Abstract
Background:
The conflicting results from studies on socioeconomic status (SES) and multiple sclerosis (MS) risk might be due to a change in the distribution of environmental exposures over time or to methodological limitations in previous research.
Objective:
To examine the association between SES and MS risk during 50 years.
Methods:
We included patients registered in Norwegian MS registries and prevalence studies born between 1930 and 1979, and identified their siblings and parents using the Norwegian Population Registry. Information on education was retrieved from the National Education Registry, categorized into four levels (primary, secondary, undergraduate and graduate) and compared in patients and siblings using conditional logistic regression.
Results:
A total of 4494 MS patients and 9193 of their siblings were included in the analyses. Level of education was inversely associated with MS risk (p trend < 0.001) with an odds ratio (OR) of 0.73 (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.59–0.90) when comparing the highest and lowest levels. The effect estimates did not vary markedly between participants born before or after the median year of birth (1958), but we observed a significant effect modification by parental education (p = 0.047).
Conclusion:
Level of education was inversely associated with MS risk, and the estimates were similar in the earliest and latest birth cohorts.
Keywords: Multiple sclerosis, epidemiology, risk factors, socioeconomic status, education, environmental risk factors
Introduction
Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a demyelinating disease of the central nervous system with unknown aetiology, and is one of few diseases, including some allergic and autoimmune diseases,1 that have been associated with higher socioeconomic status (SES). For MS, early studies observed a higher disease risk among professional workers than in unskilled workers, which suggested that exposures associated with higher SES increased disease risk.2,3 Similarly, Kurtzke and Page4 observed a positive association between SES and MS risk in a large nested case–control study. More recent studies, however, have not been able to reproduce these findings, and some have arrived at opposite conclusions.5–7 This could reflect a change over time in the distribution of exposures relevant for MS or country-specific differences in access to education and associations between education and such exposures. Still, the heterogeneity observed could also be due to methodological limitations in previous research. Several studies have used study designs that are prone to selection and recall bias, which may have contributed to the apparently conflicting results.
To address this, we conducted a large population-based study using national registries to collect objective and reliable information on level of completed education, a valid marker of SES,8 on close to 4500 MS patients, their siblings and their parents over a period of 50 years.
Methods
Study population and design
We used The Norwegian MS Registry9 as the primary source to identify MS patients. This registry was established in 2001, enrols MS patients in all of Norway and currently includes close to 6000 patients, which corresponds to approximately 60% of all MS patients in Norway. We further used the Oslo MS Registry,10 a registry that was established in 1990, which enrols MS patients living in Oslo and included approximately 1200 MS patients who were not registered in the national MS registry at the time of data extraction. Moreover, available prevalence and hospital record studies11–15 were used to identify patients not registered in the registries, as described in previous studies using the same cohort of patients.16,17 All patients had been diagnosed according to the criteria of Poser et al.18 or McDonald19 and were born between 1930 and 1979. In total, the cohort included 6928 MS patients.
Identified patients were linked to the Norwegian Population registry (Statistics Norway). This registry was established in 1964 and includes national identification numbers for the total Norwegian population alive since 1960.20 We used the patients’ identification number to retrieve information on sex and birth year of their unaffected siblings and parents. A total of 9346 siblings without MS, 5711 mothers and 5611 fathers were identified and included in the cohort. The number of siblings per MS patients ranged from 1 to 13. The siblings were included as controls, while the parents were included in analyses of effect modification. The national identification numbers of all study participants were then linked to the National Education Registry, and information on levels of completed education was retrieved for all participants for whom this information was available. This registry was established in 1970 and contains information on the highest level of completed education on the total Norwegian population alive on or after this year.21 Use of the national identification number ensured that no participant was included more than once.
Due to the dependency between the cases and controls in our study, the educational level of each MS case was compared to the level of their own siblings. Thus, patients with no siblings were excluded from the analyses. A total of 4502 MS patients had one or more siblings. Among those, information on completed education was available for 4494 patients. Educational level was available for 9193 of the siblings. In the families that were included in the analyses, educational level was available for 4297 of the fathers and 4384 of the mothers. Parental education from at least one of the parents was available for 4429 of these families.
Statistical analysis
Categories for highest level of completed education were generated based on the information provided by the National Education Registry. The new variable took value 1 for primary level (10 years or less), 2 for secondary level (11–13 years), 3 for undergraduate level (14–17 years) and 4 for graduate level (18 years or more). A variable that combined parental education was generated, and it took level 1 if at least one of the parents had higher education and 0 otherwise. Higher education was defined as undergraduate and graduate levels.
The association between disease and exposure was estimated as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) matching the cases with their siblings using conditional logistic regression. Level of education was included as a categorical variable in this model, and we compared the different levels of education to the reference level, which was the lowest level. As we are using a conditional regression model comparing cases with their own siblings, the analyses are adjusted for parental education and family size by design. In a multivariate model, we adjusted for birth order (1, 2, 3 and >3) and residency at birth (North and South). North was defined as one of the three most Northern counties in Norway (Nordland, Troms and Finmark) and South was defined as any of the other counties. We tested for interaction on the multiplicative scale by introducing an interaction term into the regression model. Specifically, we tested for interaction between level of education and sex, parental education and time periods for year of births. The time periods included birth years before and after 1958, which was the median birth year for the participants. We tested for p trend by including level of education as a continuous variable in the model. All analyses were adjusted for sex and age in 5-year intervals.
The statistical analyses were performed in Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp 2015.
Ethical approval
The study was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REK Nord).
Results
Table 1 provides the baseline characteristics of the study population according to the level of education. Those with highest level of education had fewer siblings and were more likely to be first born and to have parents with higher education. The overall distribution of level of education among patients, siblings and parents is provided in Table 2.
Table 1.
Primary | Secondary | Undergraduate | Graduate | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Cases | ||||
No. of participants | 1003 | 2186 | 1050 | 255 |
Year of birth, mean | 1958.3 | 1959.2 | 1961.8 | 1961.8 |
No. of siblings, mean | 2.2 | 2.0 | 2.0 | 1.9 |
First born, % | 31.4 | 37.9 | 39.5 | 41.2 |
Female, % | 64.7 | 63.7 | 75.2 | 58.0 |
Born in North tier,a % | 14.3 | 12.6 | 14.2 | 11.7 |
Parents with higher education,b % | 4.6 | 11.0 | 29.0 | 50.8 |
Controls | ||||
No. of participants | 2104 | 4431 | 2063 | 595 |
Year of birth, mean | 1958.6 | 1959.5 | 1961.5 | 1961.3 |
No. of siblings, mean | 3.0 | 2.8 | 2.6 | 2.5 |
First born, % | 27.7 | 31.3 | 32.7 | 35.0 |
Female, % | 47.6 | 44.7 | 59.6 | 36.0 |
Born in North tier,a % | 15.0 | 14.3 | 17.6 | 12.5 |
Parents with higher education,b % | 5.0 | 9.7 | 26.4 | 46.6 |
Born in one of the three most Northern counties in Norway (Nordland, Troms and Finmark).
Defined as at least one parent with undergraduate or graduate level of education.
Table 2.
Level of education |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Primary | Secondary | Undergraduate | Graduate | Missinga | |
Cases, % | 22.3 | 48.6 | 23.3 | 5.7 | 0.2 |
Family | |||||
Siblings, % | 22.5 | 47.4 | 22.1 | 6.4 | 1.6 |
Mothers, % | 49.2 | 40.6 | 7.2 | 0.4 | 2.6 |
Fathers, % | 40.5 | 42.5 | 8.0 | 4.4 | 4.6 |
MS: multiple sclerosis.
Missing values on level of education.
We found a statistically significant inverse association between level of education and risk of MS (p trend < 0.001; Table 3). Participants with the highest level of education had a lower MS risk compared to those with the lowest level (OR: 0.73, 95% CI: 0.59–0.90). Undergraduate level of education was also significantly associated with lower MS risk (OR: 0.82, 95% CI: 0.72–0.95).
Table 3.
No. of cases/controls | OR (95% CI)a |
||
---|---|---|---|
Multivariate adjustedb | Multivariate adjustedc | ||
Level of education | |||
Primary | 1003/2104 | Ref. | Ref. |
Secondary | 2186/4431 | 0.96 (0.87–1.07) | 0.96 (0.86–1.07) |
Undergraduate | 1050/2063 | 0.82 (0.72–0.95) | 0.82 (0.72–0.95) |
Graduate | 255/595 | 0.73 (0.59–0.90) | 0.73 (0.59–0.90) |
p trend | <0.001 | <0.001 |
MS: multiple sclerosis; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval.
Effect estimates calculated using conditional logistic regression comparing level of education in patients with the level of their own siblings.
Adjusted for age, sex, parental education and number of siblings.
Further adjusted for birth order and residency at birth.
We observed significant effect modification by parental education (p = 0.047). The highest level of education was significantly associated with a decreased MS risk among participants who had no parents with higher education (OR highest vs lowest level: 0.65, 95% CI: 0.50–0.84; Table 4), but not among those with one or more parents with higher education (OR highest vs lowest level: 1.29, 95% CI: 0.79–2.09; Table 4). We further restricted the analyses to those born before and after the median year of birth (1958), and observed that there was only a significant effect modification by parental education in the last period (p = 0.006). There was no significant difference between women (OR highest vs lowest level: 0.61, 95% CI: 0.43–0.85) and men (OR highest vs lowest level: 0.81, 95% CI: 0.54–1.21) (p for effect modification = 0.36). Similarly, there was no significant difference between those born before 1958 (OR highest vs lowest level: 0.72, 95% CI: 0.50–1.03; Table 5) and after 1958 (OR highest vs lowest level: 0.84, 95% CI: 0.63–1.12), (p for effect modification = 0.49).
Table 4.
Low parental education |
High parental education |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
No. of cases/controls | OR (95% CI)b | No. of cases/controls | OR (95% CI)b | |
Level of education | ||||
Primary | 924/1959 | Ref. | 44/103 | Ref. |
Secondary | 1926/3968 | 0.94 (0.84–1.06) | 237/424 | 1.50 (0.98–2.31) |
Undergraduate | 741/1513 | 0.80 (0.69–0.93) | 303/542 | 1.28 (0.82–1.98) |
Graduate | 125/316 | 0.65 (0.50–0.84) | 129/276 | 1.29 (0.79–2.09) |
p trend | <0.001 | 0.94 |
MS: multiple sclerosis; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval.
Defined as at least one parent with undergraduate or graduate level of education.
Effect estimates calculated using conditional logistic regression comparing level of education in patients with the level of their own siblings. All estimates are adjusted for age, sex and number of siblings.
Table 5.
Born in or before 1958 |
Born after 1958 |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
No. of cases/controls | OR (95% CI)a | No. of cases/controls | OR (95% CI)a | |
Level of education | ||||
Primary | 459/976 | Ref. | 544/1128 | Ref. |
Secondary | 1082/2140 | 1.00 (0.84–1.20) | 1104/2291 | 1.00 (0.85–1.17) |
Undergraduate | 386/825 | 0.81 (0.64–1.04) | 664/1238 | 0.87 (0.72–1.07) |
Graduate | 89/240 | 0.72 (0.50–1.03) | 166/355 | 0.84 (0.63–1.12) |
p trend | 0.04 | 0.11 |
MS: multiple sclerosis; OR: odds ratio; CI: confidence interval.
Effect estimates calculated using conditional logistic regression comparing level of education in patients with the level of their own siblings. All estimates are adjusted for age, sex, parental education and number of siblings.
Discussion
We observed that the level of completed education was inversely associated with MS risk in Norway. The association did not vary significantly during the study period of 50 years.
Our finding of an inverse relationship between level of education and MS risk is consistent with several recent studies. A large Norwegian prospective study with close to 400,000 participants observed a higher MS risk among the participants with lowest level of education.5 These findings were later replicated by two case–control studies from the United States and Norway.6,7
Our observations suggest that currently unknown exposures are driving the association between education and MS. The prevalence of smoking has declined substantially over the study period, and the distribution of the habit across levels of SES has changed. In the 1950s, the majority of Norwegian doctors were smokers,22 while smoking is primarily a habit associated with lower SES today. Thus, if smoking were driving the association, we would expect to observe differences between patients born before and after 1958. Furthermore, late primary infection of Epstein–Barr virus (EBV), which increases the risk of infectious mononucleosis (IM),23 is associated with higher SES24 and can therefore not explain our observations. Two recent studies observed an inverse association between education and MS that persisted after adjustment for currently known risk factors,6,7 which is consistent with our findings.
Sibling studies are prone to overmatching as siblings share a substantial amount of both genetic and environmental exposures, which reduce the power to detect differences between cases and controls. This adds weight to our findings, as we still observe a significant association between education and MS. Since siblings are likely to share fewer exposures the older they get, our findings could suggest that education is a marker of an exposure in or after the adolescence. This is consistent with studies on other known risk factors, including vitamin D25 and EBV,26 which have observed that exposure during adolescence may be especially important for future MS risk. It has recently been suggested that sodium intake27 and the intestinal microbiome28 could be relevant for later MS risk. Although these suggestions are mainly based on animal studies, both these factors could be associated with SES and provide biologically plausible pathways for a subsequently altered MS risk. Furthermore, low SES itself has also been associated with increased proinflammatory signalling, including higher levels of the proinflammatory cytokine interleukin 6 (IL-6),29 which is likely to be important for the development of autoimmune diseases.30
We observed a significant effect modification by parental education. While there was an inverse association between education and MS risk among those with parents with lower levels of education, there was no significant association in families with higher parental education. This could reflect early life exposures associated with higher SES that compete with other risk factors later in life that are associated with lower SES. A large Danish cohort study reported a lower MS risk among children of mothers with higher education, but this study investigated the association between cases and unrelated controls.31 Moreover, a case–control study in Northern California reported that both low parental and low self-education were associated with a higher MS risk.6 Overall, this could suggest that both childhood and adulthood SES are important for subsequent MS risk. Our study was not powered to test for effect modification of the education level of mothers and fathers separately.
The use of national registries for exposure information is a strength in this study, as we are not relying on recalled or self-reported information. Furthermore, as the registries contain information on the entire Norwegian population, we have complete information on all siblings of the patients. This is important for the validity of the findings, as studies on education and disease are especially prone to bias when there is a low participation rate among the participants. The likelihood of agreeing to take part in a study is affected by the educational level of those invited,32 which could induce a selection of highly educated participants into the study, especially in the less-motivated control group in a case–control study design. Furthermore, another strength of this study is that access to education was gradually equally distributed over the study period by the government agency, The Norwegian State Educational Loan Fund, thus providing universally covered and equal financial access to education.
Our study has some limitations. First, we had no information on relevant environmental exposures, like smoking, and are thus not able to adjust for this in the analysis. Still, our findings are consistent with two recent studies that were able to adjust for these.6,7 Second, as we did not have access to year of MS onset, our results could be affected by reverse causality. If a patient has an early onset of disease, it could affect their ability take part in higher education, as cognitive impairment is common early in the course of the disease.33 However, early onset of disease did not seem to drive the association between education and MS in previous studies.6,7 Furthermore, we observed a similar association with undergraduate level of education, which makes it unlikely that reverse causality can fully explain our findings. Lastly, we cannot exclude the possibility that some participants died before the educational registry was established, which would lead to a misclassification of their educational level. Still, we observed similar results in both time periods examined, which makes it unlikely that this potential misclassification play a major role in the results.
In conclusion, we observed an inverse association between level of completed education and MS risk that persisted over the whole study period of 50 years in Norway. This suggests that there has not been a shift in the direction of the association in Norway during this period.
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: EG Celius has received funding for travel, advice and speaker’s fees from Sanofi-Aventis, Merck-Serono, Genzyme, Biogen Idec, Roche, Teva, Almirall and Novartis, and received unrestricted research support from Biogen Idec and Novartis. KM Myhr has participated on scientific advisory boards for Novartis Norway, Biogen Idec, Genzyme and Roche; received funding for travel from Allergan, Bayer, Novartis, Merck-Serono and Biogen; received speaker honoraria from Allergan, Almirall, Bayer, Biogen, Genzyme, Novartis, Merck-Serono and Teva; and received unrestricted research support from Bayer, Genzyme, Novartis, Merck-Serono, Biogen, Pronova Biocare and Bergen and Norwegian MS Society. Ø Torkildsen has served on scientific advisory boards for Biogen Idec, Genzyme and Merck-Serono and received speaker honoraria and travel grants from Genzyme, Merck-Serono, Novartis and Biogen Idec.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Contributor Information
Kjetil Bjørnevik, Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway/The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Competence Center, Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway.
Trond Riise, Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway/The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Competence Center, Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway.
Espen Benjaminsen, Department of Neurology, Nordland Hospital Trust, Bodø, Norway.
Elisabeth G Celius, Department of Neurology, Oslo University Hospital, Ullevål, Oslo, Norway/Institute of Health and Society, Faculty of Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
Ole P Dahl, Department of Neurology, Namsos Hospital, Namsos, Norway.
Margitta T Kampman, Department of Neurology, University Hospital of North Norway, Tromsø, Norway.
Kristin I Løken-Amsrud, Innlandet Hospital Trust, Lillehammer, Norway.
Rune Midgard, Molde Hospital, Molde, Norway/Unit for Applied Clinical Research, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.
Kjell-Morten Myhr, The Kristian Gerhard Jebsen Centre for MS Research, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway/The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Registry and Biobank, Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway.
Øivind Torkildsen, The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Competence Center, Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway/The Kristian Gerhard Jebsen Centre for MS Research, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway.
Anita Vatne, Department of Neurology, Sørlandet Hospital Kristiansand, Kristiansand, Norway.
Nina Grytten, The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Competence Center, Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway/The Kristian Gerhard Jebsen Centre for MS Research, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway.
References
- 1. Bach JF. The effect of infections on susceptibility to autoimmune and allergic diseases. N Engl J Med 2002; 347: 911–920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Miller H, Ridley A, Schapira K. Multiple sclerosis. A note on social incidence. Br Med J 1960; 2: 343–345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Russell WR. Multiple sclerosis: Occupation and social group at onset. Lancet 1971; 2: 832–834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Kurtzke JF, Page WF. Epidemiology of multiple sclerosis in US veterans: VII. Risk factors for MS. Neurology 1997; 48: 204–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Riise T, Kirkeleit J, Aarseth JH, et al. Risk of MS is not associated with exposure to crude oil, but increases with low level of education. Mult Scler 2011; 17: 780–787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Briggs FB, Acuna BS, Shen L, et al. Adverse socioeconomic position during the life course is associated with multiple sclerosis. J Epidemiol Community Health 2014; 68: 622–629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Bjornevik K, Riise T, Cortese M, et al. Level of education and multiple sclerosis risk after adjustment for known risk factors: The EnvIMS study. Mult Scler 2016; 22: 104–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Shavers VL. Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. J Natl Med Assoc 2007; 99: 1013–1023. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Myhr KM, Grytten N, Torkildsen O, et al. The Norwegian Multiple Sclerosis Registry and Biobank. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 2015; 132: 24–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Celius EG, Smestad C. Change in sex ratio, disease course and age at diagnosis in Oslo MS patients through seven decades. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 2009; 189: 27–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Benjaminsen E, Olavsen J, Karlberg M, et al. Multiple sclerosis in the far north – Incidence and prevalence in Nordland County, Norway, 1970-2010. BMC Neurol 2014; 14: 226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Dahl OP, Aarseth JH, Myhr KM, et al. Multiple sclerosis in Nord-Trondelag County, Norway: A prevalence and incidence study. Acta Neurol Scand 2004; 109: 378–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Grytten N, Aarseth JH, Lunde HM, et al. A 60-year follow-up of the incidence and prevalence of multiple sclerosis in Hordaland County, Western Norway. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. Epub ahead of print 24 February 2015. DOI: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Risberg G, Aarseth JH, Nyland H, et al. Prevalence and incidence of multiple sclerosis in Oppland County: A cross-sectional population-based study in a landlocked county of Eastern Norway. Acta Neurol Scand 2011; 124: 250–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Vatne A, Mygland A, Ljostad U. Multiple sclerosis in Vest-Agder County, Norway. Acta Neurol Scand 2011; 123: 396–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Kampman MT, Aarseth JH, Grytten N, et al. Sex ratio of multiple sclerosis in persons born from 1930 to 1979 and its relation to latitude in Norway. J Neurol 2013; 260: 1481–1488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Grytten N, Torkildsen O, Aarseth JH, et al. Month of birth as a latitude-dependent risk factor for multiple sclerosis in Norway. Mult Scler 2013; 19: 1028–1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Poser CM, Paty DW, Scheinberg L, et al. New diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: Guidelines for research protocols. Ann Neurol 1983; 13: 227–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Polman CH, Reingold SC, Edan G, et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2005 revisions to the ‘McDonald Criteria’. Ann Neurol 2005; 58: 840–846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Furseth J, Ljones O. 50-årsjubilant med behov for oppgradering. Samf/Stat sent 2015; 1: 23–28. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Vangen T. Nasjonal utdanningsdatabase NUDB. Dokumentasjonsrapport: datavarehus for utdanningsdata, 1970-2006. Stat sent 2007; 54: 1–136. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Aaro LE, Bjartveit K, Vellar OD, et al. Smoking habits among Norwegian doctors 1974. Scand J Soc Med 1977; 5: 127–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ascherio A, Munger KL, Lunemann JD. The initiation and prevention of multiple sclerosis. Nat Rev Neurol 2012; 8: 602–612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hjalgrim H, Friborg J, Melbye M. The epidemiology of EBV and its association with malignant disease. In: Arvin A, Campadelli-Fiume G, Mocarski E, et al. (eds) Human herpesviruses: biology, therapy, and immunoprophylaxis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2007, pp. 929–959. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Cortese M, Riise T, Bjornevik K, et al. Timing of use of cod liver oil, a vitamin D source, and multiple sclerosis risk: The EnvIMS study. Mult Scler 2015; 21: 1856–1864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Thacker EL, Mirzaei F, Ascherio A. Infectious mononucleosis and risk for multiple sclerosis: A meta-analysis. Ann Neurol 2006; 59: 499–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Kleinewietfeld M, Manzel A, Titze J, et al. Sodium chloride drives autoimmune disease by the induction of pathogenic TH17 cells. Nature 2013; 496: 518–522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Berer K, Mues M, Koutrolos M, et al. Commensal microbiota and myelin autoantigen cooperate to trigger autoimmune demyelination. Nature 2011; 479: 538–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 14716–14721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Bettelli E, Carrier Y, Gao W, et al. Reciprocal developmental pathways for the generation of pathogenic effector TH17 and regulatory T cells. Nature 2006; 441: 235–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Nielsen NM, Jorgensen KT, Bager P, et al. Socioeconomic factors in childhood and the risk of multiple sclerosis. Am J Epidemiol 2013; 177: 1289–1295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Sonne-Holm S, Sorensen TI, Jensen G, et al. Influence of fatness, intelligence, education and sociodemographic factors on response rate in a health survey. J Epidemiol Community Health 1989; 43: 369–374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Zipoli V, Goretti B, Hakiki B, et al. Cognitive impairment predicts conversion to multiple sclerosis in clinically isolated syndromes. Mult Scler 2010; 16: 62–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]