Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Feb 7;6:e21543. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21543

Functional double dissociation within the entorhinal cortex for visual scene-dependent choice behavior

Seung-Woo Yoo 1, Inah Lee 1,*
Editor: Neil Burgess2
PMCID: PMC5308889  PMID: 28169828

Abstract

How visual scene memory is processed differentially by the upstream structures of the hippocampus is largely unknown. We sought to dissociate functionally the lateral and medial subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex (LEC and MEC, respectively) in visual scene-dependent tasks by temporarily inactivating the LEC and MEC in the same rat. When the rat made spatial choices in a T-maze using visual scenes displayed on LCD screens, the inactivation of the MEC but not the LEC produced severe deficits in performance. However, when the task required the animal to push a jar or to dig in the sand in the jar using the same scene stimuli, the LEC but not the MEC became important. Our findings suggest that the entorhinal cortex is critical for scene-dependent mnemonic behavior, and the response modality may interact with a sensory modality to determine the involvement of the LEC and MEC in scene-based memory tasks.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.001

Research Organism: Rat

Introduction

The visual scene appears to be a strong sensory attribute that functionally recruits the hippocampal system based on human studies (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998; Hartley et al., 2007; Hassabis et al., 2007; Staresina et al., 2011; Zeidman et al., 2015) and nonhuman primate studies (Gaffan and Harrison, 1989; Wirth et al., 2003). Memory for the visual scene may function as the linchpin for explaining the involvement of the hippocampus in various cognitive tasks requiring spatial navigation, episodic memory, and imagining future events [see Maguire and Mullally (2013) for review]. In our earlier studies, we confirmed that this involvement was also true in rats by showing that rats were impaired in making behavioral choices using background visual scenes when the dorsal hippocampus was inactivated by a GABA-A receptor agonist, muscimol (MUS) (Kim et al., 2012; Delcasso et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2014). In addition, hippocampal neurons showed significant modulation in the firing rate in a visual scene-dependent manner (Delcasso et al., 2014).

Despite the significant involvement of the hippocampus in visual scene-dependent memory, the functional significance of its upstream structures, that is, the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) and lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC), is unclear. Because the entorhinal cortex (EC) provides major inputs to the hippocampus (Lavenex and Amaral, 2000; Witter et al., 2000; Knierim et al., 2006; Kerr et al., 2007), the EC is expected to play key roles in scene memory. However, the visual scene has not been considered a key factor in most theories for understanding information processing in the hippocampal system (but see Knierim et al., 2014). Specifically, a majority of recent studies have been guided by a theory that proposed differential functions of the LEC and MEC for nonspatial and spatial memory, respectively (Hargreaves et al., 2005; Knierim et al., 2006; Henriksen et al., 2010; Ito and Schuman, 2012; Tsao et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013). According to the theory, the LEC receives more projections from the perirhinal cortex (PER), an area that is well known for its role in object recognition memory, and the LEC projects to the hippocampus and subiculum. The MEC receives most of the projections from the postrhinal cortex (POR), the rodent homolog of the parahippocampal cortex in primates and humans, and projects to the hippocampus and subiculum. It has been hypothesized that LEC-dependent nonspatial memory and MEC-dependent spatial memory are combined as an integrative memory representation within the hippocampus. Physiological studies that have reported differential functions between the LEC and MEC in recent years seemingly support this theoretical framework (Hargreaves et al., 2005; Henriksen et al., 2010; Ito and Schuman, 2012; Tsao et al., 2013). For example, the presence of grid cells in the MEC is believed to provide key evidence in support of the spatial functions of the MEC (Hafting et al., 2005), and the cells in the LEC fire in a nonspatial manner (Hargreaves et al., 2005).

According to the dominant theory described above, it is implied that the visual scene information is processed by the spatial information processing stream involving the POR and MEC (Knierim et al., 2014). That is, the POR is characterized as receiving ‘visuospatial’ sensory information, and the visual scene may belong to this information category (Burwell and Amaral, 1998). Because nonspatial information is often characterized based on individual sensory attributes (e.g., olfactory, tactile, and auditory information) from the unimodal and polymodal sensory areas (Burwell and Amaral, 1998) that may be associated more with individual objects than scenes, the existing theory may predict that visual scene-dependent memory is uniquely processed by the spatial information-processing stream involving the POR and MEC, but not the PER and LEC. Although this idea might be supported indirectly by behavioral studies testing animals with spatial or contextual memory tasks (Majchrzak et al., 2006; Hunsaker et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Morrissey and Takehara-Nishiuchi, 2014), visual scene has hardly been systematically controlled in prior studies that examined the functions of the LEC and MEC.

Another important point that has been overlooked is that sensory and motor components appear to be mixed when existing theories characterize the spatial information stream in particular. For example, if one makes lesions in the POR or MEC and obtains performance deficits in a behavioral task (usually a spatial navigation task), it is difficult to know whether the animal made errors because visuospatial sensory information processing was impaired or because the lesions damaged the response component associated with making spatial choices. The dominant theory seemingly divides the spatial and nonspatial information-processing streams based on the characteristics of sensory information (Burwell and Amaral, 1998; Witter et al., 2000; Eichenbaum et al., 2012), but this division may be an oversimplification of how the brain works. Examining this issue requires testing animals in various memory tasks that share the same sensory stimuli but require different motor responses (e.g., spatial versus nonspatial responses).

In the current study, we examined whether the LEC and MEC were differentially involved in visual scene memory by directly comparing the results of reversibly inactivating the LEC and MEC separately within the same rats. We used a scene-dependent spatial choice (SSC) task and a scene-dependent nonspatial choice (SNSC) task because, based on our prior studies (Kim et al., 2012; Delcasso et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2014), both tasks are hippocampal-dependent but they require spatial and nonspatial responses, respectively. If visual scene memory is exclusively processed in the MEC, inactivating the MEC, but not the LEC, should result in severe deficits in performance in both SSC and SNSC tasks. Here, for the first time to our knowledge, we report a functional double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in visual scene memory. Our results suggest that visual scene information is not uniquely processed in the MEC, and the type of motor response also seems to be important in determining the involvement of entorhinal subdivisions in scene memory tasks.

Results

Double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in visual scene-based choice behavior

We tested rats (n = 8) implanted with bilateral cannulae targeting both the LEC and MEC simultaneously (Figure 1A to Figure 1C) in a scene-based spatial choice (SSC) task in which the rats were required to choose either the left or right arm in a T-maze using the surrounding visual scene displayed in an array of LCD panels around the maze (Kim et al., 2012; Delcasso et al., 2014) (Figure 2A; Video 1). The rats performed at above 90% correct when they were retested after surgery with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) injected into the EC (EC-aCSF) (Figure 2B; Figure 2—source data 1). When MUS was injected into the LEC (LEC-MUS) on the next day, performance did not significantly change. However, MUS injections into the MEC (MEC-MUS) on the following day significantly impaired rats’ performance. A repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect of the drug (F(2,14) = 19.3, p<0.0001). A Bonferroni-Dunn post-hoc test revealed significant differences in performance between animals in the vehicle group and the MEC-MUS group (p<0.0001), as well as between the LEC-MUS and MEC-MUS groups (p<0.001). There were no significant differences in choice latency among the drug conditions (F(2,14) = 0.2, p=0.8; repeated-measures ANOVA), suggesting that the impairment in performance in the MEC-MUS condition was not attributable to generic sensory-motor deficits. Furthermore, the MEC-MUS did not disrupt the rat’s capability of associating spatial responses with any type of sensory stimulus because the same rats were able to make spatial choices (i.e., left and right turns) normally under the MUS conditions when tactile cues (wire mesh or sandpaper) were used as cues instead of visual scenes (F(2,18) = 1.39, p=0.27; repeated-measures ANOVA) (Figure 2C; Figure 2—source data 2).

Video 1. SSC task.

Download video file (1.3MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.006

Normal performance of rats in the SSC task. Sample trials, each associated with zebra stripes, bamboos, mountains, or pebbles scene are shown in the video.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.006

Figure 1. Bilateral cannula implantations in the LEC and MEC within the same rat and histological results.

Figure 1.

(A) Custom-made 3D-printed cannula complex. (B) Representative photomicrographs showing the cannula tracks in the LEC (upper left) and MEC (upper right) from the same rat. The numbers denote the relative positions from bregma (mm). (C) Locations of cannula tips of all rats used in the SSC task (n = 8). (D) Locations of cannula tips of all rats used in the SNSC task (n = 9). For LEC cannula locations, red circles indicate medially placed cannula positions in the LEC and blue circles indicate laterally placed cannula positions. The numbers besides the sections denote relative positions of the sections from bregma (mm).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.002

Figure 2. Visual scene-based and tactile cue-based spatial choice tasks.

Figure 2.

(A) Visual scene stimuli that were used in the SSC task. The two scenes were associated with the left turn, and the other two scenes were associated with the right turn. (B) Post-surgical performance in the SSC task. The MEC-MUS group exhibited significant performance deficits compared to the EC-aCSF and LEC-MUS groups. (C) Performance in the tactile cue-based choice task in the T-maze.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.003

Figure 2—source data 1. (SSC).
Source data for performance in the SSC task (Figure 2B).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.004
Figure 2—source data 2. (Tactile).
Source data for performance in the tactile-based spatial choice task (Figure 2C).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.005

To test whether the MEC-dependent performance deficits in the SSC task could be observed also when a visual scene must be associated with a nonspatial response, we tested a different group of rats (n = 9) in the SNSC task in which nonspatial choice behavior (digging or pushing) was required to manipulate a sand-filled jar in association with visual scenes (Lee and Shin, 2012; Lee et al., 2014) (Figure 3A; Video 2). The animals performed at approximately 90% correct when they were retested after surgery with aCSF injected into the EC. To our surprise, MUS injections into the LEC (LEC-MUS), however, significantly disrupted performance in the same rats, whereas MUS did not produce a significant impairment in performance in the MEC-MUS condition (Figure 3B; Figure 3—source data 1). A repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect of drug on performance (F(2,16) = 10.1, p=0.001). A post-hoc test (Bonferroni-Dunn) showed significant differences between the EC-aCSF and LEC groups (p<0.001) and between the LEC-MUS and MEC-MUS groups (p=0.01). No significant effect of drug was found for choice latency (F(2,16) = 0.29, p=0.74).

Video 2. SNSC task.

Download video file (8.8MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.009

Performance of rats when injected with aCSF in the EC versus MUS in the LEC in the SNSC task. Scenes are not shown in the video.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.009

Figure 3. Visual scene-dependent nonspatial memory task and behavioral performance.

Figure 3.

(A) Visual scene stimuli and their associated choice responses in the SNSC task. (B) Post-surgical performance in the SNSC task. The LEC-MUS condition induced significant performance deficits compared to the EC-aCSF condition. **p<0.01, ***p<0.0001. (C) Rats with the LEC cannulae implanted more medially showed bigger deficits in performance than those with more lateral LEC cannulae, whereas those rats were not different from each other when aCSF was injected.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.007

Figure 3—source data 1. (SNSC).
Source data for performance in the SNSC task (Figure 3B and C).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.008

Despite the significant impairment in performance with MUS injections in the LEC, some rats seemed less affected by LEC-MUS, which was noticeable in the large variance in the performance data for the LEC-MUS condition compared to the other drug conditions (Figure 3B). We examined whether there was a relationship between the locations of the cannula tip positions in the LEC and performance levels in individual rats. If one of the cannula was only placed laterally, the animal’s cannula position was still labeled lateral (e.g., rat 417 in Figure 1D). Interestingly, it turns out that the rats with their LEC cannulae implanted more laterally along the superficial layers (marked in blue circles in Figure 1D) were not as severely impaired as the animals with their LEC cannulae implanted more medially along the deeper layers (marked in red circles in Figure 1D) when performance of the two subgroups under MUS were directly compared (t(7) = −4.35, p<0.01) (Figure 3C). The two subgroups were not significantly different from each other in the aCSF condition (t(7) = 0.3, p=0.77) (Figure 3C).

The functional double dissociation between the LEC and MEC can be more clearly visible when the amount of performance deficit (measured by subtracting performance under MUS from performance under aCSF) was plotted for all MUS conditions for the LEC and MEC (Figure 4). An ANOVA was performed with the task as a between-group factor and the drug condition as a within-group factor, revealing a highly significant interaction between the task and drug conditions (F(1,15) = 34.75, p<0.0001). Post-hoc tests (Bonferroni-corrected t-tests) showed that the LEC-MUS condition produced significantly bigger deficits compared to the control condition in the SNSC task (p<0.05), but not in the SSC task (p>0.5), whereas the reverse was true in the SSC task (p=0.001 with MEC-MUS).

Figure 4. Double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in the SSC and SNSC tasks.

Figure 4.

Ordinate represents the amount of performance difference between aCSF and MUS conditions. *p<0.05, **p<0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.010

These results collectively show that the LEC and MEC were both involved in visual scene-based memory tasks, although different task demands recruited the two regions differentially. That is, the LEC was important when rats made nonspatial choices (digging versus pushing) toward the same object (sand-filled jar) using visual scenes in the background, but not for spatial choices (left turn versus right turn). Conversely, the MEC was more involved in the SSC task but not in the SNSC task.

No involvement of the LEC and MEC when an object alone cued choice behavior

It is important to note that compared to the SSC task, the SNSC task required the animal to interact with the object (sand-filled jar). Because several studies have described the roles of the LEC in item-based memory (Deshmukh and Knierim, 2011; Hunsaker et al., 2013; Keene et al., 2016), the performance deficits of the animals in the LEC-MUS group during the SNSC task might be attributable to the involvement of the object, but not necessarily to the involvement of visual scene-based memory. Therefore, to test whether the object factor alone (without scenes) could result in performance deficit, we tested subsets of rats from both tasks (n = 6 from the SSC task and n = 3 from the SNSC task) in an object-based nonspatial choice task (Figure 5A). In this task, rats were required to push or dig the sand-filled jar after sampling an object cue attached to the jar. The task thus required the rats to use the same response type (i.e., digging and pushing responses) used in the SNSC task, but instead of using background visual scenes, rats made choices using only objects as cues.

Figure 5. Illustration of the object-based choice task and post-surgical performance.

Figure 5.

(A) In the task, the rats were trained to respond either by digging or pushing the jar based on the magnetic object cue attached to the jar. (B) Post-surgical performance on the task. No significant differences were found between animals in either drug condition.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.011

Figure 5—source data 1. (Object).
Source data for performance in the object memory task (Figure 5B).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.012

When injected with aCSF in the EC after being trained to criterion in the object-based choice task, rats produced correct responses on more than 90% of the trials (Figure 5B; Figure 5—source data 1), and the injection of MUS in neither the LEC nor MEC produced performance deficits compared to the aCSF condition (F(2,16) = 1, p=0.37; repeated-measures ANOVA). These results clearly demonstrate that neither subdivision of the EC was necessary when rats used a single object to make a digging versus pushing response to the jar. The performance level under aCSF (mean = 92.9%) in the object memory task was equivalent to the performance levels of the control conditions in the SSC (mean = 92.8%) and SNSC (mean = 91.7%) tasks (F(2,23) = 0.13, p=0.88; ANOVA), suggesting that the null effect of MUS in the object memory task was not attributable to the ceiling effect for performance in the control condition. The results also demonstrate that the digging and pushing behaviors themselves were not necessarily affected by MUS injections into the LEC or MEC. Our findings thus suggest that the performance impairment observed in animals that received the MUS injection into the LEC in the SNSC task may not be solely based on the presence of the object variable compared to the SSC task.

Discussion

Visual scene memory-dependent spatial information processing in the MEC

In the current study, the inactivation of the MEC severely disrupted performance on the SSC task, but not on the SNSC task, whereas the inactivation of the LEC resulted in the opposite pattern of results. However, the MEC-MUS rats were normal in making spatial choices in response to tactile cues. These findings strongly suggest that the MEC is critically involved in using background visual scenes for making spatial responses (e.g., left or right turn response). It is possible that the rodent POR is uniquely involved in scene information processing, as in the parahippocampal cortex in humans (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998), and the MEC, a downstream structure of the POR, is also heavily involved in processing scene representations. However, because MEC-MUS rats were not impaired on the SNSC task, the MEC may not be responsible for simply processing visual scenes in any type of behavior. Instead, our SSC task required the rat to associate external visual scene information with an egocentric spatial response (left or right turn). Considering that the MEC is connected to a number of areas in the brain that process idiothetic spatial information (e.g., the parasubiculum, retrosplenial cortex, etc.) (McNaughton et al., 2006; Knierim et al., 2014), the performance deficits observed in the current study with MEC-MUS in the SSC task might be related to the roles of the MEC in associating egocentric spatial information with visual scenes. Our experimental data from the tactile memory task suggest that such associative function of the MEC is uniquely related to visual scenes because the MEC was not involved in associating tactile cues with egocentric responses. By contrast, the LEC is not connected to those egocentric spatial areas, which may explain why the LEC was not significantly involved in the SSC task. Our results suggest that the hippocampus relies heavily on MEC inputs in order for the rat to perform the SSC task normally.

Visual scene-based disambiguation of object-associated responses in the LEC

The results from the previous studies showed that the effects of the LEC lesions became more critical when objects were recognized in association with their positions in the testing arena (Hunsaker et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2013a, 2013b; Rodo et al., 2017). The significant decrease in performance in the SNSC task after MUS injections in the LEC (but not in the MEC) may support those previous findings because the rat should make discrete choices when facing the common (thus ambiguous) object in the current task, and only the visual scene information disambiguated which response was appropriate. The presence of the object by itself may not explain the MUS-induced deficits in the LEC because, as shown in prior studies (Van Cauter et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2013a, 2013b), using an object alone as a cue did not produce a significant impairment in performance in the LEC-MUS group in our object memory task. It is possible that the LEC becomes critical when object-associated ambiguity should be reduced using scene or contextual information. In a spontaneous object exploration paradigm, object ambiguity may increase as an animal experiences more objects in the same environment (Hunsaker et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013). In our study, the object-associated ambiguity may stem from the task demand that two different behavioral responses should be conditionally emitted to the same object.

In our study, the rats with LEC cannulae implanted more medially along the deeper layers showed more severe deficits than the rats with more laterally implanted cannulae in the LEC. It is possible that the inactivating agent injected through the laterally implanted cannulae near the cortical surface may have been leaked partially to the cortical surface instead of being fully injected into the LEC. It is also possible that the deeper layers of the LEC play more important roles in the SNSC task than the superficial layers of the LEC, possibly because the deeper layers of the LEC receive the inputs from the distal CA1 and thus may have access to both object and place information, which may in turn play critical roles in contextually responding to an object in different contexts.

Based on our surgical coordinates, the LEC cannulae inevitably damaged some of the auditory cortices, the TE, and the PER. However, visual cortices were mostly spared. For MEC cannulae, visual cortices were partially damaged. Despite the cortical damage, however, rats performed the tasks very well when aCSF was injected, and this normal performance in the control condition strongly argues against the possibility that MUS effects might be due to the damage in the overlying cortices. Attention and visual capabilities were apparently normal based on the performance data from the control conditions. We also saw no relationships between the amount of damage and the severity of the MUS effects in our study.

Significance and implications of the current findings

One important caveat of prior behavioral studies was that permanent lesions were produced by the injection of an excitotoxic drug (Wirth et al., 1998; Brun et al., 2008; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2013a, Wilson et al., 2013b) or by the passage of a radio-frequency current (Kesner and Giles, 1998; Parron et al., 2004; Van Cauter et al., 2013) into the LEC or MEC. Permanently damaging a region in the brain by using these methods may induce plastic changes in the functionally related neural circuits, and therefore, the results from these studies may not reflect the normal functions of a brain selectively devoid of the target region (Ramirez and Stein, 1984; Cassel et al., 1997; Lavenex et al., 2007; Braun et al., 2008; Otchy et al., 2015). The temporary inactivation of a brain region with the use of an inactivating agent, such as MUS or lidocaine, is relatively free from such caveats because normal circuits are preserved during testing. More importantly, the reversible inactivation method allows for the comparison of the normal mode of operation of the circuit (by injecting vehicle solution) with its inactivated mode within the same animal, as opposed to the between-group comparisons that can only be performed in lesion studies. Surprisingly, to our knowledge, no experimental study has used the reversible inactivation for either the LEC or MEC to examine spatial versus nonspatial memory functions in those regions, although some studies have used the methods to test the roles of the LEC in the eye-blink conditioning paradigm (Morrissey et al., 2012; Tanninen et al., 2013).

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to compare the behavioral effects of the reversible inactivation of the LEC and MEC directly within the same animals. The results of the current study largely verified experimental findings separately reported in prior studies and imply that a significant portion of the MEC-dependent deficits previously reported could be attributed to the roles of the MEC in the association of external visual contextual information with egocentric or path-integrative information (Parron et al., 2004; Hafting et al., 2005; Hargreaves et al., 2005; Brun et al., 2008; Van Cauter et al., 2013). Our findings also indicate that the LEC may play an important role in contextual response to an object using background visual scene or context. This conjecture may find support in the literature because lesions targeting the LEC did not appear to produce memory deficits when objects and places had to be remembered independently from each other, but produced deficits when objects were required to be recognized in association with places or context (Deshmukh and Knierim, 2011; Hunsaker et al., 2013; Tsao et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2013a, 2013b).

Our results, together with other studies (Lavenex and Amaral, 2000; Witter et al., 2000; Burwell et al., 2004; Hargreaves et al., 2005; Knierim et al., 2006; Kerr et al., 2007; Henriksen et al., 2010; Ito and Schuman, 2012; Hunsaker et al., 2013; Tsao et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Knierim and Neunuebel, 2016), beg the question of whether the association of the nonspatial and spatial information is the unique role of the hippocampus and suggest that its upstream areas may be engaged in such associative functions as well. The unique contribution of the hippocampus may come from other cognitive processes instead, including pattern completion and separation (Marr, 1971; Lee et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2005), temporal processing of events (Lee and Wilson, 2002; Eichenbaum, 2014; Allen et al., 2016), and value-based decision making (Lee et al., 2012; Mizumori and Tryon, 2015; Palombo et al., 2015; Schumacher et al., 2016), when receiving inputs from upstream structures. Our findings also strongly suggest that behavioral response components should be incorporated when theorizing the functions of different regions in the medial temporal lobe. For example, an excellent review by Knierim et al. (2014) incorporated path integration and movement information to the spatial information processing stream. Our study suggests that, instead of representing ‘what’ information, the LEC may process ‘what should I do to this object in this context?', whereas the MEC may represent ‘where should I go from here in this context?'

The results of the current study, together with our prior studies (Kim et al., 2012; Ahn and Lee, 2014; Delcasso et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2014), suggest that visual scene memory tasks provide valuable opportunities for functionally dissociating different areas of the hippocampal memory system. This utility may be important particularly for examining the functions of the EC because prior behavioral studies have taught us that results may be highly variable if a behavioral task allows the animal to choose from multiple strategies (Johnson and Kesner, 1994; Pouzet et al., 1999; Burwell et al., 2004; Hunsaker et al., 2013; Van Cauter et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2013a, 2013b; Rodo et al., 2017). The scene memory tasks used in the current study do not allow rats to solve the task other than using the surrounding visual scenes in the background and may suffer less from such confounding factors.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Seventeen male Long-Evans rats (RRID:RGD_68074) weighing 340–400 g were used. All animals were housed individually and underwent a two-week acclimation period in Plexiglas cages in a temperature- (26°C) and humidity-controlled environment (40–60%). Rats were kept on a 12 hr light-dark cycle. For behavioral experiments, all animals were food-restricted to 80% of their free-feeding weights with free access to water. All protocols (SNU-120925-1-7) were in compliance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Seoul National University.

Behavioral apparatus

A T-shaped linear track (86 × 40 cm) elevated to 88 cm above the floor was used in a visual scene-based spatial choice (SSC) task in the current study (Delcasso et al., 2014). A guillotine door-operated start box (22.5 cm x 16 cm x 31.5 cm) was attached to the end of the stem of the T-track. A food well (2.5 cm in diameter and 0.8 cm in depth) was placed at the end of each arm of the T-track. Three 17-inch LCD monitors were installed as an array to surround the upper portion of the T-track to provide visual background scenes. Four optic fiber sensors (Autonics, Korea) were installed on both side walls of the linear track at different points (1 cm, 27 cm, 47 cm and 67 cm from the start box) to record the animal’s position on the track. The activation of the first optic fiber sensor triggered the onset of the scene stimuli on the LCD monitors. The apparatus was surrounded by black curtains, and no external visual cues were provided on the curtains. The experiment room was dimly lit by an overhead array of LED lights (four lx) and two loud speakers played white noise (80 dB) during behavioral sessions to mask environmental noise. A digital camera installed on the ceiling recorded the behavioral testing sessions and transferred the data to a PC outside the testing room.

The T-track was also used in a scene-based nonspatial choice (SNSC) task. In this task, however, the two arms of the track were made unavailable to the rat by the insertion of a linear track (82.5 cm x 7.5 cm). Two side walls (each 10 cm x 7.5 cm x 0.7 cm) and a food well were found at the end of the track. A wide-mouth amber glass jar (5.8 cm in diameter and 7 cm in height) was placed above the food well. The amber jar contained play sand mixed with cereal powder (Froot Loops, Kellogg’s, USA) at a 7:1 ratio.

An object-based nonspatial choice task was conducted in a rectangular acrylic box (55 cm x 23 cm x 23 cm) made of transparent red acrylic panels. A guillotine door-operated start box (22.5 cm x 16 cm x 31.5 cm) was attached to the end of the acrylic box. A linear track (52 cm x 20 cm) was placed inside the box by installing side walls (each 52 cm x 8 cm x 0.5 cm). A food well was placed 10 cm from the end of the linear track. The wide-mouth amber glass jar (filled with play sand and cereal powder) was also used in the task, and a magnet was attached to the glass jar so that an object could be attached to the jar during the task.

Handling and familiarization

Naïve rats were handled by an experimenter for 30 min a day for 3 days. Following the handling phase, rats were placed on a lab cart (99 cm x 45 cm x 84 cm) to forage for multiple pieces of cereal scattered on the surface of the cart. After acclimation to the environment during the foraging session on the cart for 3 days, rats were familiarized to the experimental room and the behavioral apparatus. During this period, rats were allowed to freely explore the apparatus while consuming cereal pieces scattered over the track and in the food wells. Once rats consumed over 80 pieces of cereal within 30 min for two consecutive days, a shaping phase began.

Shaping

  1. SSC task: A rat was first placed in the start box. When the start box door was opened by an experimenter, the rat exited the start box. Once the rat reached the choice point of the T-track, the rat was trained to enter the arm in which a silver metal washer (4 cm in diameter, 0.5 cm in height) had been placed over the food well. Once the rat displaced the washer, a quarter of a piece of cereal was found. The rat was allowed to eat it and was then gently guided back to the start box by the experimenter. The food well in the opposite, unrewarded arm was covered with a black metal washer. Rats were trained until the following criteria were satisfied. First, the median choice latency (from the opening of the start-box door to the displacement of the washer) was less than 5 s. Second, rats could correctly remove the silver washer covering the baited food well in more than 20 out of 40 trials. A daily session was finished when the rat conducted 40 trials or when 30 m had passed, whichever came first.

  2. SNSC task: A trial began when the experimenter opened the start box. Once exiting the start box, the rat was required to respond by either digging into or pushing the amber jar to obtain a food reward. To shape the digging behavior, rats were allowed to find a piece of cereal placed on the top of the play sand in the jar. The cereal reward was then gradually hidden in the sand over many trials and was eventually completely hidden in the sand (1 cm from the surface of the sand). When the rat found the cereal reward, it was allowed to consume it and was then gently guided back into the start box. For pushing behavior, in the earlier shaping stage, the food well was half-closed by a glass jar so that the rat could see the food reward. The food well was gradually closed by the glass jar over the course of many trials and was eventually completely closed. To shape both digging and pushing responses, rats were trained until the following two criteria were satisfied: first, the median latency from the opening of the start box door to the moment the rat received food reward was less than 5 s. Second, the rat could completely displace the glass jar covering the baited food well (for the pushing response) or could find a food reward completely hidden in the sand in more than 20 out of 40 trials. A daily session was forcibly finished when the rat completed 40 trials or when 30 m passed, whichever came first. Once the criteria were met, pre-surgical training for the main task began.

Pre-surgical training

  1. SSC task: A trial began when the rat exited the start box and triggered a sensor placed 1 cm from the door. The activity of the sensor led to a visual scene display (e.g., pebbles or zebra stripes) on the LCD monitors. Rats were trained to choose the right arm for the pebbles scene and the left arm for the zebra stripes scene. A correct choice resulted in a piece of cereal available in the food well (when displaced by the rat). The rat was trained to consume the reward in the start box after being guided back into the start box. A wrong choice resulted in the rat being returned to the start box without a reward. Each scene appeared, in a pseudo-randomly intermixed fashion for 20 trials in a daily session. The rat was trained until its performance level reached greater than 80% correct responses per scene or above 75% correct responses on average for all scenes, with a response bias of less than 0.15. The response bias was calculated by first subtracting the number of right response-associated trials from left response-associated trials and then dividing the difference by the sum of the trials. When the rat reached the performance criterion for the first pair of scene stimuli for two consecutive days, the animal was trained with the second pair of scene stimuli, e.g., mountain and bamboo patterns, until it reached the same criterion level with the first pair of scene stimuli. Then, when the rat reached the performance criteria for both the first and the second scene pairs, all four scenes were pseudo-randomly presented across trials to train the rat until it reached greater than 80% correct responses for each scene.

  2. SNSC task: Upon opening the start box door and the rat exiting the start box, one of the first pair of scene stimuli, pebbles or zebra stripes, was displayed on the LCD monitors in a pseudo-random sequence. Rats were trained to respond by either digging in the sand or pushing the jar in association with the scene stimulus (e.g., pushing for pebbles and digging for zebra stripes) for 40 trials a day. If the rat stood up and touched the sand with both paws, it was recorded as a digging response. If the rat moved the jar with its snout (even if the food well was not exposed), it was recorded as a pushing response. No correction was allowed when the rat made an error. When the rats reached the performance criterion for the first pair of scene stimuli for two consecutive days, the second pair of scene stimuli (e.g., palm trees and peacock feathers) was introduced for training. The performance criterion for each pair of scene stimuli was greater than 80% correct responses per scene or greater than 75% correct responses for the overall scenes, with a response bias of less than 0.15. When the rats reached the performance criterion for the second pair of scene stimuli, they were trained to the criterion (>80% correct response for each scene) for all four scenes.

Surgery

Once the rats reached the performance criteria for four scene stimuli for two consecutive days, a 3D-printed cannula complex (20 mm x 9 mm x 26 mm) was surgically implanted. The 3D complex was composed of four stainless steel guide cannulae targeting both the lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) and medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) bilaterally. First, the rat was deeply anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbital (Nembutal, 65 mg/kg), and its head was fixed in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments, USA). Anesthesia was then maintained by isoflurane inhalation (0.5–2% isoflurane mixed with 100% O2) throughout the surgery. An incision was made along the midline of the scalp and the skull was exposed. The skull surface was leveled after adjusting the levels of bregma and lambda on the same horizontal plane. Six small burr holes were made on the skull to place anchoring skull screws to hold the 3D-printed cannula complex with dental cement. Four burr holes were also drilled to insert the guide cannulae of the cannula complex into the LEC and the MEC at the following coordinates: 5.9 mm posterior to bregma, 6.8 mm lateral to midline, 6.4 mm ventral from dura for the LEC, and 7.8 mm posterior to bregma, 4.8 mm lateral to midline, 5.9 mm ventral from dura. Following insertion of the guide cannulae into the target areas, medical grade silicone (Kwik-Sil, World Precision Instruments, USA) was applied to fill the burr holes to block the influx of dental cement into the exposed area of the brain during the cementing procedures. Dental cement was then applied to the skull surface to firmly hold the 3D-printed cannula complex. The rat was allowed to recover for 7 days before behavioral testing began.

Post-surgical testing and drug injection

For both SSC (n = 8) and SNSC (n = 9) tasks, after recovery from surgery, rats were retrained to the pre-surgical criteria with the four scene stimuli for two consecutive days. Then, on the next day, artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) was injected into the EC (i.e., both the LEC and the MEC, 0.3 μL per site) 20 m before behavioral testing. The vehicle solution was injected into both the LEC and MEC in the current study because our prior pilot studies showed no difference in performance between the two conditions. More importantly, prolonging the testing period with aCSF might carry the risk of overtraining the rats, which we wanted to avoid to properly test the functions of the LEC and MEC while those areas were engaged in the normal performance of the task. Over the next two days, the same animal received an injection of the GABA-A receptor agonist muscimol (MUS) into the LEC and then into the MEC (0.3 μL per site), or vice versa (the injection sequence was counterbalanced among the rats).

Histology

When all experimental testing was completed, fluorophore-conjugated muscimol (f-MUS; Sigma, USA) was injected bilaterally into both the LEC and MEC (0.3 μL per site) to verify the diffusion range of the inactivating agent. Twenty minutes after the injection of f-MUS, rats were killed by CO2 overdose and were perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered saline, followed by a 4% v/v formaldehyde solution. The brain was extracted from the skull and was soaked in 4% v/v formaldehyde-30% sucrose solution at 4°C until it completely sank. The brain was then gelatin-coated and soaked again in a 4% v/v formaldehyde solution-30% sucrose solution at 4°C until it completely sank. Then, the brain was cut into 40 mm coronal sections on a freezing microtome (HM 430, Thermo Fisher Scientific, USA). Every second section was collected for thionin staining to verify the position of the 3D complex cannula, and every third section was used to examine the diffusion of f-MUS with a fluorescent microscope (Eclipse 80i, Nikon, Japan).

Data analysis

Performance was quantified as the proportion of correct trials in each session. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to examine the effects of the drugs. A t-test corrected for multiple comparisons was used to examine the presence of any significant differences according to injection sequences. The response latency was defined as the duration from the time that the animal exited the start box to the time when the animal began to exhibit behavioral responses to obtain food rewards. The proportions of correct trials over the overall trials were regarded as behavioral performance assessments for each session.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Brain Research Program (NRF-2015M3C7A1031969), the Basic Science Research Program (NRF-2013R1A1A2062882 and NRF-2016R1A2B4008692), the Science Research Center grant (2014051826), the BK21+ program through the National Research Foundation of Korea. We thank Choong-Hee Lee for his statistical help and Eun-Hye Park for her helpful comments for the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Research Foundation of Korea 2013R1A1A2062882 to Inah Lee.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea 2016R1A2B4008692 to Inah Lee.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea 2014051826 to Inah Lee.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea BK21 plus program to Inah Lee.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea 2015M3C7A1031969 to Inah Lee.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

S-WY, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration.

IL, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Seoul National University. All protocols (SNU-120925-1-7) were in compliance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Seoul National University.

References

  1. Ahn JR, Lee I. Intact CA3 in the Hippocampus is only sufficient for contextual behavior based on well-learned and unaltered visual background. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1081–1093. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen TA, Salz DM, McKenzie S, Fortin NJ. Nonspatial sequence coding in CA1 neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:1547–1563. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2874-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Braun M, Finke C, Ostendorf F, Lehmann TN, Hoffmann KT, Ploner CJ. Reorganization of associative memory in humans with long-standing hippocampal damage. Brain. 2008;131:2742–2750. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brun VH, Leutgeb S, Wu HQ, Schwarcz R, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB. Impaired spatial representation in CA1 after lesion of direct input from entorhinal cortex. Neuron. 2008;57:290–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Burwell RD, Amaral DG. Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, Postrhinal, and entorhinal cortices of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1998;398:179–205. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980824)398:2&#x0003c;179::AID-CNE3&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burwell RD, Saddoris MP, Bucci DJ, Wiig KA. Corticohippocampal contributions to spatial and contextual learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:3826–3836. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0410-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cassel JC, Duconseille E, Jeltsch H, Will B. The fimbria-fornix/cingular bundle pathways: a review of neurochemical and behavioural approaches using lesions and transplantation techniques. Progress in Neurobiology. 1997;51:663–716. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00009-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Delcasso S, Huh N, Byeon JS, Lee J, Jung MW, Lee I. Functional relationships between the Hippocampus and dorsomedial striatum in learning a visual scene-based memory task in rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:15534–15547. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0622-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Deshmukh SS, Knierim JJ. Representation of non-spatial and spatial information in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;5:69. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eichenbaum H, Sauvage M, Fortin N, Komorowski R, Lipton P. Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36:1597–1608. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Eichenbaum H. Time cells in the Hippocampus: a new dimension for mapping memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15:732–744. doi: 10.1038/nrn3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Epstein R, Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature. 1998;392:598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gaffan D, Harrison S. Place memory and scene memory: effects of fornix transection in the monkey. Experimental Brain Research. 1989;74:202–212. doi: 10.1007/BF00248293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hargreaves EL, Rao G, Lee I, Knierim JJ. Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal Hippocampus. Science. 2005;308:1792–1794. doi: 10.1126/science.1110449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hartley T, Bird CM, Chan D, Cipolotti L, Husain M, Vargha-Khadem F, Burgess N. The Hippocampus is required for short-term topographical memory in humans. Hippocampus. 2007;17:34–48. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Vann SD, Maguire EA. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. PNAS. 2007;104:1726–1731. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610561104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Henriksen EJ, Colgin LL, Barnes CA, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Spatial representation along the proximodistal Axis of CA1. Neuron. 2010;68:127–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hunsaker MR, Chen V, Tran GT, Kesner RP. The medial and lateral entorhinal cortex both contribute to contextual and item recognition memory: a test of the binding of items and context model. Hippocampus. 2013;23:380–391. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ito HT, Schuman EM. Functional division of hippocampal area CA1 via modulatory gating of entorhinal cortical inputs. Hippocampus. 2012;22:372–387. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johnson DL, Kesner RP. The effects of lesions of the entorhinal cortex and the horizontal nucleus of the diagonal band of broca upon performance of a spatial location recognition task. Behavioural Brain Research. 1994;61:1–8. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90002-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Keene CS, Bladon J, McKenzie S, Liu CD, O'Keefe J, Eichenbaum H. Complementary functional organization of neuronal activity patterns in the perirhinal, lateral entorhinal, and medial entorhinal cortices. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:3660–3675. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4368-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kerr KM, Agster KL, Furtak SC, Burwell RD. Functional neuroanatomy of the parahippocampal region: the lateral and medial entorhinal areas. Hippocampus. 2007;17:697–708. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kesner RP, Giles R. Neural circuit analysis of spatial working memory: role of pre- and parasubiculum, medial and lateral entorhinal cortex. Hippocampus. 1998;8:416–423. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:4&#x0003c;416::AID-HIPO9&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kim S, Lee J, Lee I. The Hippocampus is required for visually cued contextual response selection, but not for visual discrimination of contexts. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;6:66. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Knierim JJ, Lee I, Hargreaves EL. Hippocampal place cells: parallel input streams, subregional processing, and implications for episodic memory. Hippocampus. 2006;16:755–764. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Knierim JJ, Neunuebel JP, Deshmukh SS. Functional correlates of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference frames. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130369. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Knierim JJ, Neunuebel JP. Tracking the flow of hippocampal computation: pattern separation, pattern completion, and attractor dynamics. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2016;129:38–49. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lavenex P, Amaral DG. Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: a hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus. 2000;10:420–430. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4&#x0003c;420::AID-HIPO8&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lavenex P, Lavenex PB, Amaral DG. Spatial relational learning persists following neonatal hippocampal lesions in macaque monkeys. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:234–239. doi: 10.1038/nn1820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the Hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron. 2002;36:1183–1194. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lee H, Ghim JW, Kim H, Lee D, Jung M. Hippocampal neural correlates for values of experienced events. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:15053–15065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2806-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lee I, Shin JY. Medial prefrontal cortex is selectively involved in response selection using visual context in the background. Learning & Memory. 2012;19:247–250. doi: 10.1101/lm.025890.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lee I, Yoganarasimha D, Rao G, Knierim JJ. Comparison of population coherence of place cells in hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3. Nature. 2004;430:456–459. doi: 10.1038/nature02739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lee KJ, Park SB, Lee I. Elemental or contextual? it depends: individual difference in the hippocampal dependence of associative learning for a simple sensory stimulus. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:217. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Barnes CA, Moser EI, McNaughton BL, Moser MB. Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science. 2005;309:619–623. doi: 10.1126/science.1114037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maguire EA, Mullally SL. The Hippocampus: a manifesto for change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013;142:1180–1189. doi: 10.1037/a0033650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Majchrzak M, Ferry B, Marchand AR, Herbeaux K, Seillier A, Barbelivien A. Entorhinal cortex lesions disrupt fear conditioning to background context but spare fear conditioning to a tone in the rat. Hippocampus. 2006;16:114–124. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1971;262:23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser MB. Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map'. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:663–678. doi: 10.1038/nrn1932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mizumori SJ, Tryon VL. Integrative hippocampal and decision-making neurocircuitry during goal-relevant predictions and encoding. Progress in Brain Research. 2015;219:217–242. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Morrissey MD, Maal-Bared G, Brady S, Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Functional dissociation within the entorhinal cortex for memory retrieval of an association between temporally discontiguous stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:5356–5361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5227-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Morrissey MD, Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Diversity of mnemonic function within the entorhinal cortex: a meta-analysis of rodent behavioral studies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2014;115:95–107. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Otchy TM, Wolff SB, Rhee JY, Pehlevan C, Kawai R, Kempf A, Gobes SM, Ölveczky BP. Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulations. Nature. 2015;528:358–363. doi: 10.1038/nature16442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Palombo DJ, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. How does the Hippocampus shape decisions? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2015;125:93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Parron C, Poucet B, Save E. Entorhinal cortex lesions impair the use of distal but not proximal landmarks during place navigation in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research. 2004;154:345–352. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pouzet B, Welzl H, Gubler MK, Broersen L, Veenman CL, Feldon J, Rawlins JN, Yee BK. The effects of NMDA-induced retrohippocampal lesions on performance of four spatial memory tasks known to be sensitive to hippocampal damage in the rat. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;11:123–140. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00413.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ramirez JJ, Stein DG. Sparing and recovery of spatial alternation performance after entorhinal cortex lesions in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 1984;13:53–61. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(84)90029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rodo C, Sargolini F, Save E. Processing of spatial and non-spatial information in rats with lesions of the medial and lateral entorhinal cortex: environmental complexity matters. Behavioural Brain Research. 2017;320:200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schumacher A, Vlassov E, Ito R. The ventral Hippocampus, but not the dorsal Hippocampus is critical for learned approach-avoidance decision making. Hippocampus. 2016;26:530–542. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Staresina BP, Duncan KD, Davachi L. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices differentially contribute to later recollection of object- and scene-related event details. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8739–8747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4978-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tanninen SE, Morrissey MD, Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Unilateral lateral entorhinal inactivation impairs memory expression in trace eyeblink conditioning. PLoS One. 2013;8:e84543. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tsao A, Moser MB, Moser EI. Traces of experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Current Biology. 2013;23:399–405. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Van Cauter T, Camon J, Alvernhe A, Elduayen C, Sargolini F, Save E. Distinct roles of medial and lateral entorhinal cortex in spatial cognition. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:451–459. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wilson DI, Langston RF, Schlesiger MI, Wagner M, Watanabe S, Ainge JA. Lateral entorhinal cortex is critical for novel object-context recognition. Hippocampus. 2013a;23:352–366. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wilson DI, Watanabe S, Milner H, Ainge JA. Lateral entorhinal cortex is necessary for associative but not nonassociative recognition memory. Hippocampus. 2013b;23:1280–1290. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wirth S, Ferry B, Di Scala G. Facilitation of olfactory recognition by lateral entorhinal cortex lesion in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 1998;91:49–59. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00102-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wirth S, Yanike M, Frank LM, Smith AC, Brown EN, Suzuki WA. Single neurons in the monkey Hippocampus and learning of new associations. Science. 2003;300:1578–1581. doi: 10.1126/science.1084324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Witter MP, Wouterlood FG, Naber PA, Van Haeften T. Anatomical organization of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2000;911:1–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Zeidman P, Mullally SL, Maguire EA. Constructing, perceiving, and maintaining scenes: hippocampal activity and connectivity. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:3836–3855. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Feb 7;6:e21543. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21543.013

Decision letter

Editor: Neil Burgess1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Functional double dissociation within the entorhinal cortex for visual scene-dependent choice behavior" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Sabine Kastner as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: Alex Easton (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The referees find this an interesting inactivation study in the topical area of differentiating the functions of lateral and medial Entorhinal cortex (LEC and MEC). However, both have some significant issues that need to be addressed (see comments copied below).

One concerns the power of the results, specifically the weakness of the LEC inactivation effect: the fact that it is only present in 3 out of 6 animals, and might potentially be due to spread of muscimol to area X, where area X is closer to LEC than to MEC (and influences digging/ pushing) in some animals. The issue being that the much smaller non-flourescent muscimol molecules would diffuse further than shown by the f-muscimol. Is a solution to perform the behavioral study using f-muscimol in a few more animals? Referee 3 requests a diagram showing implantation points for all animals, for comparison with the supplemental data on performance regarding this point.

The other concerns the interpretation of the findings – what is the critical task difference: is digging (down) or pushing (ahead) really so different than turning left or right? Is this difference due to being more/less spatial? Are there any videos or descriptions of the way in which the LEC animals fail the 'non-spatial' scene task, so that we could try to understand why they fail?

Main comments (Referee 2):

This is a fascinating result from a careful piece of work and is of very high interest to a wide range of researchers. However, the implications rely heavily on the detail of the methods, which can be sparse at times. It is important that the authors clarify several things to allow the importance of the findings to be fully understood:

1) Numbers of animals in each task are small – is there sufficient power in these results? Some form of power analysis is important. If one looks at the data for (for example) the SNSC task in Figure 1, the error bars suggest that there is reasonable overlap in the performance of the groups and whilst the means are statistically different from one another, without understanding the power of the study it is possible with such low numbers that this would be a 'fluke'. Whilst the double dissociation makes this somewhat unlikely it does need to be addressed.

2) To what extent are the object and scene tasks comparable? Much of the interpretation relies on the object based version of the tasks being unaffected by temporary inactivation of either area – suggesting the effect is limited to scenes. However, power remains to be addressed (point 1) but also performance is much higher in the object task. One assumes it is easier given the stimuli are likely more discriminable. Therefore, is it that a dissociation exists between LEC and MEC in this task, but is merely hidden by ceiling effects in performance?

3) There is significant damage dorsal to the infusion sites from the cannula (one assumes). Although unlikely to have a significant bearing (as the animals can do the task when muscimol is not infused) the authors do need to consider any relevance of this extraneous damage – especially as it may be damaging attention and/or visual systems which will impact on normal task performance.

Main comments (Referee 3):

Using a within-subject design in which the same rats received local intra-cerebral injections of muscimol, the authors assessed the functional role of the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) and lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) in two tasks that both relied on the identification of visual scenes but differed in the type of response that was required. In the first (spatial) task, the visual scene told the rat whether to turn right or left in a T-maze. In the second task, the visual scene told the rat whether to push a jar or to dig in that jar. The results revealed that MEC inactivations induced a strong deficit in the first task, but not in the second task and that LEC inactivations induced a relatively smaller deficit in the second task, but no deficit in the first task. The authors' conclusions question standard hypotheses about MEC and LEC functions based on their anatomical connections and on previous behavioral data.

This is an interesting piece of work that will usefully complement the growing body of literature tackling the functional roles of the MEC and LEC. My main concern is that it does not however provide any insight into what these functions might be. While the results of the first task (scene-based spatial choice task) and clear and fit existing data, those of the second task (scene-based non-spatial choice task) are more difficult to interpret.

First, it is not clear why the authors designed this task (besides the idea of having a non-spatial task), what their prediction was, and what the specific function that seems to be handled by the LEC might be. The discussion does not provide any hint as to this last point.

Second, the data are a bit weak as the deficit induced by LEC inactivations is relatively small (as noticed by the authors) and does not appear to be very robust. Looking at the individual data of the six rats that were tested revealed virtually no deficit in 3 of the rats.

In addition to these main concerns, I would like to see a diagram showing the implantation points for all rats. Although Figure 1B is useful, it does not discard the possibility that some of the reported effects are due to diffusion of muscimol into neighboring structures (including MEC). The visualization of diffusion extent is useful but not sufficient as the molecular weight of fluorescent muscimol (ca 600) is much greater than that of regular muscimol (ca 100), leading to the possibility of underestimation of diffusion range.

eLife. 2017 Feb 7;6:e21543. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21543.014

Author response


The referees find this an interesting inactivation study in the topical area of differentiating the functions of lateral and medial Entorhinal cortex (LEC and MEC). However, both have some significant issues that need to be addressed (see comments copied below).

One concerns the power of the results, specifically the weakness of the LEC inactivation effect: the fact that it is only present in 3 out of 6 animals, and might potentially be due to spread of muscimol to area X, where area X is closer to LEC than to MEC (and influences digging/ pushing) in some animals. The issue being that the much smaller non-flourescent muscimol molecules would diffuse further than shown by the f-muscimol. Is a solution to perform the behavioral study using f-muscimol in a few more animals?

Regarding the power of the results:

To address this concern of the Reviewing Editor (also raised by both reviewer 2 and reviewer 3), we ran four more rats in the SNSC task during the revision period of the manuscript. One of the rats was not usable because the rat’s cannula in the left LEC was blocked by a broken dummy cannula, but three rats’ data were successfully obtained and their cannula positions were located in the LEC correctly (Rats# 455, 457, and 458 in our raw data summary table). Adding these rats’ data to the original data set made the statistical results more significant compared to our original manuscript. That is, a repeated-measures ANOVA showed highly significant effects of the drug condition (F(2,16)=10.1, p = 0.001; improved from p = 0.02 in the original manuscript) and there were significant differences in performance between aCSF and LEC-MUS conditions (p < 0.001; improved from p < 0.01 in the original manuscript) and also between LEC-MUS and MEC-MUS conditions (p < 0.01; improved from p = 0.05 in the original manuscript). We revised our manuscript accordingly (subsection “Double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in visual scene-based choice behavior”, first paragraph).

Although the additional data gave us more confidence regarding the roles of the LEC in the SNSC task, we observed the same pattern of results that we saw during the original experiment in the rats additionally run during the revision period. Specifically, two of the rats (rat# 455, 457) in these rats showed severe performance deficits, but one rat (rat# 458) showed only a mild deficit in the SNSC task although all three rats had their LEC cannulae implanted in the LEC. We observed this pattern repeatedly, where some rats were severely impaired while other rats were only mildly affected by MUS in the LEC in both the original experimental period and the additional data-collecting period. The large individual difference underlies the greatest variability in performance data in the LEC-MUS condition in the current study (revised Figure 3B). Therefore, we checked whether cannula positions in different rats might have affected the amount of severity in performance deficit. Figure 1C of our revised manuscript shows the LEC cannula tip positions in all the rats (n=9). Comparing the cannula positions together with the summary table for the raw data suggests that the rats (rat# 362,404,417,458) showing seemingly mild or no deficits in the LEC-MUS condition compared to the aCSF condition had their LEC cannulae implanted mostly in the lateral edges of the LEC along the superficial layers. However, the LEC cannula-tips of the rats (rat# 341,361,403,455,457) that showed relatively severe deficits with LEC-MUS compared to the aCSF condition were located more medially targeting deeper layers in the LEC (revised Figure 1C). Confirming these observations, when we compared the performance levels of the rats with medially vs. laterally placed LEC cannulae, there was no significant difference in performance between the two groups in aCSF conditions (t(7) = 0.3, p = 0.77) (revised Figure 3C). However, MUS injections into the LEC induced a significant difference in performance between the medial and lateral cannula groups (t(7) = -4.35, p < 0.01) (revised Figure 3C). We added these newly discovered important findings in our revised manuscript (subsection “Double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in visual scene-based choice behavior”, third paragraph).

The results may suggest that the LEC deficits in the SNSC task could be layer-specific or that MUS injected through superficially implanted LEC cannulae might have been leaked to the brain surface. These possibilities are discussed in our revised manuscript (subsection “Visual scene-based disambiguation of object-associated responses in the LEC”, second paragraph).

Regarding the drug diffusion issues:

The Reviewing Editor expressed his/her concern that the performance deficits in LEC-MUS might be spurious (because only half of rats showed deficits in the original manuscript) and suggested that this could be due to the uncontrolled spread of MUS into an unknown area closer to the LEC in those rats showing performance deficits. First of all, in our response above, we verified the originally reported results by showing that adding more rats (n=3) to the original data set made our statistical results more compelling compared to the original manuscript (with p-values improved from 0.02 to 0.001 in repeated-measures ANOVA). Second, we also showed in our revised manuscript that the rats showing seemingly intact performance in the LEC-MUS condition had their cannulae mostly in the lateral edges of the LEC along more superficial layers. In contrast, the rats showing more severe performance deficits in the same drug conditions had their cannula tips placed more medially along deeper layers of the LEC. Based on our histological results, we think it is unlikely that our LEC-MUS impaired performance through other unknown areas because LEC cannula tips were all within the LEC region. Third, regarding the comments of R.E. that the spread of fluorescent MUS (f-MUS) might be underestimating the real diffusion range of MUS, we completely agree with the comment, and this has been shown by the Brown group (Allen et al., 2008). However, for the suggestion of running additional rats with f-MUS injected in the LEC, our prior experience with prolonged injections of f-MUS in rats in this type of behavioral paradigm (i.e., injecting MUS and testing rats for multiple days) taught us that such protocol sometimes caused health problems in rats for unknown reasons. In that case, it is difficult to attribute resulting performance deficits specifically to inactivation of the local area. Furthermore, injecting f-MUS for behavioral testing requires usually 40 min of diffusion time (which is twice longer than the MUS injection protocol), compared to 20 min with regular MUS in the current study, and the lipophilic nature of f-MUS makes drug spread uneven (Allen et al., 2008). Our experience is also that f-MUS appears to show bigger upward spread along the cannula track compared to regular MUS and thus may require larger injection quantity compared to regular MUS. With these known caveats of using f-MUS in behavioral testing, we decided not to run our rats with f-MUS injections. Since the fluorescent photomicrographs might underrepresent the real diffusion range of MUS (and this is visible in our histological data) and the cannula tracks are clearly visible in the photomicrographs of the Nissl-stained sections, we removed the fluorescent photomicrographs in our revised manuscript.

In sum, based on our (i) additional behavioral data showing highly significant, replicable effects of LEC-MUS, (ii) additionally reported relationships between LEC cannula tip positions and performance levels, (iii) highly significant double dissociation between LEC and MEC within the same rats (revised Figure 3B) (even though the two areas are adjacent to each other), we are confident that the performance deficits in the SNSC task with LEC-MUS were genuine.

Referee 3 requests a diagram showing implantation points for all animals, for comparison with the supplemental data on performance regarding this point.

Please see our detailed responses to the comments of the Reviewing Editor above and updated contents of our revised manuscript.

The other concerns the interpretation of the findings – what is the critical task difference: is digging (down) or pushing (ahead) really so different than turning left or right? Is this difference due to being more/less spatial? Are there any videos or descriptions of the way in which the LEC animals fail the 'non-spatial' scene task, so that we could try to understand why they fail?

We included video clips showing the normal trials under aCSF and error trials when MUS injected in LEC (Figure 3A; Video 2). As can be seen in the video clip, one big difference that is obvious when one compares the response types required between the SSC task (left vs. right turns required for choice; Figure 2A; Video 1) and SNSC task (digging vs. pushing required to an object, the sand-filled jar, for choice) is that the animal’s response is registered through the object in the SNSC task, but not in the SSC task. That is, in the SSC task, the rat’s response is purely spatial, but not in the SNSC task. Once trained, the rat does not stop at the choice point of the T-maze before making a choice in the SSC task, but it does pause briefly in front of the sand-filled jar before making a response in the SNSC task. When one observes the error trials in the SNSC task in the video (starting from 43s in the video), compared to the normal trials, the rat injected with MUS in the LEC appeared to make hesitating or up/down responses in front of the jar. In the zebra-stripe trial (starting from 53s in the video), the rat even correctly chose to dig in the sand initially but then came down and push the jar to make an error (which suggests that the rats were not using olfactory cues from the reward buried in the sand in our task). This behavior is observed when the rats were injected with MUS in the LEC and may suggest that LEC inactivation may play critical roles when an animal chooses a response to an object in a contextual manner. Our study suggests that the digging-pushing behavior itself is not LEC-dependent because the LEC was not required when the same behavioral responses were cued by an object, but not visual context, in the object-based choice task (Figure 4).

Main comments (Referee 2):

This is a fascinating result from a careful piece of work and is of very high interest to a wide range of researchers. However, the implications rely heavily on the detail of the methods, which can be sparse at times. It is important that the authors clarify several things to allow the importance of the findings to be fully understood:

1) Numbers of animals in each task are small – is there sufficient power in these results? Some form of power analysis is important. If one looks at the data for (for example) the SNSC task in Figure 1, the error bars suggest that there is reasonable overlap in the performance of the groups and whilst the means are statistically different from one another, without understanding the power of the study it is possible with such low numbers that this would be a 'fluke'. Whilst the double dissociation makes this somewhat unlikely it does need to be addressed.

Most of the comments of reviewer 2 have been addressed while responding to the comments of the Reviewing Editor as described above. Reviewer 2’s comment on the seemingly overlapping error bars of performance data in our original Figure 2D (Reviewer 2 mentioned Figure 1 but we believe he/she meant Figure 2) turned out to be an important one, and we redrew all of our performance data using box plots instead of bar graphs (revised Figure 3B). The box plot of the LEC-MUS condition made the large variance in the data more visible, and this turned out to be attributable to the fact that performance levels of some rats were severely affected by LEC-MUS than those of other rats. This discrepancy was related to their LEC cannula positions based on our additional analysis (Figure 1C and Figure 3C). See details in our response to the Reviewing Editor above. Power analysis might not tell us much information regarding this issue mainly because our study used a within-subject testing paradigm. Specifically, based on our power analysis with the following parameters (effect size f=5.505; α error probability=0.05; power=0.95; the number of group=1; the number of measurements=3; correlation among representative measures=0.360; non-sphericity correction=1), one could obtain the actual power of 0.99 with only the sample size of 2. Our extensive experience with behaviorally testing rats with within-subject testing designs has taught us that n of 6 to 8 is usually sufficient to draw firm conclusion when using behavioral tasks in which rats were pre-trained to criterion across many days before being tested with drug injections (Lee & Solivan, 2008; Jo & Lee, 2010; Kim & Lee, 2011; Yoon et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2012; Lee & Shin, 2012; Lee et al., 2014). This is in contrast to those behavioral testing paradigms that require no or minimal pre-training such as fear conditioning because individual variability is often bigger in those one-shot paradigms compared to our paradigm.

2) To what extent are the object and scene tasks comparable? Much of the interpretation relies on the object based version of the tasks being unaffected by temporary inactivation of either area – suggesting the effect is limited to scenes. However, power remains to be addressed (point 1) but also performance is much higher in the object task. One assumes it is easier given the stimuli are likely more discriminable. Therefore, is it that a dissociation exists between LEC and MEC in this task, but is merely hidden by ceiling effects in performance?

Power issues have been addressed above (see our responses to reviewers in (1)). Regarding reviewer 2’s comment on the possibility that the object memory task might be easier than the scene-based tasks, we respectfully disagree with the reviewer’s comment. First of all, reviewer 2 mentioned that performance was much higher in the object task, but we do not understand what this comment was based on. Specifically, when comparing aCSF performance across the tasks, we see no significant differences among the tasks; the mean performance was 92.8% for SSC, 91.7% for SNSC, and 92.9% for the object task (F(2,23)=0.13, p = 0.88; ANOVA). So, normal performance was not much higher in the object task compared to the SSC and SNSC tasks. More importantly, in our recent study using the same object task for testing the differential roles of the perirhinal cortex and postrhinal cortex, there was dissociation in performance under MUS infusions in the object memory task (Park et al., 2016, SFN); MUS significantly impaired performance when injected into the perirhinal cortex, but not in the postrhinal cortex. These results may argue against the comment of the reviewer (i.e., ceiling effect-based null effect), and we have added these contents in our revised manuscript (subsection 2 No involvement of the LEC and MEC when an object alone cued choice behavior”, last paragraph).

3) There is significant damage dorsal to the infusion sites from the cannula (one assumes). Although unlikely to have a significant bearing (as the animals can do the task when muscimol is not infused) the authors do need to consider any relevance of this extraneous damage – especially as it may be damaging attention and/or visual systems which will impact on normal task performance.

We agree with the reviewer that there is a concern for this in all cannula-implantation experiments. Since the cannulae should penetrate the overlying cortices during the implantation surgery, it is inevitable to cause some damage over the cortical areas as R2 mentioned. Specifically, based on our surgical coordinates, the LEC cannulae inevitably damaged some of the auditory cortices, the TE, and the perirhinal cortex. Visual cortices were mostly spared. For MEC cannulae, some visual cortices were damaged. Importantly, despite the cortical damage, rats performed the tasks very well when aCSF was injected, and this normal performance in the control condition strongly argues against the possibility that MUS effects were due to the overlying cortical damage. Attention and visual capabilities were obviously normal based on the performance data from the control conditions. We also see no relationships between the amount of cortical damage and the severity of the MUS effects in our study. We think mentioning this is important in our manuscript and added the related discussion (subsection 2 Visual scene-based disambiguation of object-associated responses in the LEC”, last paragraph).

Main comments (Referee 3):

[…] This is an interesting piece of work that will usefully complement the growing body of literature tackling the functional roles of the MEC and LEC. My main concern is that it does not however provide any insight into what these functions might be. While the results of the first task (scene-based spatial choice task) and clear and fit existing data, those of the second task (scene-based non-spatial choice task) are more difficult to interpret.

First, it is not clear why the authors designed this task (besides the idea of having a non-spatial task), what their prediction was, and what the specific function that seems to be handled by the LEC might be. The discussion does not provide any hint as to this last point.

We agree with the comments that our initial manuscript did not explain the rationale and implications of the study well. We have extensively revised our manuscript, and the current version explains the above points better. Briefly, our motivation behind the study was initially to test whether the LEC and MEC would be differentially involved in a visual scene memory task. Since our lab has shown that the dorsal hippocampus is critical for using a visual scene to make a spatial or nonspatial choice conditionally, we wanted to test whether its upstream input regions also played critical roles. We’d like to emphasize that visual scene has never been systematically used in rodent literature for testing the entorhinal functions and our study is the first one that systematically used the visual scene as the major variable for testing the differential functions of the LEC and MEC. Prior studies assumed that visual scene could be used during spatial navigation, but this has never been directly tested.

We observed the MEC-dependent performance deficits, but no significant deficit was found with the LEC-MUS in the SSC task. Since this could be (a) because the MEC, but not the LEC, is critical for scene information processing or (b) because the MEC, but not the LEC, is critical for making spatial responses, or both. We originally tested the rats in a tactile-version of the T-maze task and confirmed that the rats with the MEC inactivated with MUS were normal compared to the aCSF condition in making spatial choices in the T-maze by using a tactile cue (wire mesh or sandpaper in the stem of the maze). Considering the reviewer’s comment, these results are important to show that the MEC’s involvement is specific to the condition where visual scenes should be associated with spatial responses and we added the results from the tactile memory task in our revised manuscript (Figure 2C and subsection “Double dissociation between the LEC and MEC in visual scene-based choice behavior”, first paragraph). Then, our question became whether the MEC was also important when visual scene should be associated with nonspatial choice behavior. So we tested rats in a different scene-based task (i.e., SNSC task) that had relatively little spatial response component. We used the SNSC task specifically because we knew through our previous study that inactivating the dorsal hippocampus prevented rats from exhibiting normal performance in that task. As a result, we saw that the LEC-MUS impaired performance in the SNSC task, whereas no impairment was seen with the MEC-MUS in the same rats.

We revised our manuscript extensively to include this rationale and theoretical implications of our findings: see changes in our Abstract, Introduction, “Significance and implications of the current findings” in the Discussion of our revised manuscript. The following excerpt from our Discussion may help the reviewer to understand our viewpoint better:

“Our findings also strongly suggest that behavioral response components should be incorporated when theorizing the functions of different regions in the medial temporal lobe. […] Our study suggests that, instead of representing “what” information, the LEC may process “what should I do to this object in this context?”, whereas the MEC may represent “where should I go from here in this context?””

Second, the data are a bit weak as the deficit induced by LEC inactivations is relatively small (as noticed by the authors) and does not appear to be very robust. Looking at the individual data of the six rats that were tested revealed virtually no deficit in 3 of the rats.

In addition to these main concerns, I would like to see a diagram showing the implantation points for all rats. Although Figure 1B is useful, it does not discard the possibility that some of the reported effects are due to diffusion of muscimol into neighboring structures (including MEC). The visualization of diffusion extent is useful but not sufficient as the molecular weight of fluorescent muscimol (ca 600) is much greater than that of regular muscimol (ca 100), leading to the possibility of underestimation of diffusion range.

Please see our detailed responses to the comments of the Reviewing Editor above and updated contents of our revised manuscript.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. (SSC).

    Source data for performance in the SSC task (Figure 2B).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.004

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.004
    Figure 2—source data 2. (Tactile).

    Source data for performance in the tactile-based spatial choice task (Figure 2C).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.005

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.005
    Figure 3—source data 1. (SNSC).

    Source data for performance in the SNSC task (Figure 3B and C).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.008

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.008
    Figure 5—source data 1. (Object).

    Source data for performance in the object memory task (Figure 5B).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21543.012

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.21543.012

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES