 |
p ≤ 0.05 (indicated in bold).
Table 4.
χ2 | p-value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
Marital Status | Married | Not Married | ||
Nonresponder | 39 (74%) | 14 (26%) | 0.01 | 0.56 |
Responder | 24 (73%) | 9 (27%) | ||
Employment Status | Employed | Not Employed | ||
Nonresponder | 19 (37%) | 33 (63%) | 1.61 | 0.45 |
Responder | 14 (41%) | 20 (59%) | ||
Level of Education | HS or less | Post HS | ||
Nonresponder | 31 (66%) | 16 (34%) | 0.22 | 0.42 |
Responder | 22 (71%) | 9 (29%) | ||
Self-Reported Tinnitus Characteristics | ||||
Duration of Tinnitus | ≤20 Years | >20 Years | ||
Nonresponder | n = 19 (36%) | n = 34 (64%) | 4.4 | 0.03* |
Responder | n = 21 (58%) | n = 15 (42%) | ||
Perceived Localization of Tinnitus | In the Ears | In the Head | ||
Nonresponder | n = 34 (64%) | n = 19 (36%) | 5.5 | 0.02* |
Responder | n = 14 (39%) | n = 22 (61%) | ||
Number of Tinnitus Sounds | One Sound | Multiple Sounds | ||
Nonresponder | 27 (56%) | 21 (44%) | 2.6 | 0.09 |
Responder | 10 (37%) | 17 (63%) | ||
Tinnitus Onset: Sudden or Gradual | Sudden | Gradual | ||
Nonresponder | 14 (30%) | 33 (70%) | 0.41 | 0.35 |
Responder | 10 (37%) | 17 (63%) | ||
Tinnitus Varies in Loudness | Not Daily | Daily | ||
Nonresponder | 31 (60%) | 21 (40%) | 0.33 | 0.57 |
Responder | 23 (66%) | 12 (34%) | ||
Degree of Tinnitus Problem | Small-Moderate | Big-Very Big | ||
Nonresponder | 16 (31%) | 35 (69%) | 2.40 | 0.10 |
Responder | 6 (17%) | 30 (83%) | ||
Which is Worse: Tinnitus/Hearing | Tinnitus | Tinnitus & Hearing | ||
Nonresponder | 20 (47%) | 23 (53%) | 0.39 | 0.63 |
Responder | 15 (54%) | 13 (46%) | ||
Physical Health Status | ||||
Self-Rated Health Status | Excellent-Good | Fair-Poor | ||
Nonresponder | 30 (58%) | 22 (42%) | 0.10 | 0.83 |
Responder | 22 (61%) | 14 (39%) | ||
Admitted to Hospital Past 6 mo | No | Yes | ||
Nonresponder | 40 (80%) | 10 (20%) | 0.19 | 0.45 |
Responder | 26 (84%) | 5 (16%) | ||
Admitted to ER Past 6 mo | No | Yes | ||
Nonresponder | 32 (64%) | 18 (36%) | 0.06 | 0.50 |
Responder | 20 (67%) | 10 (33%) | ||
Self-Report Hearing And Sound Tolerance Concerns | ||||
Decreased Sound Tolerance | Yes | No | ||
Nonresponder | 18 (34%) | 35 (66%) | 0.24 | 0.53 |
Responder | 11 (32%) | 23 (68%) | ||
Hearing Problems | Mild-Moderate | Big-Very Big | ||
Nonresponder | 31 (65%) | 17 (35%) | 3.90 | 0.05* |
Responder | 12 (41%) | 17 (59%) | ||
Hearing-Aid Use | No | Yes | ||
Nonresponder | 39 (78%) | 11 (22%) | 0.00 | 0.58 |
Responder | 24 (77%) | 7 (23%) |
Notes: ER = emergency room.
p ≤ 0.05 (indicated in bold).
Logistic Regression Analysis
Results of the logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 5. After controlling for colinearity (the possibility of the predicting factors being correlated among themselves), only two of the five factors were statistically significant at the p ≤ 0.05 level: localization of tinnitus (p = 0.04) and self-report of hearing problems (p = 0.01). Localization of tinnitus had an odds ratio of 3.30, indicating that participants who perceived their tinnitus to be in their head were 3.3 times more likely to respond to treatment compared with those who perceived the tinnitus to be in their ears. Report of hearing problems had an odds ratio of 4.8, indicating that participants who reported big to very big problems with their hearing were 4.8 times more likely to respond to treatment than those who reported mild to moderate problems with their hearing ability. Duration of tinnitus and LF-PTA in the left ear approached significance (p = 0.07), indicating that shorter-duration tinnitus and low-frequency hearing loss may also be potential predictors of treatment success.
Table 5.
Patient Factor | B | SE | Wald | df | p-value | OR | 95% CI for OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
Lower | Upper | |||||||
Age | −0.007 | 0.034 | 0.037 | 1 | 0.85 | 0.99 | 0.93 | 1.06 |
Tinnitus duration (≤20 yr vs. >20 yr) | −1.105 | 0.601 | 3.380 | 1 | 0.07 | 0.33 | 0.10 | 1.10 |
Tinnitus location (in ears vs. in head) | 1.200 | 0.582 | 4.300 | 1 | 0.04** | 3.30 | 1.10 | 10.50 |
LF-PTA (0.5, 1, 2 kHz) right ear | −0.055 | 0.030 | 3.400 | 1 | 0.07 | 0.95 | 0.89 | 1.00 |
Self-report of hearing problem (mild to moderate vs. big or very big problem) | 1.6 | 0.64 | 6.000 | 1 | 0.01** | 4.80 | 1.40 | 16.60 |
Constant | 0.219 | 1.900 | 0.013 | 1 | 0.91 | 1.20 |
Note: CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom; OR = odds ratio; SE = standard error.
p ≤ 0.05 (indicated in bold).
DISCUSSION
This study involved a retrospective analysis of data from a controlled clinical study (Henry et al, 2006). The analysis was conducted to determine if individual patient factors were associated with outcomes of interventions for tinnitus. Results of the analysis supported the hypothesis that certain patient factors would be significantly different between individuals who respond to intervention (20 point or better improvement in THI) compared with those who do not. Patient factors were categorized according to individuals’ demographics, tinnitus characteristics, psychoacoustic tinnitus measures, audiometric data, and overall physical/emotional health status.
A preliminary analysis was performed revealing similar distribution patterns between responders and nonresponders across the two treatment groups. A few exceptions, however, were noted (see Table 2). Overall emotional health (depression and anxiety) differed between the two treatment groups such that TRT participants who had higher depression and anxiety scores at baseline were more likely to respond to treatment, whereas TM participants with high depression and anxiety scores at baseline were less likely to respond to treatment. This finding could be explained as a consequence of clinician variables. More specifically, one treatment specialist conducted TRT and one conducted TM (Henry et al, 2006). These clinicians had different personalities and different patient workloads, which could have affected outcomes. In addition, participants in the TRT group received more clinician contact time than participants in the TM group.
In general, results from the preliminary analysis found essentially no significant differences between responders and nonresponders across the two treatment groups. The few factors that showed opposite findings are of questionable significance because of the small sample size. To overcome this issue, we decided to pool the data from the two treatment groups to increase the statistical power. The preliminary analysis that informed this decision and patient factors that showed opposite results when the groups were analyzed separately (in essence, canceling out possible effects) were not included in the pooled data. Combining the data allowed for analyses to be conducted with sufficient power to identify patient factors predictive of treatment outcome (identify responder versus nonresponder).
On examining the pooled treatment group data, we found five factors to be significantly different between the responders and nonresponders: responders tended to (1) be younger in age; (2) perceive the tinnitus to be located in the head versus in the ears; (3) experience tinnitus for shorter periods (shorter duration of tinnitus); (4) have better low-frequency hearing sensitivity; and (5) have greater overall self-reported problems with hearing. Of these five factors, a logistic regression analysis revealed self-report of hearing problems and perceived tinnitus location to be most significant (p = 0.01 and p = 0.04, respectively; see Table 5). These results suggest that patients who perceive tinnitus to be located in the head and those who self-report more severe hearing problems may be more likely to respond positively to both TRT and TM than those who perceive tinnitus to be located in the ears and who self-report less severe hearing problems.
Participants who reported perceived tinnitus location as being inside the head were 3.3 times more likely to respond positively to therapy compared with those who perceived tinnitus to be located in their ears. Numerous studies have shown that only a small percentage of patients with tinnitus report the location of their tinnitus as “in the head.” Stouffer and Tyler (1990) found that 9.8% of 528 patients described their tinnitus as being located “in head.” Meikle and Taylor-Walsh (1984) reported that 11% of more than 1,800 clinic patients described their tinnitus location as “in the head.” Meikle and Griest (1989) reported that 5% of 872 patients described their tinnitus as being located “in the head,” whereas approximately 10% perceived their tinnitus location as “both in the head and ears.” A tinnitus trial conducted by Henry et al (2007) found that 14.9% of 268 research participants described their tinnitus as being located “inside head.”
It is clear that the location of tinnitus is perceived far more often in the ears than in the head. What is unknown is if this perceptual difference is related to etiologic factors. Meikle and Griest (1989) suggested that “men exposed to long-duration occupational noise tend to report tinnitus that is in ‘both ears,’ while tinnitus that is due to illness or other non-noise-related causes tends to be localized in the head.” (p. 74) Vernon (1978) may have been the first to posit that tinnitus located within the head may be due to head injury. In a recent study (Henry et al, 2012), almost half of the participants with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury reported that their tinnitus was perceived “inside the head,” whereas only 8% of the participants without traumatic brain injury reported this same perception. If these findings of differential tinnitus location are repeated in further trials, then this could have implications regarding underlying mechanisms of tinnitus generation. Different mechanisms might exist that generate tinnitus and help to explain why some people respond to treatment whereas others do not.
The other finding from the current study was that participants who perceived their hearing to be a big or very big problem were more likely to respond to treatment compared with those who did not. This finding could be explained because hearing aids and combination instruments (ear-level masking plus amplification devices) were provided to many participants as part of their treatment protocol. Therefore, these participants received treatment that addressed their hearing problem in addition to their tinnitus-related problems.
A potential confounder is that patients often blame their tinnitus for their hearing difficulties (Coles, 1995; Zaugg et al, 2002; Dobie, 2004b; Henry et al, 2008). These patients believe that the tinnitus interferes with their hearing, which is usually a misconception on their part (in very few cases, hearing ability may actually be impaired by tinnitus). Therefore, it is possible that some of the “responders” who reported hearing to be a problem received benefit primarily because their hearing needs were addressed. It is well known that hearing aids can be beneficial for mitigating the effects of tinnitus (Surr et al, 1985; Henry et al, 2008; Searchfield et al, 2010). The controlled clinical trial did not evaluate this potential confounder (i.e., attributing problems hearing to the tinnitus itself). Therefore, it is not possible to know how useful the patient factor “self-reported poorer hearing” by itself predicts outcomes of tinnitus therapy.
The potential for patients to ascribe their hearing problem to their tinnitus is a general concern whenever patients are evaluated to determine if intervention for tinnitus is warranted. A drawback of tinnitus outcome questionnaires is that they do not separate out hearing-related problems from tinnitus-related problems, resulting in artificially inflated index scores for some patients. Henry et al (2008) developed the Tinnitus and Hearing Survey to make this distinction at the time of the audiologic evaluation. Use of the Tinnitus and Hearing Survey helps to identify individuals who are candidates for tinnitus-specific therapy. All patients reporting both tinnitus and a hearing problem should first have their hearing problem addressed before considering tinnitus-specific intervention.
The present study highlights the need to develop systematic methodology for assessing individual patient factors that could be useful in determining the likelihood of successful therapeutic outcomes as well as the most appropriate form of therapy. Development of such methodology would also enable more direct comparisons among research studies. For example, Kröner-Herwig et al (2006) also examined hearing problems and duration of tinnitus as possible predictors of treatment outcome. Their results, however, did not reveal these to be significant patient factors. Without knowing how the current methods differ from those of Kröner-Herwig and colleagues, we can only speculate that the discrepant results could be attributed to different outcome measures being used as well as recruitment from different patient populations. Dobie (1999) addressed multiple caveats affecting direct comparison of research studies, including a lack of consensus regarding study design and use of a common outcome measure.
Future Directions
The patient factors evaluated in this study were selected retrospectively using data collected in a controlled clinical study (Henry et al, 2006). Prospective studies should be conducted to determine if the significant factors identified in this study are generalizable to other patient populations. More broadly, it will be necessary to standardize criteria for defining responders and nonresponders as well as methodologies used to determine the significance of individual factors.
Newman et al (1998) determined that a change of 20 points on the THI was clinically significant; the current study used this criterion change in the THI to classify an individual as a “responder.” Other methods of identifying responders could include performing statistical analyses on selected variables to detect statistically significant differences among groups. Patient factors identified as significant may vary according to the specific intervention and the population sampled. In general, the question is how much improvement needs to occur to classify a patient as responding to treatment. A major component with respect to defining what constitutes “response to treatment” pertains to the outcome measure used. Indeed, different measures can result in the identification of different patient factors associated with treatment outcome. Furthermore, a specific amount of change may be significant for some patients but not for others. These issues necessitate standardizing (1) the method of identifying patient factors; (2) the outcome measures used to compare patient factors; and (3) the methods used to determine responders versus nonresponders. RCTs are needed to address these issues. In the current study, treatment-related changes were not compared with a control group; using a control group would more definitively classify individuals responding to tinnitus therapy, above and beyond placebo effects, and validate patient factors associated with specific interventions. Results reported here are not meant to be suggestive of specific patient factors able to predict treatment benefit but, rather, that patient factors should be evaluated as possible variables leading to a better understanding of what factors contribute to responsiveness to treatment.
The need to standardize outcome measures used to evaluate tinnitus treatments led to the development of the Tinnitus Functional Index (TFI) (Meikle et al, 2012). The TFI is a 25-item self-report questionnaire that can be used to evaluate responsiveness to tinnitus treatment. As a standardized measure of tinnitus outcome, the TFI has certain advantages compared with other tinnitus outcome questionnaires. Mainly, the concept of responsiveness was central to the design of the TFI. All tinnitus questionnaires before the TFI, including the THI (Newman et al, 1996), Tinnitus Handicap Questionnaire (Kuk et al, 1990), Tinnitus Reaction Questionnaire (Wilson et al, 1991), Tinnitus Severity Index (Meikle et al, 1995), and many others were not validated for responsiveness. Another advantage of the TFI versus other questionnaires is the inclusion of multiple domains of tinnitus severity as well as an auditory/hearing subscale that can be used to distinguish between improvements in hearing versus tinnitus after completion of treatment. Overall, it is important for future research to look toward using a standardized outcome measure evaluating tinnitus treatment responsiveness, such as the TFI, to allow for comparisons across tinnitus treatments. Another benefit would be to guide in the selection of tinnitus therapies associated with responsiveness for specific patient factors and various types of tinnitus.
In the future, including factors related to tinnitus distress might yield different outcomes than those seen in our study. Wallhäusser-Franke et al (2012) evaluated a range of tinnitus characteristics, hearing, and psychological factors in individuals with tinnitus to determine the relationship between the perception of tinnitus loudness and tinnitus-related distress. Consistent with other studies (Henry and Meikle, 2000; Folmer et al, 2001), a modest relationship was reported to exist between tinnitus loudness and tinnitus-related distress. When considering responsiveness to treatment outcome, a benefit can be associated with reductions in tinnitus-related distress separate from any reduction in the perceptual characteristics of tinnitus. Using multiple outcome measures in future studies to differentiate between hearing-related problems, tinnitus-related problems, and tinnitus perceptions will assist in understanding the potential predictive qualities of certain patient factors and tinnitus treatment outcome. It is possible that certain patient factors might offer insight into whether an individual might respond to a specific treatment as well as being predictive of selecting one treatment versus another. Important and logical next steps include (1) examining patient factors associated with treatment benefit (responders); (2) looking at differences among tinnitus interventions; and (3) replicating these findings through inclusion of other outcome measures. More research is needed to further address these possibilities using prospective, randomized, controlled designs.
Acknowledgments
Support for this project was provided by the National Institutes of Health National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders T35 Research Training Grant (DC008764), and VA Rehabilitation Research and Development Service (C2887R, C3214R, and F7070S). Additional support was provided by the National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research at the Portland VA Medical Center.
Abbreviations
- BDI
Beck Depression Inventory
- HF-PTA
high-frequency pure-tone average
- LF-PTA
low-frequency pure-tone average
- MML
minimal masking level
- RCTs
randomized clinical trials
- RI
residual inhibition
- SD
standard deviation
- SRT
speech reception threshold
- STAI
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
- TFI
Tinnitus Functional Index
- THI
Tinnitus Handicap Inventory
- TM
tinnitus masking
- TRT
tinnitus retraining therapy
Footnotes
This study was presented at the Biennial NCRAR International Conference, October 8, 2009, Portland OR; and the American Auditory Society Annual Meeting, March 5, 2010, Scottsdale, AZ.
References
- Andersson G, Vretblad P, Larsen HC, Lyttkens L. Longitudinal follow-up of tinnitus complaints. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2001;127(2):175–179. doi: 10.1001/archotol.127.2.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American National Standards Institute (ANSI) American National Standard Maximum Permissible Ambient Noise Levels for Audiometric Test Rooms. American National Standards Institute; 1991. ANSI S3.1-1991. [Google Scholar]
- Ariizumi Y, Hatanaka A, Kitamura K. Clinical prognostic factors for tinnitus retraining therapy with a sound generator in tinnitus patients. J Med Dent Sci. 2010;57(1):45–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coles RRA. Classification of causes, mechanisms of patient disturbance, and associated counseling. In: Vernon JA, Moller AR, editors. Mechanisms of Tinnitus. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1995. pp. 11–19. [Google Scholar]
- Davis A, Refaie AE. Epidemiology of tinnitus. In: Tyler R, editor. Tinnitus Handbook. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group; 2000. pp. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Dobie RA. A review of randomized clinical trials in tinnitus. Laryngoscope. 1999;109(8):1202–1211. doi: 10.1097/00005537-199908000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobie RA. Clinical trials and drug therapy for tinnitus. In: Snow JB, editor. Tinnitus: Theory and Management. Lewiston, NY: BC Decker Inc; 2004a. pp. 266–277. [Google Scholar]
- Dobie RA. Overview: suffering from tinnitus. In: Snow JB, editor. Tinnitus: Theory and Management. Lewiston, NY: BC Decker Inc; 2004b. pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Folmer RL, Griest SE, Martin WH. Chronic tinnitus as phantom auditory pain. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2001;124(4):394–400. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2001.114673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallin JI, Ognibene FP. Principles and Practice of Clinical Research. 3. San Diego, CA and Waltham MA: Elsevier; 2007. pp. 15–26. [Google Scholar]
- Graul J, Klinger R, Greimel KV, Rustenbach S, Nutzinger DO. Differential outcome of a multimodal cognitive-behavioral inpatient treatment for patients with chronic decompensated tinnitus. Int Tinnitus J. 2008;14(1):73–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Schechter MA, Nagler SM, Fausti SA. Comparison of tinnitus masking and tinnitus retraining therapy. J Am Acad Audiol. 2002;13(10):559–581. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Dennis KC, Schechter MA. General review of tinnitus: prevalence, mechanisms, effects, and management. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48(5):1204–1235. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/084). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Flick CL, Gilbert A, Ellingson RM, Fausti SA. Reliability of tinnitus loudness matches under procedural variation. J Am Acad Audiol. 1999;10(9):502–520. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Loovis C, Montero M, et al. Randomized clinical trial: group counseling based on tinnitus retraining therapy. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2007;44(1):21–32. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2006.02.0018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Meikle MB. Psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus. J Am Acad Audiol. 2000;11(3):138–155. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Schechter MA, Zaugg TL, et al. Outcomes of clinical trial: tinnitus masking versus tinnitus retraining therapy. J Am Acad Audiol. 2006;17(2):104–132. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.17.2.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Zaugg TL, Myers PJ, et al. Pilot study to develop telehealth tinnitus management for persons with and without traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2012;49(7):1025–1042. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2010.07.0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JA, Zaugg TL, Myers PJ, Schechter MA. The role of audiologic evaluation in progressive audiologic tinnitus management. Trends Amplif. 2008;12(3):170–187. doi: 10.1177/1084713808319941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herraiz C, Hernandez FJ, Toledano A, Aparicio JM. Tinnitus retraining therapy: prognosis factors. Am J Otolaryngol. 2007;28(4):225–229. doi: 10.1016/j.amjoto.2006.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesser H, Weise C, Westin VZ, Andersson G. A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive-behavioral therapy for tinnitus distress. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(4):545–553. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoare DJ, Kowalkowski VL, Kang S, Hall DA. Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials examining tinnitus management. Laryngoscope. 2011;121(7):1555–1564. doi: 10.1002/lary.21825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobson J, Chisholm E, El Refaie A. Sound therapy (masking) in the management of tinnitus in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;(12):CD006371. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006371.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman HJ, Reed GW. Epidemiology of tinnitus. In: Snow JB, editor. Tinnitus: Theory and Management. Lewiston, NY: BC Decker Inc; 2004. pp. 16–41. [Google Scholar]
- Jastreboff PJ, Hazell JWP. Treatment of tinnitus based on a neurophysiological model. In: Vernon JA, editor. Tinnitus Treatment and Relief. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998. pp. 201–217. [Google Scholar]
- Jastreboff PJ, Hazell JW, Graham RL. Neurophysiological model of tinnitus: dependence of the minimal masking level on treatment outcome. Hear Res. 1994;80(2):216–232. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(94)90113-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus retraining therapy. In: Snow JB, editor. Tinnitus: Theory and Management. Lewiston, NY: BC Decker Inc; 2004. pp. 295–309. [Google Scholar]
- Kaldo-Sandström V, Larsen HC, Andersson G. Internet-based cognitive-behavioral self-help treatment of tinnitus: clinical effectiveness and predictors of outcome. AmJ Audiol. 2004;13(2):185–192. doi: 10.1044/1059-0889(2004/023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleinjung T, Steffens T, Sand P, et al. Which tinnitus patients benefit from transcranial magnetic stimulation? Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2007;137(4):589–595. doi: 10.1016/j.otohns.2006.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koizumi T, Nishimura T, Sakaguchi T, Okamoto M, Hosoi H. Estimation of factors influencing the results of tinnitus retraining therapy. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 2009 Jun;(562):40–45. doi: 10.1080/00016480902933072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kröner-Herwig B, Zachriat C, Weigand D. Do patient characteristics predict outcome in the outpatient treatment of chronic tinnitus? Psychosom Med. 2006;3:Doc07. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuk FK, Tyler RS, Russell D, Jordan H. The psychometric properties of a tinnitus handicap questionnaire. Ear Hear. 1990;11(6):434–445. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199012000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langenbach M, Olderog M, Michel O, Albus C, Köhle K. Psychosocial and personality predictors of tinnitus-related distress. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2005;27(1):73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Devesa P, Perera R, Theodoulou M, Waddell A. Cognitive behavioural therapy for tinnitus. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;8(9):CD005233. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005233.pub3.Review. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meikle MB, Griest SE. Gender-based differences in characteristics of tinnitus. The Hearing Journal. 1989;42:68–76. [Google Scholar]
- Meikle MB, Griest SE, Stewart BJ, Press LS. Measuring the negative impact of tinnitus: A brief severity index. In: Ryan A, editor. Abstracts of the Eighteenth Midwinter Research Meeting, Association for Research in Otolaryngology. Des Moines, IA: Association for Research in Otolaryngology; 1995. p. 167. [Google Scholar]
- Meikle MB, Henry JA, Griest SE, et al. The tinnitus functional index: development of a new clinical measure for chronic, intrusive tinnitus. Ear Hear. 2012;33(2):153–176. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31822f67c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meikle M, Taylor-Walsh E. Characteristics of tinnitus and related observations in over 1800 tinnitus patients. J Laryngol Otol; Proceedings of the Second International Tinnitus Seminar; New York. 1983; Ashford, Kent: Invicta Press; 1984. pp. 17–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman CW, Jacobson GP, Spitzer JB. Development of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1996;122(2):143–148. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1996.01890140029007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman CW, Sandridge SA, Jacobson GP. Psychometric adequacy of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI) for evaluating treatment outcome. J Am Acad Audiol. 1998;9(2):153–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman GR, Sloan JA, Wyrwich KW. Interpretation of changes in health-related quality of life: the remarkable universality of half a standard deviation. Med Care. 2003;41(5):582–592. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000062554.74615.4C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riley RD, Hayden JA, Steyerberg EW, et al. Prognosis Research Strategy (PROGRESS) 2: prognostic factor research. PLoS Med. 2013;10(2):e1001380. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Searchfield GD, Kaur M, Martin WH. Hearing aids as an adjunct to counseling: tinnitus patients who choose amplification do better than those that don’t. Int J Audiol. 2010;49(8):574–579. doi: 10.3109/14992021003777267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults. Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Stouffer JL, Tyler RS. Characterization of tinnitus by tinnitus patients. J Speech Hear Disord. 1990;55(3):439–453. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5503.439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surr RK, Montgomery AA, Mueller HG. Effect of amplification on tinnitus among new hearing aid users. Ear Hear. 1985;6(2):71–75. doi: 10.1097/00003446-198503000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyler RS, Oleson J, Noble W, Coelho C, Ji H. Clinical trials for tinnitus: study populations, designs, measurement variables, and data analysis. Prog Brain Res. 2007;166:499–509. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66048-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyler R, Coelho C, Tao P, et al. Identifying tinnitus sub-groups with cluster analysis. Am J Audiol. 2008;17(2):S176–S184. doi: 10.1044/1059-0889(2008/07-0044). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernon J. Relief of tinnitus by masking treatment. In: English GM, editor. Otolaryngology. Philadelphia: Harper & Row; 1982. pp. 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Vernon JA. Information from UOHSC Tinnitus Clinic. Amer Tinnitus Assn Newsletter. 1978;3:1–4. [Google Scholar]
- Vernon JA, Meikle MB. Measurement of tinnitus: an update. In: Kitahara M, editor. Tinnitus. Pathophysiology and Management. Tokyo, Japan: Igaku-Shoin; 1988. pp. 36–52. [Google Scholar]
- Vernon JA, Meikle MB. Tinnitus masking. In: Tyler RS, editor. Tinnitus Handbook. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group; 2000. pp. 313–356. [Google Scholar]
- Wallhäusser-Franke E, Brade J, Balkenhol T, D’Amelio R, Seegmüller A, Delb W. Tinnitus: distinguishing between subjectively perceived loudness and tinnitus-related distress. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(4):e34583. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson PH, Henry J, Bowen M, Haralambous G. Tinnitus reaction questionnaire: psychometric properties of a measure of distress associated with tinnitus. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(1):197–201. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaugg TL, Schechter MA, Fausti SA, Henry JA. Difficulties caused by patients’ misconceptions that hearing problems are due to tinnitus. In: Patuzzi R, editor. Proceedings of the Seventh International Tinnitus Seminar. Crawley: University of Western Australia; 2002. pp. 226–228. [Google Scholar]