Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Jan 28;6:e21589. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21589

Stimulus relevance modulates contrast adaptation in visual cortex

Andreas J Keller 1,2,3,*, Rachael Houlton 4, Björn M Kampa 2,5,6, Nicholas A Lesica 7, Thomas D Mrsic-Flogel 4,8, Georg B Keller 3,9,, Fritjof Helmchen 2,
Editor: David Kleinfeld10
PMCID: PMC5298877  PMID: 28130922

Abstract

A general principle of sensory processing is that neurons adapt to sustained stimuli by reducing their response over time. Most of our knowledge on adaptation in single cells is based on experiments in anesthetized animals. How responses adapt in awake animals, when stimuli may be behaviorally relevant or not, remains unclear. Here we show that contrast adaptation in mouse primary visual cortex depends on the behavioral relevance of the stimulus. Cells that adapted to contrast under anesthesia maintained or even increased their activity in awake naïve mice. When engaged in a visually guided task, contrast adaptation re-occurred for stimuli that were irrelevant for solving the task. However, contrast adaptation was reversed when stimuli acquired behavioral relevance. Regulation of cortical adaptation by task demand may allow dynamic control of sensory-evoked signal flow in the neocortex.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21589.001

Research Organism: Mouse

Introduction

Our sensory systems constantly receive streams of sensory signals. The computational resources to process this input, however, are limited. Neural circuits in sensory systems have been shown to reduce responses to sustained stimuli (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926; Albrecht et al., 1984; Maffei et al., 1973) or selectively enhance aspects of the sensory input that are relevant to a behavioral task (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Ito and Gilbert, 1999; Kato et al., 2015; Reynolds and Heeger, 2009; Zhang et al., 2014). In visual cortex, neural responses to a sustained stimulus adapt over the course of a few seconds. Thus, a proposed function of adaptation is to redistribute processing resources to behaviorally relevant or novel stimuli. Most experiments on adaptation, however, were carried out in anesthetized animals. While sensory-evoked responses are known to be modulated by task engagement or attention (Ito and Gilbert, 1999; Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Reynolds and Chelazzi, 2004), it is still unclear if cortical adaptation is modulated by the behavioral relevance of the stimulus.

Results and discussion

To test the behavioral dependence of cortical response adaptation, we presented sustained moving grating stimuli to mice in different behavioral states and in conditions with different behavioral relevance of the visual test stimulus. Consistent with previous findings (Ahmed et al., 1997; Carandini and Ferster, 1997; Sanchez-Vives et al., 2000; Vidyasagar, 1990; Keller and Martin, 2015), we found that the responses of neurons in anaesthetized mouse primary visual cortex (V1) adapt to sustained high-contrast grating stimuli (Figure 1), and that this adaptation depends on local cortical activity (King et al., 2016) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Several mechanisms have been proposed to underlie such contrast adaptation (Ahmed et al., 1997; Carandini and Ferster, 1997; Sanchez-Vives et al., 2000; Vidyasagar, 1990; Keller and Martin, 2015), including tonic feedforward inhibition mediated by parvalbumin positive (PV+) interneurons (Ahmed et al., 1997; Keller and Martin, 2015). Accordingly, we found that PV+ neurons adapt less than putative excitatory neurons and that adaptation is only weakly orientation-specific for both neuron types (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). To test if neural responses also adapt in awake mice, we compared adaptation measured in the same neurons using two-photon calcium imaging under anesthesia and during wakefulness. As opposed to data obtained under anesthesia, we found that adaptation was absent and neural activity even increased during sustained grating presentations in awake recordings (Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplements 2i–l, 3 and 4). Adaptation was stronger (i.e. slope of adaptation more negative) for almost all cells in anesthetized compared to awake mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 3a). This reversal of adaptation in awake mice could be explained neither by response saturation, nor locomotion, nor eye movements (Figure 1d and Figure 1—figure supplements 2i,k and 3). We hypothesize that an attentional mechanism could prevent adaptation when a stimulus is of unknown relevance to the animal. If so, adaptation should reappear if mice divert attention away from the stimulus and learn that the stimulus is behaviorally irrelevant.

Figure 1. Contrast adaptation in awake and anesthetized mice.

(a) Schematic of the experimental setup. Calcium imaging with GCaMP6m (Chen et al., 2013) was performed during the presentation of drifting sinusoidal gratings. (b) Calcium transients from four example putative excitatory cells tuned to a moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast (presented for 10 s; grey shadings). The same cells were recorded during wakefulness and anesthesia. (c) Averaged calcium responses of tuned putative excitatory cells. Note that even small differences in adaptation can be detected using two-photon imaging (Figure 1—figure supplement 2a–d). Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (d) Slope of adaptation of single cells recorded in different behavioral states (same data as in c; line fit to the data in time window 1–9.75 s). Anesthetized mice show a significantly more negative slope compared to all other states [anest. (169 cells) – awake (169 cells): p<10−10; Wilcoxon signed-rank; running (51 cells): p<10−4; resting (169 cells): p<10−10; eye movements (168 cells): p<10−10; eye movement-free (165 cells): p<10−10; Wilcoxon rank-sum]. There was no significant difference between running and resting mice, as opposed to the significant but small difference in eye movement and eye movement-free trials (p=0.49 and p=0.0047, respectively; Wilcoxon rank-sum). NS, not significant; **p<0.005; ***p<0.0005.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21589.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Adaptation in visual cortex of anesthetized mice is prevented by optogenetic silencing of cortical neurons (see also King et al. 2016).

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Neural activity was recorded with multi-tetrode arrays in anesthetized mice that express channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in parvalbumin-positive (PV+) neurons. To test whether adaptation in V1 depends on local cortical activity, we presented a sustained moving grating with or without locally silencing the visual cortex and probed the V1 activity level immediately after the manipulation time window. (a) Averaged and binned (bin size 333 ms) spike responses of tuned putative excitatory cells to a moving square-wave grating at 100% contrast in anesthetized mice. The responses for three stimulus conditions are shown: Adapted, grating presented for 7 s; Control, grey screen presented for 3.5 s followed by 3.5 s of grating; LED+adapted, grating presented for 7 s with simultaneous optogenetic activation of PV+ cells during the first 3.5 s followed by a 0.5 s decrease of the optogenetic activation of PV+ cells (see Materials and methods). LED (optogenetic activation) and visual stimulus timings are illustrated by the top traces and marked by the vertical solid lines. The vertical dotted line indicates the time bin (first bin after LED off) used to compare the state of adaptation of the three stimulus conditions in b. Average traces are plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (b) Averaged spike rate for the three stimulus conditions for the bin indicated by the vertical dashed line in a. The LED+adapted condition evokes similar spike rates compared to the control and significantly more than the adapted condition (tuned putative excitatory: 109 cells; LED+adapted/adapted: p<10−10; LED+adapted/control: p=0.13; adapted/control: p<10−10; Wilcoxon signed-rank). Bars plotted as mean ± SEM. (c) Same as a but for PV+ cells. (d) Same as b but for PV+ neurons. All conditions evoke similar spike rates (tuned PV+: 27 cells; LED+adapted/adapted: p=0.73; LED+adapted/control: p=0.067; adapted/control: p=0.17; Wilcoxon signed-rank). (e) Scatterplot of the average spike rate responses of all cells during the first 3.5 s of their preferred grating stimulus, with and without optogenetic stimulation of PV+ cells. Cells that increased their firing during the optogenetic stimulation of PV+ cells were classified as PV+ neurons, the remaining ones as putative excitatory cells. Highlighted cells (1–3) are shown in f–h. (f) Same as a but for a single cell classified as PV+. (g,h) Same as f but for two example cells classified as putative excitatory. NS, not significant; ***p<0.0005.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Differences in contrast adaptation across cell-types can be revealed using two-photon imaging.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Under anesthesia, PV+ neurons adapted less than putative excitatory cells, consistent with the idea that they might inhibit other neurons to produce contrast adaptation (Keller and Martin, 2015). Moreover, iso- and cross-orientation adaptation had similar effects on the population (see also Stroud et al., 2012). In mouse V1, PV+ neurons are much less selective to a grating of different orientations than excitatory neurons (Atallah et al., 2012; Hofer et al., 2011; Kerlin et al., 2010). Therefore, an adaptation mechanism based on PV+ neurons would result in adaptation that is only weakly dependent on the orientation of the stimulus. Indeed, adapting putative excitatory cells to their preferred or null (orthogonal to preferred) orientation results in a significant, but small difference in adapted firing rates. Moreover, no difference was found when adapting PV+ cells with either iso- or cross-orientation under anesthesia (with respect to test orientation). (a) Neural activity was recorded with multi-tetrode arrays in anesthetized mice (same cells as in Figure 1—figure supplement 1a,b). Averaged and normalized responses of tuned putative excitatory and PV+ cells to a moving square-wave grating at 100% contrast presented for 7 s in anesthetized mice. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (b) Adaptation is quantified as the mean decrease in the normalized spike rate per second during the stimulus presentation (tuned putative excitatory: 109 cells; PV+: 27 cells; p=0.010; Wilcoxon rank-sum). Bars plotted as mean ± SEM. (c) The neural activity of tuned putative excitatory compared to PV+ cells recorded with two-photon calcium imaging. Averaged and normalized calcium responses to a moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast presented for 7 s in anesthetized mice. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (d) Adaptation is quantified as the mean decrease in normalized ∆F/F per second during the stimulus presentation (tuned putative excitatory: 545 cells; PV+: 78 cells; p=3.7×10−4; Wilcoxon rank-sum). The initial rise was excluded in the estimation of the slope (see Materials and methods). (e) Averaged responses of tuned putative excitatory cells in anesthetized mice to a moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast presented for 10 s followed by 10 s of a second stimulus of increased (100% contrast; dark traces) or decreased contrast (25% contrast; light traces). The first stimulus was either presented at the optimal (solid traces) or the orthogonal orientation of the neurons (dotted traces). The second stimulus was always presented at the optimal orientation. The two vertical dotted lines indicate the time window which was used to compare iso- and cross-orientation adaptation in f. ∆F/F traces plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (f) Iso-orientation compared to cross-orientation adaptation (50% contrast) of tuned putative excitatory cells measured at 25% or 100% contrast. This is quantified as average ∆F/F after adaptation (seconds 11–12; indicated by the vertical dotted lines in e; 299 cells; 25%: p<10−10; 100%: p<10−4; Wilcoxon signed-rank). (g) Same as e but for PV+ cells. (h) Same as f but for PV+ cells (40 cells; 25%: p=0.12; 100%: p=0.98; Wilcoxon signed-rank). (i) Same as e but in awake mice. Note that the absence of adaptation to the moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast (0–10 s) cannot be explained by a response ceiling since the cells increase their responses when increasing the contrast to 100% (at 10 s). (j) Same as f but in awake mice (239 cells; 25%: p=0.0082; 100%: p=0.14; Wilcoxon signed-rank). (k) Same as i but for PV+ cells. (l) Same as j but for PV+ cells (33 cells; 25%: p=0.0064; 100%: p=0.20; Wilcoxon signed-rank). Bars plotted as mean ± SEM. NS, not significant; *p<0.05; ***p<0.0005.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Contrast adaptation in anesthetized compared to awake mice and effects of running and eye movements on adaptation in awake mice.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Scatterplot showing slopes of adaptation of tuned putative excitatory cells recorded in anesthetized and awake mice (same cells as in Figure 1c,d; line fit to the data in time window 1–9.75 s). Seventeen cells not visible in the plot as they lie outside of the axis shown - 13 above and 4 below the diagonal. Red cross shows mean ± SEM. (b) Averaged responses of tuned putative excitatory cells to a moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast presented for 10 s in awake running and resting mice. See also Figure 1d. Note that the number of cells for running (51 cells) is smaller than for the resting condition (169 cells) because not all cells were recorded in both conditions. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (c) Same as b but for trials with eye movements and eye movement-free trials.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4. A large fraction of neurons in awake mice were suppressed during the stimulus and decreased their activity below baseline.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

The group of cells that were suppressed showed a positive average response under anesthesia. Of the putative excitatory cells, fewer cells showed a decrease compared to PV+ cells. Moreover, the magnitude of the decrease was larger for PV+ cells compared to putative excitatory cells. (a) All putative excitatory cells recorded in awake mice divided into groups which increase or decrease their activity in response to a moving sinusoidal grating at 50% contrast. The responses of the same groups of neurons are shown for the anesthetized mice. Note that the cells that are suppressed by the grating stimulation in awake mice are excited in anesthetized mice. The two vertical dotted lines indicate the time window which was used to compare the decrease in activity in c. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (b) Same as a but for PV+ cells. (c) Cumulative density of cells based on their average activity in response to the grating. (d) Fraction of putative excitatory and PV+ cells in awake and anesthetized mice with a negative average ∆F/F (compared to baseline) in response to the grating. (e) The average decrease from baseline (of decreasing cells) in response to the grating (average of 8.75–9.75 s as indicated by the vertical dotted lines in a,b). Decreases are significantly larger in awake mice compared to anesthetized mice (putative excitatory awake: 208 cells; putative excitatory anesthetized: 99 cells; p=0.011; PV+ awake: 28 cells; PV+ anesthetized: 9 cells; p=0.010; Wilcoxon rank-sum). In awake mice, the decrease is significantly larger in PV+ compared to putative excitatory cells (putative excitatory: 208 cells; PV+: 28 cells; p=0.0038; Wilcoxon rank-sum). *p<0.05; **p<0.005.

To test the role of stimulus relevance for adaptation, we designed a simple visual navigation task (Figure 2), in which mice were trained to run to reach the end of a virtual tunnel using visual feedback, while a drifting grating was presented in a fixed part of the visual field (probe patch, centered on the retinotopic location of the recording site; see Materials and methods). Consistent with the lack of adaptation in the passively observing awake mouse (Figure 1), we found that adaptation to the grating stimulus was absent initially. As mice learned to perform the navigation task, however, adaptation of neural responses to the grating stimulus reappeared (‘grating-irrelevant’ condition, Figure 2e,g,h, Figure 2—figure supplement 1a, Video 1). This reappearance of adaptation suggests that mice, as they learned to interact with the task-relevant part of the visual field, diverted attention away from the grating stimulus that contained no task-relevant information. Based on this finding, we predicted that for an identical visual input, but when the grating stimulus is behaviorally relevant, neural responses should not adapt with experience. To test this prediction, we showed a different group of mice a replay of the visual stimulus sequence generated by one of the mice in the grating-irrelevant group but increased the behavioral relevance of the grating stimuli by delivering a water reward at the offset of the grating (‘grating-relevant’ condition, Figure 2f–h, Figure 2—figure supplement 1b). We found that adaptation remained absent over training sessions in the grating-relevant group, despite visual experience being identical to the grating-irrelevant group. Moreover, when mice exhibited anticipatory licking to the reward, neural responses showed an effect opposite to adaptation and activity increased over the course of the stimulus presentation (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a–i). We verified that the differences in adaptation between the grating-relevant group and the grating-irrelevant group cannot be explained by learning-related changes in mean running speed, time spent running or number of saccades (Figure 2—figure supplement 2j–l). These results suggest, therefore, that in behaving animals contrast adaptation is modulated bidirectionally by stimulus relevance (Figure 2—figure supplement 2f,i).

Video 1. Sample mouse under grating-irrelevant condition in session 4.

Download video file (15.6MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21589.010

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21589.010

Figure 2. Adaptation is modulated by stimulus relevance in awake mice.

(a) Schematic of the behavioral task. For the grating-irrelevant condition, movement of a virtual tunnel projected on a toroidal screen was coupled to the locomotion (rotation and running on a spherical treadmill) of the head-restrained mice (Dombeck et al., 2007). Mice were trained to orient and run to the end of the tunnel for a water reward. We presented a horizontal sinusoidal moving grating in a circular probe patch centered on the retinotopic location of the recording site, interspersed with random intervals of gray (10–20 s; Video 1). (b) First and last paths of a sample mouse (four days apart). The colors show individual trials. (c) Task difficulty (length of the tunnel) was increased over learning to keep the number of rewards approximately constant. (d) Learning curve of an example mouse (solid line: exponential fit). The performance is quantified as the fraction of time spent running in the direction of the goal (±25°). (e) Data from animals trained in the behavioral task under grating-irrelevant conditions. Traces show averaged calcium responses (GCaMP6f) (Chen et al., 2013) of tuned putative excitatory cells to a moving sinusoidal grating. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (f) Same as e but for animals exposed to the grating-relevant condition. For this condition, the visual stimulus on the screen was a replay of the visual flow from one of the mice in the grating-irrelevant group. To match the initial responses of the grating-relevant and the grating-irrelevant traces, ten percent of neurons were excluded from analysis (see Materials and methods). Note that this did not change the results. (g) Slopes of adaptation of the same cells as in e and f (line fit to the data in time window 1–10 s). In the grating-irrelevant condition, the slope significantly decreases from the first to the following sessions, as opposed to the grating-relevant condition (putative excitatory: 332 and 303 cells, respectively; p=0.017 and p=0.28, respectively; Wilcoxon signed-rank). The slopes for the two conditions are similar during the first session, but significantly differ during later sessions (putative excitatory: 332 and 303 cells; p=0.84 and p=0.0036, respectively; Wilcoxon rank-sum). The solid curves are exponential fits to the data. Error bars represent mean ± SEM. (h) Same as g but for mean response to the grating. In the grating-irrelevant condition, the mean response significantly decreases from the first to the following sessions, as opposed to the grating-relevant condition (putative excitatory: 332 and 303 cells, respectively; p<10−4 and p=0.85, respectively; Wilcoxon signed-rank). The mean responses for the two conditions are similar during the first session, but significantly differ during later sessions (putative excitatory: 332 and 303 cells; p=0.61 and p=0.0015, respectively; Wilcoxon rank-sum). NS, not significant; *p<0.05; **p<0.005; ***p<0.0005.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21589.007

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Scatterplots showing slopes of adaptation of the cells in training session one compared to the average slope in sessions 2–5 in awake mice (see also Figure 2; line fit to the data in time window 1–10 s).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a) Grating-irrelevant condition (same cells as in Figure 2e,g; 28 cells not visible in the plot as they lie outside of the axis shown, 16 above and 12 below the diagonal). Red cross shows mean ± SEM. (b) Grating-relevant condition (same cells as in Figure 2f,g; 12 cells not visible in the plot as they lie outside of the axis shown, 3 above and 9 below the diagonal). Red cross shows mean ± SEM.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Licking, running, and eye-movement behavior in awake mice.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(a–c) Lick frequency for grating-relevant condition. (a) Baseline subtracted lick frequency of the two animals showing a significant increase in anticipatory licking from session 1 (dotted colored lines) to session 5 (solid colored lines). The two vertical dotted lines indicate the stimulus presentation and the vertical solid line at 0 s indicates the water reward. Inset: Average anticipatory lick frequency (−0.5–0 s) is significantly higher in session five compared to session one in these two animals (session 1: 111 trials, session 5: 144 trials, p<10−4 and session 1: 146 trials, session 5: 143 trials, p<10−10; Wilcoxon rank-sum). (b) Same as a, but for the animals with no significant increase in anticipatory licking (session 1: 145 trials, session 5: 144 trials, p=0.093; session 1: 108 trials, session 5: 131 trials, p=0.083; session 1: 108 trials, session 5: 135 trials, p=0.20 and session 1: 144 trials, session 5: 135 trials, p=0.49; Wilcoxon rank-sum). (c) Average pre-reward lick frequency (−0.5–0s) for all animals over all sessions. (d) Averaged responses of tuned excitatory cells of the same animals as in a, to a moving sinusoidal grating (displayed in the probe patch) for sessions 1 and 5. Ten percent of the neurons were excluded to match the initial conditions of the grating-relevant traces to the grating-irrelevant traces (see Materials and methods for details). Note that this did not change the results. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (e) Averaged responses of tuned excitatory cells of the same animals as in b, to a moving sinusoidal grating (displayed in the probe patch) for sessions 1 and 5. Ten percent of the neurons were excluded to match the initial conditions of the grating-relevant traces to the grating-irrelevant traces (see Materials and methods for details). Note that this did not change the results. Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (f) Traces show the differences in averaged responses of tuned excitatory cells between session 1 and 5 for the three conditions: grating-irrelevant; grating-relevant without anticipatory licking; and grating-relevant with anticipatory licking. (g) Slopes of adaptation of the same cells as in d and e are shown. For reward anticipating mice, the slope significantly increases from the first to the following sessions (putative excitatory: 77 cells; p=0.0022; Wilcoxon signed-rank). For the non-anticipating mice, the slope did not show any significant difference from the first to the following sessions (putative excitatory: 226 cells; p=0.45; Wilcoxon signed-rank). The trials of each session were divided into four quarters. The vertical dashed lines separate the individual sessions. The solid curve is an exponential fit to the data. Error bars represent mean ± SEM. (h) Same as g but for mean response to the grating. For reward anticipating mice, the mean response significantly increases from the first to the following sessions (putative excitatory: 77 cells; p=0.020; Wilcoxon signed-rank). For the non-anticipating mice, the mean response did not show any significant change from the first to the following sessions (putative excitatory: 226 cells; p=0. 23; Wilcoxon signed-rank). (i) Bar plot shows the mean response difference (session 5 – session 1) for the three traces in f. Reward anticipating mice have a significantly larger response difference compared to mice under grating-relevant condition (77 cells and 332 cells, respectively; p<10−4; Wilcoxon rank-sum) or non-anticipating mice (77 cells and 226 cells, respectively; p=9.0×10−4; Wilcoxon rank-sum). There is no significant difference between grating-irrelevant and non-anticipating mice (332 cells and 226 cells, respectively; p=0.13; Wilcoxon rank-sum). (j) Proportion of time spent running over sessions for grating-irrelevant and grating-relevant conditions. The time spent running is not significantly different for the grating-irrelevant compared to the grating-relevant condition (7 and 6 mice; session 1: p=0.84; session 2: p=0.18; session 3: p=0.53; session 4: p=0.45; session 5: p=0.23; Wilcoxon rank-sum). Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (k) Same as j, but for mean speed. The mean speed is not significantly different for the grating-irrelevant compared to the grating-relevant condition (7 and 6 mice; session 1: p=0.84; session 2: p=0.45; session 3: p=0.95; session 4: p=0.84; session 5: p=0.29; Wilcoxon rank-sum). Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). (l) Same as j but for saccade frequency. The saccade frequency is not significantly different for the grating-irrelevant compared to the grating-relevant condition (7 and 6 mice; session 1: p=0.63; session 2: p=0.84; session 3: p=0.53; session 4: p=0.45; session 5: p=0.073; Wilcoxon rank-sum). Curves plotted as mean ± SEM (shading). NS, not significant; *p<0.05; **p<0.005; ***p<0.0005.

Thus, the responses of neurons in layer 2/3 of V1 do not adapt to sustained stimuli that are behaviorally relevant, but they do adapt if the stimulus within their receptive field is irrelevant and animals learn to direct attention away from it to other parts of the visual field. These effects are likely mediated by attentional mechanisms (Zhang et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2016) that could directly enhance the responses to relevant stimuli to prevent adaptation. The attentional modulation of adaptation was also not simply explained by changes in the adaptation of inhibitory neurons (data not shown) and is unlikely to be generated only locally.

In contrast to our findings in mouse V1, fMRI studies on awake humans have found response adaptation in V1 upon the presentation of visual patterns (Gardner et al., 2005; Fang et al., 2005; Huk and Heeger, 2001, but see Kastner et al., 2004). This disparity is likely the result of small but relevant differences in study design. For example, Huk and Heeger (2001) find a weak adaptation in V1 when participants were attending to two separate moving plaid stimuli. This could be explained by the fact that distributing attention decreases attentional effects (Ito and Gilbert, 1999). Other studies (Gardner et al., 2005; Fang et al., 2005) used stimuli that are known to cause adaptation in thalamus and even the retina (Smirnakis et al., 1997; Chander and Chichilnisky, 2001) and cannot distinguish the effects of cortical adaptation from those of subcortical adaptation.

In summary, we have shown that adaptation is dynamically regulated by task demand during learning. Our data are consistent with the idea that cortex dynamically regulates the flow of sensory information by suppressing responses to non-relevant stimuli through mechanisms of adaptation, while boosting sensory responses that are behaviorally important.

Materials and methods

Animals

All experiments and surgical procedures were carried out in accordance with the UK Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act under project license 70/7573, approved by the Cantonal Veterinary Office of Zurich, Switzerland, under license number 62/2011, or by the Cantonal Veterinary Office of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland, under license number 2537.

For the electrophysiology experiments, we used transgenic mice selectively expressing channel-rhodopsin-2 in parvalbumin-positive neurons (PV+). These mice were generated by crossing PvalbCre (Jackson 008069) and Ai32 animals (Jackson 012569). Data were collected from six mice (two female, four male, P39-P83).

For the two-photon experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, we used transgenic mice selectively expressing tdTomato in PV+ neurons. These mice were generated by crossing PvalbCre mice (Jackson 008069) with the Ai14 reporter line (Jackson 007914). Data were collected from eight adult mice (two female, six male, P90-161).

For the two-photon experiments on mice engaged in visually-guided behavior (grating-irrelevant and grating-relevant condition), we used transgenic mice selectively expressing tdTomato in GABAergic neurons. These mice were generated by crossing Slc32a1Cre mice (Jackson 016962) with the Ai9 reporter line (Jackson 007909). Data were collected from seven mice (two female, five male, P80-P282) for the grating-irrelevant condition and six mice (one female, five male, P80-P286) for the grating-relevant condition.

Surgical procedures and anesthesia

For the electrophysiology experiments, animals were anesthetized with a mixture of Fentanyl (Sublimaze, 0.05 µg/g of body weight), Midazolam (Dormicum, 5.0 µg/g of body weight) and Medetomidine (Domitor, 0.5 µg/g of body weight) injected intraperitoneal (i.p.). An adequate depth of anesthesia was indicated by lack of response to toe pinch. Eye cream (Isoptomax) was applied to the eyes to prevent dehydration during surgery. Atropine Sulphate (Hameln Pharmaceuticals, 0.02 µg/g of body weight) and Dexamethasone Sodium Phosphate (Hospira, 0.8 µg/g of body weight) were injected subcutaneously to reduce secretions and edema, respectively. Cortex buffer solution (125 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 10 mM glucose, 10 mM HEPES, 2 mM MgSO4, and 2 mM CaCl2 [pH 7.4], 50 µl) was also injected subcutaneously to prevent dehydration. Throughout the experiment, the body temperature was maintained at 38°C, measured with a rectal probe and controlled with a heating blanket. Fur was trimmed and an incision was made at the rear of the head, approximately level with the ears. The skull was cleared of tissue and immobilized by affixing it to a metal head plate using dental cement (Paladur, Heraeus Kulzer). The plate was then secured in a frame with the head in a horizontal position. A small craniotomy (~2 mm diameter) was made above the right monocular primary visual cortex (V1), determined by stereotaxic coordinates, using a high-speed foot-operated drill (Foredom). The exposed cortical surface was kept moist with cortex buffer solution (see above). The dura was removed and the cortex was covered with 2% agarose following initial electrode array insertion. After surgery, the eye cream was removed except for a thin layer, keeping the eye moist whilst minimizing any visual disturbance.

For the two-photon experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, the implantation of the hook for head fixation and the virus injection were performed in two separate surgeries. First, the animals were anesthetized with isoflurane (1–2%). Throughout the surgery, body temperature was measured and controlled with a heating pad. An eye cream (Vitamine A, Bausch&Lomb) and a local anesthetic (Xylocain Gel 2%, AstraZeneca) were applied. Atropine (0.3 µg/g of body weight) and dexamethasone (2 µg/g of body weight) were injected subcutaneously. The skull was cleared and a bonding agent (iBOND Total Etch, Heraeus Kulzer) applied. A hook for head fixation was implanted by first applying a droplet of super-glue (Ultra Gel, Pattex). The hook was fixated using light curable dental cement (Tetric EvoFlow, ivoclar vivadent). Betadine was applied to the wound. Antibiotics (100 µg/g of body weight, ceftriaxone, Rocephin, Roche) and pain killers (5 µg/g of body weight, Metacam, Boehringer Ingelheim) were injected subcutaneously before animals woke up. In the second surgery, the skull was thinned above the right monocular visual cortex, determined by stereotaxic coordinates. The eye cream was carefully removed and optical intrinsic imaging was performed to map V1 (see below). After making a craniotomy (3 or 4 mm diameter), 2–3 injections of 150 nl of AAV2/1-hsyn-GCaMP6m were made based on the intrinsic imaging and a glass coverslip was positioned. Each experiment consisted of an awake followed by an anesthetized part. For the latter we used isoflurane (0.4–1%). Throughout the anesthetized part, body temperature was measured and maintained at 38°C with a heating pad.

For the two-photon experiments on mice engaged in visually-guided behavior (grating-irrelevant and grating-relevant condition), surgical procedures have been described elsewhere (Leinweber et al., 2014). Briefly, the animals were anesthetized with an i.p. injection of a mixture of Fentanyl (Sublimaze, 0.05 µg/g of body weight), Midazolam (Dormicum, 5.0 µg/g of body weight) and Medetomidim (Domitor, 0.5 µg/g of body weight). A craniotomy was made over the right monocular V1, determined by stereotaxic coordinates. The mice were injected with 5 injections of 100–200 nl of AAV2/1-ef1α-GCaMP6f, before the coverslip was positioned. Finally, a head plate was implanted.

Electrophysiological recordings and optogenetic stimulation

Extracellular recordings were made using a multi-tetrode array (Neuronexus, Michigan, A4 × 2-tet-5mm-150-200-121) that was perpendicularly inserted into the brain with a computer-controlled micromanipulator (Scientifica, UK). The probe consisted of 4 evenly spaced shanks, spanning 600 μm of visual cortex in a medial-lateral plane. Each shank contained eight electrode sites, split between two tetrode configurations that were separated by a vertical distance of 150 μm. A reference electrode was also inserted into the cortex, away from the recording site, via a separate craniotomy. In order to target superficial cortical layers, the array was slowly lowered until visually responsive neurons were first encountered. Visual responsiveness was assessed online from multi-unit PSTHs obtained during full-field flash stimuli. Signals were digitized at a sampling frequency of 25 kHz (Tucker Davis Technologies, Florida, RZ2 Bioamp processor). For the optogenetic stimulation of the PV+ cells, illumination (470 nm) was provided by a high-power LED light source (Thorlabs, New Jersey), and directed via a fiber optic cable (400 μm, Thorlabs) which was positioned 3–4 mm from the surface of the cortex, where it dispersed to cover an area approximately 3 mm in diameter. LED illumination was kept constant except for the last 500 ms, where the intensity instantaneously reduced to 50% and then linearly decreased to zero to avoid rebound activation (Chuong et al., 2014).

Intrinsic signal optical imaging

For the experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, optical imaging of intrinsic signals was performed before the virus injection of the calcium indicator. Anesthetized mice were placed in front of a monitor and the cortical surface was illuminated with a 630 nm LED light (Thorlabs). The angle of the monitor was ~45° with respect to the craniocaudal axis of the mice with a distance of 20 cm between the center of the screen and the left eye of the mice. The position of the monitor with respect to the mice was kept constant in the following two-photon experiments. In a circular region with a diameter of 10° in the center of the monitor, a square-wave grating was presented for 5 s. Reflectance images were collected through a 4x objective (Olympus, Japan) with a CCD camera (Toshiba, Japan, TELI CS3960DCL). Intrinsic signal changes were analyzed as fractional reflectance changes relative to the prestimulus average. Injections of the calcium indicator were made based on the intrinsic signals.

Two-photon calcium imaging

For the experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, fluorescence was measured with a custom-built two-photon microscope controlled by HelioScan (www.helioscan.org) (Langer et al., 2013). The scanhead was based on an 8 kHz resonant scanner (Cambridge Technology, Switzerland), used in bidirectional mode. Images were acquired at 77.7 Hz with a resolution of 200 by 200 pixels. The illumination light source was a Ti:sapphire laser (MaiTai HP, Newport Spectra Physics, California). The excitation wavelength was set to 940 nm or 960 nm. Laser power under the objective (Nikon, Japan, 16 × 0.8 NA) never exceeded 50 mW (laser pulse width ≤100 fs at a repetition rate of 80 MHz). A volume stack was acquired at every imaging site.

For the two-photon experiments on mice engaged in visually-guided behavior (grating-irrelevant and grating-relevant condition), fluorescence was measured with a custom-built two-photon microscope (https://sourceforge.net/projects/iris-scanning/) (Leinweber et al., 2014). The scanhead was based on an 8 kHz resonant scanner (Cambridge Technology), used in bidirectional mode. This enabled frame rates of 40 Hz at 400 by 600 pixels. A high-power objective z-piezo stage (Physik Instrumente, Germany) was used to move the objective down in steps of approximately 20 µm between frames and return to the initial position after four frames. With this system, we acquired data at four different depths, reducing the effective frame rate from 40 Hz to 10 Hz. Data were acquired with a 250 MHz digitizer (National Instruments, Texas) and pre-processed with a custom programmed (https://sourceforge.net/projects/iris-scanning/) FPGA (National Instruments). The illumination light source was a Ti:sapphire laser with a prechirp unit (MaiTai eHP DS, Newport Spectra Physics). The excitation wavelength was set to 910 nm. Laser power under the objective (Nikon 16 × 0.8 NA) never exceeded 50 mW (pulse width ≤70 fs at a repetition rate of 80 MHz).

Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation

For the electrophysiology experiments, visual stimuli were generated using the open-source MATLAB (MathWorks) Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997). Drifting square-wave gratings (3 Hz, 0.04 cpd, 100% contrast) moving in eight different directions were presented on an LCD monitor (isoluminant at 82 cd/m2).

Then, responses to stimulus blocks of 7 s were measured. Stimulus blocks were interspersed with 3 s of grey screen. Baseline values were obtained from the 2 s time window before each stimulus. On alternate trials, cortex was optogenetically silenced during the first 3.5 s (see above). The stimuli were presented 5–40 times each.

For the two-photon calcium imaging experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, head-restrained mice were placed on a spherical air-supported treadmill (Dombeck et al., 2007), which allowed the mice to run or rest at their whim. Visual stimuli were generated using the open-source MATLAB (MathWorks) toolbox StimServer (Muir and Kampa, 2014). Drifting sinusoidal gratings (1.5 Hz, 0.04 cpd, 80% contrast) moving in eight different directions were presented (2 s grating interleaved with 4 s grey screen) on a LED-backlit monitor (BenQ XL2410T, iso-luminant at 23 cd/m2). The power-source of the LED-backlight was synchronized with the resonant scanner turnaround points (when data are not acquired) to minimize light-leak from the monitor (Leinweber et al., 2014). An iso- and cross-orientation (with an angular difference of 90°) were chosen for the adaptation paradigm. We presented a grating for 10 s at the iso- or cross-orientation at 50% contrast followed by a grating for 10 s at the iso-orientation at 25% or 100% contrast. This resulted in a total of 4 stimulus conditions which were presented in a pseudo-random order for 13–31 times each. The stimulus conditions were interleaved with an iso-luminant grey screen for at least 10 s. Subsequently, the orientation and contrast adaptation paradigms were repeated under anesthesia (presented 30–48 times each). Throughout all imaging sessions, we measured running speed and eye movements. Saccades were detected using a CMOS based video camera at 30 Hz (Imaging Source, North Carolina, DMK 22BUC03). Pupil position was computed offline by smoothing and thresholding the images and fitting a circle to the pupil. The filter radius and the image threshold were adapted manually for each experiment. Pupil position was filtered using a median filter. Eye movements were detected automatically by applying an adapted threshold. This method was cross-validated in several experiments using manual detection of eye movements.

For the experiments on mice engaged in visually-guided behavior with feedback coupling (grating-irrelevant condition), we first mapped the toroidal screen onto the cortical surface using intrinsic optical signal imaging. Single horizontal and vertical bars were shown moving over the whole surface of the screen. The treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation have been described previously (Leinweber et al., 2014; Dombeck et al., 2007). Briefly, head-restrained mice ran on a spherical air-supported treadmill. Throughout all imaging sessions, we measured the trajectories of the mice in the tunnel and eye movements with a CMOS based video camera at 30 Hz (Imaging Source, DMK 22BUC03). Mice were learning to use a part of the visual field to navigate to a target location in a virtual reality environment. Each mouse had five training sessions on consecutive days (sessions were spaced by 16–32 hr). Starting two days before the first experimental session, mice were water restricted and given a total of at least 1 ml water daily. The weight of the mice was measured daily before and after the training sessions. Before each session, orientation tuning was measured (4 s grating interleaved with 4 s grey screen). During the training session, movement in the virtual tunnel was coupled to the movements of the mice on the spherical treadmill. Mice were trained to orient and run to the end of the tunnel for a water reward (~10 µl per reward) and were immediately teleported back to the start after passing the end of the tunnel. The difficulty of the task (length of the tunnel) was increased during learning to keep the number of rewards approximately constant (~100 per session). Fraction of time spent running across sessions was kept approximately stable by applying occasional air-puffs. Throughout all imaging sessions, we presented a horizontal sinusoidal moving grating (both directions) at 100% contrast in a circular patch (50 degrees in diameter) centered on the retinotopic location of the recording site (45 degrees to the left from the point of view of the mice). This probe patch took up only about an eighth of the entire field of view of the toroidal screen (approximately 200 degrees horizontally, 90 degrees vertically). Grating presentations in the probe patch lasted 10 s (120–163 repetitions per session) and were interspersed with random intervals of grey (10–20 s). Presentations of the drifting grating were not coupled to the behavior of the mice.

For the grating-relevant condition, we repeated the experiment in a new set of mice with two differences. First, the movement in the tunnel was not coupled to their movement on the treadmill but was an exact replay of the visual stimulation used for a mouse under grating-irrelevant condition. The six mice under grating-relevant condition were matched to 6 of the 7 mice under grating-irrelevant condition. Second, the mice were not rewarded at the end of the virtual tunnel but 1 s after the offset of the grating.

The experimental paradigm was chosen to allow us to direct the attention of the mouse either away from or towards the gratings stimulus. One potential concern with a choice of paradigm in which the animal has control of the visual flow feedback in the grating-irrelevant condition is that the difference between predicted and actual visual feedback in the probe patch could result in a mismatch response (Keller et al., 2012). Mismatch responses are confined to spatially localized regions in visual space that align to the visual retinotopy (Zmarz and Keller, 2016). For this reason, grating-relevant and grating-irrelevant conditions were designed to have an equivalent visual flow mismatch in the retinotopic region of the probe patch. Therefore, any potential influence of mismatch responses is equivalent in both conditions. Moreover, neurons are either mismatch responsive, visually driven, or driven by a combination of both (Zmarz and Keller, 2016). The neurons we select for in our analysis are the most visually responsive neurons and hence are unlikely to respond to mismatch (Zmarz and Keller, 2016).

Analysis of electrophysiological data

Electrophysiological data were processed using Matlab (Math-Works) using custom-written code. Single unit spikes were isolated. To this end, the channels were bandpass filtered between 500 Hz and 5000 Hz and tetrodes were whitened. We identified potential spikes using an action potential detector described elsewhere (Choi et al., 2006). Then, we performed a principal component analysis (PCA) for each channel using the open-source cluster analysis program KlustaKwik (http://klusta-team.github.io/klustakwik/) (Watters and Reeke, 2014). Clusters of potential spikes were determined based on the first three components of the PCA. We calculated the isolation distance of each cluster (Schmitzer-Torbert et al., 2005) and excluded clusters with an isolation distance below 20. The number of potential spikes in the poorly isolated multi-unit activity for each tetrode was always at least as large as the number of spikes in any single-unit cluster. Spike times were determined with a 1 ms resolution.

The preferred stimuli and cell types were determined using the average responses over the first 3.5 s of visual stimulation (see Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation). For each neuron (total 210 cells), we determined the preferred cardinal orientation. Cells were excluded if they failed to respond in at least half of the trials of their preferred cardinal orientation. Then, we compared the average responses to their preferred cardinal orientation in the presence and absence of the optogenetic stimulation. Cells that had a higher average response during optogenetic stimulation were classified as PV+ cells and putative excitatory cells otherwise (data not shown).

All traces of spike rates were binned (~333 ms). Slopes of adaptation (Figure 1—figure supplement 2a,b) were estimated by performing a linear regression over 7 s of visual stimulation after normalizing.

Analysis of two-photon calcium imaging data

Two-photon calcium images were processed using custom written MATLAB (Math-Works) software.

For the experiments on mice not engaged in visually-guided behavior, we used the open-source toolbox FocusStack (https://bitbucket.org/DylanMuir/twophotonanalysis/) (Muir and Kampa, 2014). Cells were manually selected using ImageJ (National Institute of Mental Health, NIH). All traces were filtered using a sliding block filter (20 data points corresponding to ~0.26 s). Fluorescence changes (∆F/F) were calculated as (F-F0)/F0 using a 2 s baseline before the stimulus to determine F0. To determine orientation tuning curves, responses were calculated as averages over the whole 2 s presentation of visual stimulus. Preferred orientations were determined by fitting a sum-of-Gaussians to single-cell tuning curves. The Gaussians were forced to peak 180° apart and to have the same tuning width. Cells were classified as tuned to the iso- or cross-orientation (see Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation) depending on which was closer to the peak of the Gaussian fit. Cells were classified as responsive if in at least 50% of the trials the responses to the preferred orientation (iso- or cross-orientation) were significantly above baseline (Z score >2.58 corresponding to p<0.01). Fluorescence changes (∆F/F) for contrast tuning were calculated using a 3 s baseline before the stimulus.

Slopes of adaptation of tuned cells in Figure 1—figure supplement 2c,d were estimated by performing a linear regression over 1–7 s of visual stimulation after normalizing. The initial rise (approximated by 1 s) was excluded from the fit. Adaptation in awake and anesthetized mice (Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 3) was compared using neurons tuned in both states. Slopes of adaptation in Figure 1d were estimated by performing a linear regression over 1–9.75 s of visual stimulation (9.75–10 s was excluded due to filtering). Trials were classified as ‘running’ if at least during half the visual stimulation the running speed of the mice exceeded 1 cm/s and ‘resting’ otherwise. Trials were classified as ‘eye movement’ trials if the mice made at least one saccade during the visual stimulation and ‘eye movement-free’ otherwise. Cross- and iso-orientation adaptation in anesthetized and awake mice were compared using neurons that were tuned in anesthetized or awake mice, respectively (in Figure 1—figure supplement 2e–h and Figure 1—figure supplement 2i–l, respectively).

For the experiments on mice engaged in visually-guided behavior (grating-irrelevant and grating-relevant condition), analysis of functional imaging data was conducted as described previously (Keller et al., 2012). Briefly, data were full-frame registered using a custom written software (https://sourceforge.net/projects/iris-scanning/). Cells were selected manually based on mean and maximum projections. Raw fluorescence traces were calculated as the average fluorescence of all pixels within a selected region for each frame. To calculate the fluorescence changes (∆F/F), the 8-percentile value of the fluorescence distribution in a ±15 s window was subtracted from the raw fluorescence signal, which was then divided by the median of each cell’s fluorescence distribution (Dombeck et al., 2007). Responses were calculated as averages over the whole 4 s presentation time of visual stimulus. Preferred orientations were determined by fitting a sum-of-Gaussians to single-cell tuning curves averaged over all sessions. Gaussians were fixed to peak 180° apart and to have the same tuning widths. Cells were classified as tuned to the horizontal grating if the peak of the Gaussian fit was within horizontal ±45°. For all activity traces in Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplement 2, average activity during a pre-stimulus baseline of 2 s was subtracted. Cells were classified as responsive if in at least half of the sessions the average responses to the horizontal grating were significantly above or below baseline (|Z score| > 3.29 corresponding to p<0.001). To match the initial conditions of the grating-relevant and grating-irrelevant conditions, 10% of the neurons in the grating-relevant, reward anticipating and non-anticipating condition were excluded. We excluded cells which in session one showed the largest deviations from the mean response in the grating-irrelevant condition. Note that this did not change the results. The performance was quantified as the fraction of time spent running (>1 cm/s) in the direction of the goal with a tolerance of ±25°. Slopes of adaptation and mean responses were estimated by performing a linear regression and averaging over 1–10 s of visual stimulation, respectively (Figure 2g,h and Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2g,h). To estimate the slopes and means, trial responses were divided into four bins per session. Exponential fits were done based on the binned data (four bins per session).

All lick frequencies were baseline-corrected by subtracting the mean lick frequency 15 s to 13 s before the reward. The pre-reward licking (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a–c) was defined as the baseline-corrected lick frequency 0.5 s to 0 s before the reward.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by EU SECO Grant EU216593, ETH Grant 2-73246-8, and SNF Sinergia to Kevan AC Martin, the FP7 EU Grant 269921 (‘BrainScaleS’) (FH and BMK), the Novartis Research Foundation (GBK), the Swiss National Science Foundation (GBK), the European Research Council Grant (‘NeuroVision’, 616509, TDM-F), the Wellcome Trust Grant 095074 (TDM-F), and the Wellcome Trust Research Fellowship WT086697MA (NAL). We thank Kevan AC Martin for his inestimable support in discussions, funding, and resources. We thank Morgane MF Roth for critical comments on the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Seventh Framework Programme Grant 269921 to Björn M Kampa, Fritjof Helmchen.

  • Wellcome Trust Research Fellowship WT086697MA to Nicholas A Lesica.

  • European Research Council Grant 616509 to Thomas D Mrsic-Flogel.

  • Wellcome Trust Grant 095074 to Thomas D Mrsic-Flogel.

  • Novartis Foundation to Georg B Keller.

Additional information

Competing interests

TDM-F: Reviewing editor, eLife.

The other authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

AJK, Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

RH, Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

BMK, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

NAL, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

TDM-F, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

GBK, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

FH, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments and surgical procedures were carried out in accordance with the UK Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act under project license 70/7573, approved by the Cantonal Veterinary Office of Zurich, Switzerland, under license number 62/2011, or by the Cantonal Veterinary Office of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland, under license number 2537.

References

  1. Adrian ED, Zotterman Y. The impulses produced by sensory nerve-endings: Part II. the response of a single End-Organ. The Journal of Physiology. 1926;61:151–171. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1926.sp002281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahmed B, Allison JD, Douglas RJ, Martin KA. An intracellular study of the contrast-dependence of neuronal activity in cat visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1997;7:559–570. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.6.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Albrecht DG, Farrar SB, Hamilton DB. Spatial contrast adaptation characteristics of neurones recorded in the cat's visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1984;347:713–739. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Atallah BV, Bruns W, Carandini M, Scanziani M. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly transform cortical responses to visual stimuli. Neuron. 2012;73:159–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carandini M, Ferster D. A tonic hyperpolarization underlying contrast adaptation in cat visual cortex. Science. 1997;276:949–952. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5314.949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chander D, Chichilnisky EJ. Adaptation to temporal contrast in primate and salamander retina. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:9904–9916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09904.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Choi JH, Jung HK, Kim T. A new action potential detector using the MTEO and its effects on spike sorting systems at low signal-to-noise ratios. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2006;53:738–746. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2006.870239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chuong AS, Miri ML, Busskamp V, Matthews GA, Acker LC, Sørensen AT, Young A, Klapoetke NC, Henninger MA, Kodandaramaiah SB, Ogawa M, Ramanlal SB, Bandler RC, Allen BD, Forest CR, Chow BY, Han X, Lin Y, Tye KM, Roska B, Cardin JA, Boyden ES. Noninvasive optical inhibition with a red-shifted microbial rhodopsin. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1123–1129. doi: 10.1038/nn.3752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dombeck DA, Khabbaz AN, Collman F, Adelman TL, Tank DW. Imaging large-scale neural activity with cellular resolution in awake, mobile mice. Neuron. 2007;56:43–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fang F, Murray SO, Kersten D, He S. Orientation-tuned FMRI adaptation in human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:4188–4195. doi: 10.1152/jn.00378.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gardner JL, Sun P, Waggoner RA, Ueno K, Tanaka K, Cheng K. Contrast adaptation and representation in human early visual cortex. Neuron. 2005;47:607–620. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Harris KD, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Cortical connectivity and sensory coding. Nature. 2013;503:51–58. doi: 10.1038/nature12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hofer SB, Ko H, Pichler B, Vogelstein J, Ros H, Zeng H, Lein E, Lesica NA, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Differential connectivity and response dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1045–1052. doi: 10.1038/nn.2876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Huk AC, Heeger DJ. Pattern-motion responses in human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:72–75. doi: 10.1038/nn774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ito M, Gilbert CD. Attention modulates contextual influences in the primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Neuron. 1999;22:593–604. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80713-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kastner S, O'Connor DH, Fukui MM, Fehd HM, Herwig U, Pinsk MA. Functional imaging of the human lateral geniculate nucleus and pulvinar. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91:438–448. doi: 10.1152/jn.00553.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kato HK, Gillet SN, Isaacson JS. Flexible sensory Representations in auditory Cortex driven by behavioral Relevance. Neuron. 2015;88:1027–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Keller AJ, Martin KA. Local circuits for contrast normalization and adaptation investigated with Two-Photon imaging in cat primary visual Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:10078–10087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0906-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Keller GB, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Sensorimotor mismatch signals in primary visual cortex of the behaving mouse. Neuron. 2012;74:809–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kerlin AM, Andermann ML, Berezovskii VK, Reid RC. Broadly tuned response properties of diverse inhibitory neuron subtypes in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;67:858–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kim H, Ährlund-Richter S, Wang X, Deisseroth K, Carlén M. Prefrontal Parvalbumin Neurons in Control of Attention. Cell. 2016;164:208–218. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. King JL, Lowe MP, Stover KR, Wong AA, Crowder NA. Adaptive Processes in Thalamus and Cortex revealed by Silencing of primary visual Cortex during Contrast Adaptation. Current Biology. 2016;26:1295–1300. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Langer D, van 't Hoff M, Keller AJ, Nagaraja C, Pfäffli OA, Göldi M, Kasper H, Helmchen F. HelioScan: a software framework for controlling in vivo microscopy setups with high hardware flexibility, functional diversity and extendibility. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2013;215:38–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Leinweber M, Zmarz P, Buchmann P, Argast P, Hübener M, Bonhoeffer T, Keller GB. Two-photon calcium imaging in mice navigating a virtual reality environment. Journal of Visualized Experiments. 2014;84:e50885. doi: 10.3791/50885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Maffei L, Fiorentini A, Bisti S. Neural correlate of perceptual adaptation to gratings. Science. 1973;182:1036–1038. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4116.1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Muir DR, Kampa BM. FocusStack and StimServer: a new open source MATLAB toolchain for visual stimulation and analysis of two-photon calcium neuronal imaging data. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2014;8:85. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L. Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:611–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ. The normalization model of attention. Neuron. 2009;61:168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Sanchez-Vives MV, Nowak LG, McCormick DA. Membrane mechanisms underlying contrast adaptation in cat area 17 in vivo. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:4267–4285. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04267.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schmitzer-Torbert N, Jackson J, Henze D, Harris K, Redish AD. Quantitative measures of cluster quality for use in extracellular recordings. Neuroscience. 2005;131:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Smirnakis SM, Berry MJ, Warland DK, Bialek W, Meister M. Adaptation of retinal processing to image contrast and spatial scale. Nature. 1997;386:69–73. doi: 10.1038/386069a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Stroud AC, Ledue EE, Crowder NA. Orientation specificity of contrast adaptation in mouse primary visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:1381–1391. doi: 10.1152/jn.01148.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Vidyasagar TR. Pattern adaptation in cat visual cortex is a co-operative phenomenon. Neuroscience. 1990;36:175–179. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(90)90360-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Watters N, Reeke GN. Neuronal spike train entropy estimation by history clustering. Neural Computation. 2014;26:1840–1872. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Zhang S, Xu M, Kamigaki T, Hoang Do JP, Chang WC, Jenvay S, Miyamichi K, Luo L, Dan Y. Selective attention. Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science. 2014;345:660–665. doi: 10.1126/science.1254126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Zmarz P, Keller GB. Mismatch receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2016;92:766–772. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Jan 28;6:e21589. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21589.011

Decision letter

Editor: David Kleinfeld1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

[Editors’ note: this article was originally rejected after discussions between the reviewers, but the authors were invited to resubmit after an appeal against the decision.]

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Stimulus relevance modulates contrast adaptation in visual cortex" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, individuals that would agree are leading experimentalists, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor.

Our decision has been reached after consultation between the reviewers. Based on these discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication in eLife.

While the reviewers find considerable merit in your work, they each have voiced strong reservations about the nature of the adaptation as discussed in the detailed comments below. The reviewers further suggest a more fleshed-out study of the mismatch response in the optical flow, as well as a more scholarly presentation of literature results.

Reviewer #1:

In their study, Keller et al. address the phenomenon of contrast adaptation in the primary visual cortex in three different brain states, and find evidence that adaptation depends on brain state and/or stimulus relevance. Overall, the study addresses an interesting and important question: might adaptation act as a mechanism to dynamically modulate the gain of cortical responses to relevant vs irrelevant stimuli? They conclude that adaptation might work in this way. The authors primarily rely on two photon calcium imaging from unidentified L2/3 cells in the anesthetized, awake/naïve, and awake/trained mice. Their first result is a striking difference in the contrast adaptation between awake and anesthetized conditions, finding much weaker adaptation in awake/naïve mice. Their second, perhaps more interesting result, is that adaptation in awake conditions reappears for stimuli that become 'irrelevant' over a training task, but then can disappear when that same stimulus becomes 'relevant'. The behavioral paradigm the authors use to make this conclusion is a bit problematic. To measure responses for 'irrelevant' stimuli a grating is presented over the imaged area's receptive field, while the rest of the visual field is a feedback controlled virtual reality environment. I understand why the authors set it up this way, but this necessarily leads to an optic flow mismatch for the receptive field portion. Since prior experiments have shown that optic flow mismatch can strongly modulate V1 responses in mouse, I wonder if this contributed to the effects observed here. To then test adaptation for 'relevant' stimuli, some mice are trained on a movie generated from the 'irrelevant' visual data (from a different mouse) and given a water reward immediately after the previously 'irrelevant' grating appears, presumably providing some 'relevance' as it should act as a classical conditioning cue – which seems to be supported by the anticipatory licking. However, while a clever design, the interpretation is a bit muddied again by the complete mismatch between the virtual visual data and any movement (or lack of movement) of the mouse. It seems like a far simpler and more conventional behavioral paradigm in which a grating is either predictive of reward or not would be much easier to interpret, than a virtual reality system that engages optic flow and locomotion related circuits. Thus, I find the data interesting, but the confusing nature of the behavioral paradigm makes the conclusions the authors propose to draw not entirely convincing.

The authors suggest (mostly tangentially) that PV neurons, which adapt less to gratings, might mediate adaptation. If this were true, then PV neuron activity should also strongly depend on the relevance of the grating stimulus during the behavioral paradigm. Was this true? Perhaps the authors do not have this data, but labeling the PV cells with a reporter would make this trivial, and substantially add to any proposed mechanism for adaptation. Alternatively, these experiments could just as easily be done with extracellular probes and analysis of FS units. Of course, the synapse between PV neurons and excitatory neurons is known to show strong adaptation and changes in synaptic adaptation could well be important as well, even if adaptation at the level of spiking does not correlate well with stimulus relevance.

Reviewer #2:

This is a very impressive paper that is aimed at understanding visual adaptation to grating stimuli in the mouse primary visual cortex. The use of optogenetic methods in this paper is very powerful. However, there are significant questions about the interpretation of the data.

The main question is whether or not adaptation could have been taking place in the awake mice and in the mice in which the grating stimuli were task relevant. A secondary question is whether the adaptation phenomena measured mouse cortex are relevant to adaptation in primates including humans.

The stimuli used to test for adaptation were gratings of high contrast. Possibly the responses of the awake animals were hitting a response ceiling, masking adaptation that was taking place but was not apparent because of the response ceiling. Figure 1—figure supplement 2E offers some evidence that a response ceiling is a possibility. In that figure, if one compares the response to 50% contrast in the first 10s with response to 25% contrast in the second 10s following ortho presentation of 50%, one sees they are almost the same. The authors need to convince us that there was no response ceiling, with other experiments.

The question of whether the results can be generalized to humans is based on the history of experiments on visual pattern adaptation. The very first studies of pattern adaptation by Blakemore and Campbell (and many subsequent psychophysical studies) were done in awake humans who were adapted to patterns that were task relevant. In those experiments, there was adaptation not facilitation to exposure to high contrast patterns before behavioral tests. It is not clear how the present results could explain pattern adaptation in those old experiments. Adaptation is also used as a tool frequently in fMRI experiments on awake humans, and again it is not clear how the present results in mouse V1 are consistent with those many adaptation experiments in awake humans. The authors should address this question and explain what they think the relevance of their results is to prior results on human adaptation.

In addition, there is another more detailed question about Figure 1—figure supplement 2. It is not clear to me why the measure of adaptation is larger for the Exc-cell 2-photon data in Figure 1—figure supplement 2D than for the electrophysiological data in Figure 1—figure supplement 2B. In A the adaptation seems clearly greater than in C, yet the bar graphs in B and D state the opposite. Are the vertical scales in error in B and D?

eLife. 2017 Jan 28;6:e21589. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21589.012

Author response


[Editors’ note: the author responses to the first round of peer review follow.]

Reviewer #1:

In their study, Keller et al. address the phenomenon of contrast adaptation in the primary visual cortex in three different brain states, and find evidence that adaptation depends on brain state and/or stimulus relevance. Overall, the study addresses an interesting and important question: might adaptation act as a mechanism to dynamically modulate the gain of cortical responses to relevant vs irrelevant stimuli? They conclude that adaptation might work in this way. The authors primarily rely on two photon calcium imaging from unidentified L2/3 cells in the anesthetized, awake/naïve, and awake/trained mice. Their first result is a striking difference in the contrast adaptation between awake and anesthetized conditions, finding much weaker adaptation in awake/naïve mice. Their second, perhaps more interesting result, is that adaptation in awake conditions reappears for stimuli that become 'irrelevant' over a training task, but then can disappear when that same stimulus becomes 'relevant'. The behavioral paradigm the authors use to make this conclusion is a bit problematic. To measure responses for 'irrelevant' stimuli a grating is presented over the imaged area's receptive field, while the rest of the visual field is a feedback controlled virtual reality environment. I understand why the authors set it up this way, but this necessarily leads to an optic flow mismatch for the receptive field portion. Since prior experiments have shown that optic flow mismatch can strongly modulate V1 responses in mouse, I wonder if this contributed to the effects observed here.

We thank the reviewer for the assessment that we ‘address an interesting and important question’. The point about a potential effect of the mismatching visual input is important and very interesting. However, we do not think that visual flow mismatch is confounding the interpretation of our results for the following reasons:

1) Mismatch responses are confined to spatially localized regions in visual space that align to the visual retinotopy (Zmarz and Keller, 2016, note, this paper was not published at the time of first submission). In our paradigm, the visual flow mismatch is equivalent (in the retinotopic region of the grating patch) for both “grating irrelevant” (feedback) and “grating relevant” (replay) conditions. Taking these two things together suggests that only local mismatch (which is identical in both conditions) would influence mismatch responses in the retinotopic region of our probe patch. In other words, if mismatch responses would influence activity, their influence should be identical in replay and feedback sessions. However, we observe a strong difference in adaptation in the two conditions (see Figure 2).

2) Neurons are either mismatch responsive, visually driven, or driven by a combination of both (Zmarz and Keller, 2016). The neurons we select for in our analysis, are the most visually responsive neurons in our dataset (based on their responses during passive viewing of orientated gratings) and hence are unlikely to respond to mismatch.

3) Lastly, a key point of our experiments was to show that cortical adaptation is not adequately explained by adaptation of feedforward input. We indeed cannot identify the source or nature of the attentional signal that modulates adaptation in V1, but we can show that it is not purely feed-forward driven (because visual input in playback and feedback conditions is identical).

In summary, the mismatch responses as described by Zmarz and Keller, 2016, or Keller et al., 2012, cannot explain the differences between relevant and irrelevant conditions observed here.

On a side-note: any stimulus that is not coupled to movement generates a “mismatch” (e.g., visual experiments where neural responses are probed with moving stimuli that the animal does not self-generate). The mismatch responses described for example by Zmarz and Keller, 2016, are merely those where there is less visual flow than expected (an experiment on a passively observing animal probes mismatches of the form of more visual flow than expected). Hence, it is likely that a large part of visual processing in general is concerned mainly with detecting and processing mismatch (any visual input that is not the direct consequence of self-motion).

To address this concern, we have added a discussion about mismatch responses to the description of the grating-relevant and grating-irrelevant conditions in the Materials and methods section (in the section ‘Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation’, last paragraph).

To then test adaptation for 'relevant' stimuli, some mice are trained on a movie generated from the 'irrelevant' visual data (from a different mouse) and given a water reward immediately after the previously 'irrelevant' grating appears, presumably providing some 'relevance' as it should act as a classical conditioning cue – which seems to be supported by the anticipatory licking. However, while a clever design, the interpretation is a bit muddied again by the complete mismatch between the virtual visual data and any movement (or lack of movement) of the mouse.

Please see arguments above for the influence of mismatch.

As for similarity of movement, average fraction of time spent running and running speed were not different in the grating-relevant and grating-irrelevant condition (see Figure 2—figure supplement 2J,K).

Just to avoid a potential misunderstanding, the reward stimulus was not given immediately after the previous ‘irrelevant’ grating appears, but after the offset of the grating with a delay of one second. This was done to separate the grating presentation form the reward and avoid any contamination of the grating responses with lick related signals.

To address the concern of mismatch influencing our results, we have added a discussion to the task description in the Materials and methods section (‘Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation’, last paragraph).

It seems like a far simpler and more conventional behavioral paradigm in which a grating is either predictive of reward or not would be much easier to interpret, than a virtual reality system that engages optic flow and locomotion related circuits. Thus, I find the data interesting, but the confusing nature of the behavioral paradigm makes the conclusions the authors propose to draw not entirely convincing.

This is an interesting suggestion. The reason why we did not pursue this type of experiment is that it would remain unclear if the mouse is indeed less attentive to a grating when simply shown without an associated reward. Hence, we aimed to design an experimental paradigm that would allow us to direct the attention either away from or towards the grating. The virtual navigation task is simply one way to ensure that the animal directs its attention towards the part of the visual field informative for the task.

We do understand the hesitation of the reviewer, but given that, when designing the experiments, we took into consideration that mismatch processing is local (at least to the same extent that visual processing is local, see Zmarz and Keller, 2016). We do not think that non-local mismatch signals are more of a confound than non-local visual signals.

In the Materials and methods section, we now explain the choice of experimental paradigm more thoroughly (‘Treadmill, eye-tracking and visual stimulation’, last paragraph).

The authors suggest (mostly tangentially) that PV neurons, which adapt less to gratings, might mediate adaptation. If this were true, then PV neuron activity should also strongly depend on the relevance of the grating stimulus during the behavioral paradigm. Was this true? Perhaps the authors do not have this data, but labeling the PV cells with a reporter would make this trivial, and substantially add to any proposed mechanism for adaptation. Alternatively, these experiments could just as easily be done with extracellular probes and analysis of FS units. Of course, the synapse between PV neurons and excitatory neurons is known to show strong adaptation and changes in synaptic adaptation could well be important as well, even if adaptation at the level of spiking does not correlate well with stimulus relevance.

Please excuse, we were unclear in our phrasing. We intended to show that in the anesthetized mouse, consistent with results in the cat (Keller and Martin, 2015), adaptation could in principle be explained by PV interneurons because these adapt less to gratings. We did not mean to suggest that this is the same mechanism responsible for the behavioral modulation of adaptation. On the other hand, considering changes in adaptation of inhibitory interneurons as potential mechanism as suggested by the reviewer is interesting. In fact, we have a dataset from experiments in VGAT animals (where we have monitored activity of inhibitory neurons) to argue the behavioral modulation of adaptation cannot easily be explained by changes in inhibitory adaptation – hence the source of this signal is unlikely to be local. In these experiments, we find that inhibitory neurons essentially follow the activity of putative excitatory cells. If the reviewer thinks this would be useful, we would be happy to include these data in the manuscript, but think that this would go beyond the scope of the paper.

We now emphasize this more clearly in the Results and discussion section (third paragraph, last sentence) and the figure legend of Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

Reviewer #2:

This is a very impressive paper that is aimed at understanding visual adaptation to grating stimuli in the mouse primary visual cortex. The use of optogenetic methods in this paper is very powerful. However, there are significant questions about the interpretation of the data.

The main question is whether or not adaptation could have been taking place in the awake mice and in the mice in which the grating stimuli were task relevant. A secondary question is whether the adaptation phenomena measured mouse cortex are relevant to adaptation in primates including humans.

The stimuli used to test for adaptation were gratings of high contrast. Possibly the responses of the awake animals were hitting a response ceiling, masking adaptation that was taking place but was not apparent because of the response ceiling. Figure 1—figure supplement 2E offers some evidence that a response ceiling is a possibility. In that figure, if one compares the response to 50% contrast in the first 10s with response to 25% contrast in the second 10s following ortho presentation of 50%, one sees they are almost the same. The authors need to convince us that there was no response ceiling, with other experiments.

This is a very good point. Note, however, that the data shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2E were recorded in anesthetized animals, where we do find adaptation, especially in response to gratings of higher contrasts. To control for ceiling effects, we performed experiments in which we followed the presentation of the 50% contrast grating with a 100% contrast grating. In awake mice, cells do not adapt to the 50% contrast grating, but can further increase their activity if the contrast is increased to 100%. This demonstrates that the lack of adaptation is not due to response ceiling.

To address the concern of a response ceiling, we have added data from these experiments as Figure 1—figure supplement 2I-L to the manuscript.

The question of whether the results can be generalized to humans is based on the history of experiments on visual pattern adaptation. The very first studies of pattern adaptation by Blakemore and Campbell (and many subsequent psychophysical studies) were done in awake humans who were adapted to patterns that were task relevant. In those experiments, there was adaptation not facilitation to exposure to high contrast patterns before behavioral tests. It is not clear how the present results could explain pattern adaptation in those old experiments.

This is a very good point. Many psychophysical studies indeed suggest that the visual system adapts to sustained visual patterns. Psychophysical measurements, however, reflect the activity of the whole brain, not just primary visual cortex. Using fMRI in humans, pattern adaptation has been shown to be stronger in higher visual areas than in primary visual cortex (Huk and Heeger, 2001). Hence, it is possible that adaptation is still present in some higher visual areas independent of attention.

An alternate explanation for this discrepancy is that attention can overcome adaptation. Using psychophysical measurements, Pestilli and colleagues (Pestelli et al., 2007, PMID: 17685805) show that attention and adaptation have opposite effects.

In the Results and discussion section, we now discuss our results in the context of the human literature (fourth paragraph).

Adaptation is also used as a tool frequently in fMRI experiments on awake humans, and again it is not clear how the present results in mouse V1 are consistent with those many adaptation experiments in awake humans. The authors should address this question and explain what they think the relevance of their results is to prior results on human adaptation.

Many fMRI studies on awake humans indeed find adaptation in primary visual cortex. This disparity is likely the result of small but relevant differences in study design.

Gardner et al., 2005, show adaptation in primary visual cortex to a checkerboard pattern. In their study, subjects were performing a detection task to guide their attention away from the checkerboard stimulus. Moreover, the checkerboard was flickered, which induces temporal contrast adaptation. This type of adaptation has been demonstrated to already take place in the retina (Smirnakis et al., 1997, Chander and Chichilnisky, 2001).

Fang et al., 2005, also report pattern adaptation in human primary visual cortex using fMRI. Here, they used Gabor patches which were counter-phase flickered. Again, this type of adaptation is already strongly present in the retina.

Kastner et al., 2003, however, find no contrast adaptation in primary visual cortex despite using a flickered checkerboard stimulus. To assess contrast adaptation, they presented ascending versus descending contrasts. Gardner et al., 2005, argue that this method might not be sensitive enough to capture an adaptation effect.

Engel and Furmanski (2001, PMID: 11356883) show that there is selective adaptation to color contrast. Here, they presented low contrast red-green or dark-light sinusoidal moving gratings before and after exposure to high contrast gratings. Although, they demonstrate that there is color-contrast specific adaptation they do not show that spatial luminance contrast adaptation is present in primary visual cortex.

Huk and Heeger, 2001, find a weak adaptation in V1 when participants were attending two separate moving plaid stimuli. This could be explained by the fact that distributing attention decreases attentional effects (Ito and Gilbert, 1999).

In conclusion, the results of our study are largely consistent with the current human fMRI literature and significantly go beyond the current literature. It would be very interesting to see whether and how attention affects adaptation in human primary visual cortex for stimuli that mainly evoke adaptation in cortex (as opposed to stimuli that are known to result in significant adaptation in the retina or thalamus).

In the Results and discussion section, we now discuss our results in the context of the human literature (fourth paragraph).

In addition, there is another more detailed question about Figure 1—figure supplement 2. It is not clear to me why the measure of adaptation is larger for the Exc-cell 2-photon data in Figure 1—figure supplement 2D than for the electrophysiological data in Figure 1—figure supplement 2B. In A the adaptation seems clearly greater than in C, yet the bar graphs in B and D state the opposite. Are the vertical scales in error in B and D?

The numbers indicated are indeed correct. To minimize the influence of initial response transients, and to focus on the slow adaptation occurring over the course of seconds, we estimate adaptation using a linear fit to the data (in a window from 0 s to 7 s).


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES