Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2017 Jan 25;284(1847):20160681. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.0681

Problems with equating thermal preference with ‘emotional fever’ and sentience: comment on ‘Fish can show emotional fever: stress-induced hyperthermia in zebrafish’ by Rey et al. (2015)

B Key 1,, R Arlinghaus 2, H I Browman 3, S J Cooke 4, I G Cowx 5, B K Diggles 6, J D Rose 7, W Sawynok 8, A Schwab 9, A B Skiftesvik 3, E D Stevens 10, C A Watson 11
PMCID: PMC5310028  PMID: 28100812

Rey et al. [1] report that zebrafish captured with a net and held for 15 min at a water temperature of 27°C exhibited a subsequent preference to swim in water temperatures of 28.75 ± 0.27°C and higher for the next 4 h, compared with control fish that were neither captured nor held in nets. They report that approximately 25% more net-confined fish resided in areas with water temperatures of 29°C or higher (compared with controls; their fig. 2). Based on these results, they conclude that: (i) net-confined fish exhibit hyperthermia; and (ii) this hyperthermia is caused by psychological stress (e.g. anxiety) which they refer to as ‘emotional fever’ [1, p. 1]. Rey et al. state that ‘ … lack of emotional fever in fishes … ’ would reflect ‘ … a lack of consciousness … ’ [1, p. 1] and claim that the occurrence of emotional fever in zebrafish ‘ … removes a key argument for lack of consciousness in fishes’ [1, p. 1]. Thus, Rey et al. [1] are clearly inferring that their results are consistent with consciousness in fishes. We contend that the methods, analysis and interpretation of their data are flawed and that their conclusions are, therefore, unfounded.

1. Conclusion (i)

Rey et al.'s [1] conclusion that fish increased their core body temperature is based on a purported shift of the population into warmer chambers following net confinement. However, individual fish could not be identified, and the numerical model used by Rey et al. [1] to simulate fish distribution appears to be based on data collected during brief periods that amount to only 1.67% of the total observation time (fig. 1, electronic supplementary material, fig. S2). We used electronic supplementary material, fig. S2, to calculate predicted fish counts in each chamber at time 60 min (table 1). Our analysis suggests that there were only approximately 2 more fish in hyperthermic chambers 5 and 6 compared with controls at any particular moment during the first 4 h post-treatment. These predicted changes in distribution are modest, not statistically significant, and importantly, cannot distinguish between the possibilities that the same fish entered and remained in the hyperthermic chambers versus, for example, whether all experimental fish (or a subset thereof) moved into and out of the hyperthermic chambers. Thus, the analysis presented by Rey et al. [1] does not support the inferences made about a stable change in the core body temperature of the fish, let alone their conclusions about stress-induced hyperthermia.

Table 1.

Predicted fish counts (based on modelling performed by Rey et al. [1] in their electronic supplementary material, fig. S2) in each chamber at time 60 min for control and experimental conditions. The column ‘difference in fish count’ represents the change in predicted fish counts in each chamber between experimental and control conditions. Italicized rows are hyperthermic chambers. There is only a modest total increase in predicted fish count of 1.13 (out of a total of approximately 12 fish) in the experimental compared to control hyperthermic chambers. We converted control and experimental fractional values to integers and found no statistical difference between predicted counts in the hyperthermic chambers versus all other chambers (using Fisher's exact test with either one- or two-tailed p-values; p < 0.05). Hyperthermic chambers in the experimental condition would require a predicted fish count of eight (rather than the current four) to reach statistical significance (p < 0.05).

chamber temperature (°C) control experimental difference in fish count
1 18 0.87 0.08 −0.79
2 25 2.92 1.85 −1.07
3 27 3.06 2.71 −0.35
4 29 2.77 3.17 +0.40
5 32 1.89 2.65 +0.76
6 35 0.94 1.31 +0.37
sum 12.45 11.77 −0.68

2. Conclusion (ii)

Stress can be triggered by physical stimuli (e.g. injury, pyrogens) and/or psychological states (e.g. emotions). Nonetheless, Rey et al. [1] build their case on the unsupported premise that handling and net confinement cause anxiety in zebrafish, which then leads to hyperthermia. While it is not controversial that fish exhibit somatic and physiological responses to stimuli such as net handling [2,3], Rey et al. [1] provide no evidence that the purported altered thermal preference by net-confined zebrafish is driven by fish experiencing conscious anxious states. In fact, the idea that stress-induced hyperthermia can be interpreted as an ‘emotional fever’ is highly contentious [4,5], particularly in vertebrate poikilotherms [6,7]. Alternative hypotheses that we contend are more biologically plausible and parsimonious (see below) are not considered by Rey et al. [1]. For example, handling of fish causes them to release chemicals (pheromones) into the water that affect cortisol levels in unhandled fish [8]. These pheromones can be released by both very slightly damaged (alarm) and undamaged skin (disturbance substance) during handling and confinement and possibly include factors such as Schreckstoff, urinary ammonia or bile salts [9,10]. Importantly, zebrafish respond to pheromones by changing their swimming behaviour (e.g. more erratic with zig-zagging motions) and their vertical and horizontal position in tanks [11]. Hence, by holding and replacing the net-treated fish back into compartment 3, the release of pheromones into that compartment could explain the purported subsequent change in distribution of these fish. The reported small distribution shift suggests that fish moved towards their preferred normal rearing temperature in chamber 4 and occasionally explored chambers 5 and 6 while avoiding chamber 3 (a behaviour consistent with both conditioned place avoidance and social transfer) [1214]. Thus, it is just as plausible that Rey et al. [1] were measuring chamber ‘avoidance’ rather than chamber preference. In any case, the data provided cannot differentiate between the two alternative explanations.

Because of the incomplete description of methodology, the weak and possibly inappropriate statistical analyses (in particular, inappropriate pooling of dependent samples over time and their analysis by a Mann–Whitney-U test for independent samples in their fig. 2), and the high probability that the observations were confounded by experimental artefacts (pheromones), it is impossible to know whether there actually was a shift in the spatial distribution of confined fish or a stable change in the core body temperature of some of the fish, let alone what might have caused those modest purported changes. What is clear is that these results do not support the authors' conclusion that ‘fish can show emotional fever’ [1].

Authors' contributions

All authors contributed to drafting, editing and gave final approval.

Competing interests

We have no competing interests.

Funding

We received no funding for this study.

References

  • 1.Rey S, Huntingford FA, Boltaña S, Vargas R, Knowles TG, Mackenzie S. 2015. Fish can show emotional fever: stress-induced hyperthermia in zebrafish. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20152266 ( 10.1098/rspb.2015.2266) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Noga EJ, Botts S, Yang MS, Avtalion R. 1998. Acute stress causes skin ulceration in striped bass and hybrid bass (Morone). Vet. Pathol. 35, 102–107. ( 10.1177/030098589803500203) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ramsay JM, Feist GW, Varga ZM, Westerfield M, Kent ML, Schreck CB. 2009. Whole-body cortisol response of zebrafish to acute net handling stress. Aquaculture 297, 157–162. ( 10.1016/j.aquaculture.2009.08.035) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Parviz M, Anushiravani M, Keshavarz M. 2013. The relationship between vital spirit and fevers in the ‘Canon of Medicine’: a probable solution for the controversy over stress-induced hyperthermia. Iranian J. Pub. Health 42, 1073–1074. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Olivier B. 2015. Psychogenic fever, functional fever, or psychogenic hyperthermia? Temperature 2, 324–325. ( 10.1080/23328940.2015.1071701) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Allen C. 2013. Fish cognition and consciousness. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 26, 25–39. ( 10.1007/s10806-011-9364-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bovenkerk B, Meijboom FL. 2013. Fish welfare in aquaculture: explicating the chain of interactions between science and ethics. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 26, 41–61. ( 10.1007/s10806-012-9395-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barcellos LJ, Volpato GL, Barreto RE, Coldebella I, Ferreira D. 2011. Chemical communication of handling stress in fish. Physiol. Behav. 103, 372–375. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.03.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP. 2010. Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 88, 698–724. ( 10.1139/Z10-029) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Buchinger TJ, Li W, Johnson NS. 2014. Bile salts as semiochemicals in fish. Chem. Senses 39, 647–654. ( 10.1093/chemse/bju039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Speedie N, Gerlai R. 2008. Alarm substance induced behavioral responses in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 188, 168–177. ( 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.10.031) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hall D, Suboski MD. 1995. Visual and olfactory stimuli in learned release of alarm reactions by zebra Danio fish (Brachydanio rerio). Neuro. Learn. Mem. 63, 229–240. ( 10.1006/nlme.1995.1027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Yu L, Tucci V, Kishi S, Zhdanova IV. 2006. Cognitive aging in zebrafish. PLoS ONE 1, e14 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0000014) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Blank M, Guerim LD, Cordeiro RF, Vianna MR. 2009. A one-trial inhibitory avoidance task to zebrafish: rapid acquisition of an NMDA-dependent long-term memory. Neuro. Learn. Mem. 92, 529–534. ( 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.07.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES