Skip to main content
Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring logoLink to Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring
letter
. 2016 Dec 24;6:60–61. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2016.11.008

Impaired familiarity in individuals at risk for Alzheimer's disease: Commentary on Schoemaker et al. (2016)

Christine Bastin 1,, Gabriel Besson 1
PMCID: PMC5300297  PMID: 28217737

Interventions aiming at postponing or preventing the development of Alzheimer's dementia (AD) should be implemented before the first clinical symptoms. It is therefore critical to identify individuals with incipient AD. The most inexpensive and noninvasive way to achieve this goal is the use of cognitive tests that are sensitive to initial AD cerebral pathology. More specifically, the earliest cognitive deficit should be one that affects the specific function that is supported by the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices where neurofibrillary tangles start to accumulate [1]. Recent advances in cognitive neuroscience indicate that the entorhinal and perirhinal areas play a critical role in familiarity-based memory [2], the feeling that some information has been encountered before. This function contrasts with recollection, in which one recalls qualitative details about the encounter with the information.

Although compelling and theoretically founded, the hypothesis that impaired familiarity may be a very early cognitive marker of AD has not been much investigated and led to inconsistent findings for reviews [3], [4]. Recently, Schoemaker, Poirier, Escobar, Gauthier, and Pruessner (2016, Alzheimer's and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 2, 132–139) addressed this hypothesis by testing cognitively healthy individuals who carry or not the ε4 allele of the APOE gene [5]. In this study, 21 carriers of APOE ε4 (at risk for AD) and 60 noncarriers performed a memory task in which they studied pictures of faces under two conditions that differed in terms of the spatial location of the face, the color of the background, and the judgment to make about each face. Then, participants had to recognize the faces studied under the two conditions among new faces. For each face, they had to indicate whether the face had been presented, and if so, in which condition. In this task, recollection was indexed by the proportion of targets that were correctly identified as old and that were also attributed to the correct encoding condition. In contrast, familiarity was measured by the proportion of targets that were correctly recognized as old but that the participant could not attribute to the correct encoding condition. The results showed that APOE ε4 carriers and noncarriers did not differ on the recollection score, but APOE ε4 carriers had a significantly poorer familiarity score. Schoemaker et al. (2016) [5] concluded that individuals at increased risk of developing AD have impaired familiarity. With the caveat that one does not know how many participants will eventually develop AD, as carefully considered by the authors in their discussion, these findings may represent a first step toward supporting the idea of familiarity impairment as an early and specific marker of Alzheimer's disease. However, we would like to suggest that the results do not support (nor contradict) such hypothesis. In this study, recollection and familiarity scores were derived from target faces only and were indexed by the ability to recall (or not) in which condition the face was presented (i.e., source attribution). Critically, familiarity was measured as the absence of recollection. Yet, a portion of recollected faces also probably entailed familiarity for the faces, as recollection and familiarity can co-occur. As a consequence, the contribution of familiarity may have been underestimated [6]. Moreover, even if a participant failed to recollect the encoding condition, she/he may have recalled other details associated with a target face (e.g., personal thoughts, emotional reaction). In other words, the familiarity score may encompass a portion of noncriterial recollection [7]. Finally, the poorer familiarity score in APOE ε4 carriers may actually reflect fewer instances of failed recollection. Indeed, when considering the proportion of recognized targets (i.e., hits) that were accompanied by incorrect source attribution (from the means in Table 3 [5]), it appears that failure to retrieve the source occurred for about 53% of the hits in APOE ε4 carriers versus around 61% in noncarriers. So, if this difference is significant, the poorer familiarity sore in APOE ε4 carriers may actually mean that, when correctly recognizing the faces, they retrieved the source more often.

Additional studies aiming at testing the hypothesis that familiarity is selectively impaired in the initial stages of AD are warranted as this is a promising avenue for early detection of AD. The strongest piece of evidence in favor of this hypothesis would be the demonstration of a selective deficit of familiarity in a task that provides a pure measure of this memory function in participants who are shown to have incipient AD, that is, cognitively healthy individuals who are positive on biomarkers of AD [8] and show cognitive decline in longitudinal assessments.

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by the Alzheimer's Association (grant number 2016-NIRG-394141).

References

  • 1.Didic M., Barbeau E.J., Felician O., Tramoni E., Guedj E., Poncet M. Which memory system is impaired first in Alzheimer's disease? J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27:11–22. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bowles B., Crupi C., Mirsattari S.M., Pigott S.E., Parrent A.G., Pruessner J.C. Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporal-lobe resection that spares the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:16382–16387. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705273104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Koen J.D., Yonelinas A.P. The effects of healthy aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease on recollection and familiarity: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24:332–354. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9266-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schoemaker D., Gauthier S., Pruessner J.C. Recollection and familiarity in aging individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a literature review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24:313–331. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9265-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schoemaker D., Poirier J., Escobar S., Gauthier S., Pruessner J. Selective familiarity deficits in otherwise cognitively intact aging individuals with genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's and dementia: diagnosis. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2016;2:132–139. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jacoby L.L., Yonelinas A.P., Jennings J.M. The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: a declaration of independence. In: Cohen J.D., Schooler J.W., editors. Scientific approaches to consciousness. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 1997. pp. 13–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yonelinas A.P., Jacoby L.L. Noncriterial recollection: familiarity as automatic, irrelevant recollection. Conscious Cogn. 1996;5:131–141. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sperling R.A., Aisen P.S., Beckett L.A., Bennett D.A., Craft S., Fagan A.M. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Alzheimer's & Dementia : Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES