ABSTRACT
The rapid modulation of chromatin organization is thought to play a crucial role in cognitive processes such as memory consolidation. This is supported in part by the dysregulation of many chromatin-remodelling proteins in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. A key example is ATRX, an X-linked gene commonly mutated in individuals with syndromic and nonsyndromic intellectual disability. The consequences of Atrx inactivation for learning and memory have been difficult to evaluate because of the early lethality of hemizygous-null animals. In this study, we evaluated the outcome of brain-specific Atrx deletion in heterozygous female mice. These mice exhibit a mosaic pattern of ATRX protein expression in the central nervous system attributable to the location of the gene on the X chromosome. Although the hemizygous male mice die soon after birth, heterozygous females survive to adulthood. Body growth is stunted in these animals, and they have low circulating concentrations of insulin growth factor 1. In addition, they are impaired in spatial, contextual fear and novel object recognition memory. Our findings demonstrate that mosaic loss of ATRX expression in the central nervous system leads to endocrine defects and decreased body size and has a negative impact on learning and memory.
KEY WORDS: ATRX, Central nervous system, Mouse models, Neurobehaviour
Summary: Heterozygous expression of the X-linked gene Atrx in the mouse brain causes deficits in spatial, contextual fear and object recognition memory.
INTRODUCTION
Alpha thalassemia mental retardation, X-linked, or ATR-X syndrome, is an intellectual disability (ID) disorder that arises from mutations in the ATRX gene (OMIM 301040). This rare syndrome is characterized by severe developmental delay, hypotonia, mild α-thalassemia and moderate-to-severe ID (Gibbons et al., 1995). A recent study screened a cohort of nearly 1000 individuals with ID using targeted next-generation sequencing and identified ATRX variants as one of the most common causes of ID, reinforcing its importance in cognition (Grozeva et al., 2015). The ATRX protein is a SWI/SNF-type chromatin remodeller. The N-terminal region of the protein contains a histone reader domain that mediates interaction of the protein with histone H3 trimethylated at lysine 9 (H3K9me3) and unmethylated at lysine 4 (H3K4me0) (Dhayalan et al., 2011). A SWI/SNF2-type helicase domain is located in the C-terminal half of the protein and confers ATP-dependent chromatin remodelling activity (Aapola et al., 2000; Gibbons et al., 1997; Picketts et al., 1996). Several proteins have been shown to interact with ATRX, including MeCP2, HP1α, EZH2 and DAXX (Bérubé et al., 2000; Cardoso et al., 1998; Nan et al., 2007; Xue et al., 2003). DAXX is a histone chaperone for histone variant H3.3. In association with ATRX, DAXX deposits H3.3-containing nucleosomes at telomeres and pericentromeric heterochromatin (Drane et al., 2010; Lewis et al., 2010).
Several studies have previously implicated ATRX in the regulation of gene expression through a variety of mechanisms. Chromatin immunoprecipitation (ChIP) sequencing for ATRX in human erythroblasts showed that the protein tends to bind GC-rich regions with high tendency to form G-quadruplexes. For example, ATRX was found to bind tandem repeats within the human α-globin gene cluster, and it was suggested that reduced expression of α-globin might be caused by replication-dependent mechanisms that would affect the expression of nearby genes (Law et al., 2010). The induction of replication stress was in fact detected in vivo upon inactivation of Atrx in either muscle or brain (Leung et al., 2013; Watson et al., 2013). More recently, our group demonstrated that loss of ATRX corresponds to decreased H3.3 incorporation and increased PolII occupancy in GC-rich gene bodies, including Neuroligin 4, an autism susceptibility gene (Levy et al., 2015).
Although the mechanisms by which ATRX modulates chromatin and genes is starting to be resolved, its function in neurons and cognitive processes is still obscure. To address this question, we generated mice with conditional inactivation of Atrx in the central nervous system (CNS) starting at early stages of neurogenesis. Although hemizygous male progeny died shortly after birth, heterozygous female mice (henceforth called Atrx-cHet), which exhibit mosaic expression of ATRX caused by random X-inactivation, survived to adulthood, allowing the investigation of neurobehavioural outcomes upon inactivation of Atrx in the brain.
RESULTS
Survival to adulthood depends on the extent of Atrx deletion in the CNS
Conditional inactivation of Atrx is required to elucidate its functions in specific tissues, because general inactivation of the gene is embryonic lethal (Garrick et al., 2006). We thus generated mice with Cre recombinase-mediated deletion of Atrx-floxed alleles in the CNS using the Nestin-Cre driver line of mice. Hemizygous male mice (Atrx-cKO) died by postnatal day (P)1 (Fig. 1A). Owing to random X-inactivation in females, Atrx is expressed from only one of the alleles in any specific cell, resulting in a mosaic pattern of expression in the brain of Atrx-cHet mice (e.g. if the floxed allele is the active allele, these cells are functionally null for Atrx; however, if the floxed allele is the silent one, cells are functionally wild type for Atrx). This was validated by RT-qPCR with Atrx primers in exon 17 and the excised exon 18, showing ∼50% decreased Atrx expression in the cortex and hippocampus of Atrx-cHet mice compared with littermate controls (Fig. 1B). Moreover, a mosaic pattern of ATRX protein expression was observed by immunofluorescence staining of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1C,D). This was quantified in the medial prefrontal cortex in three pairs of control and cKO animals (Fig. 1E). Haematoxylin and Eosin staining of control and Atrx-cHet brain sections did not reveal major histological alterations in the CA1, CA3 and mPFC regions (Fig. 1F). These results demonstrated that inactivation of Atrx throughout the CNS was perinatal lethal but that Atrx deletion in approximately half of cells allowed survival of the female heterozygous mice to adulthood.
Mosaic inactivation of Atrx in the CNS impedes normal body growth
Atrx-cHet mice were weighed weekly for the first 24 postnatal weeks. The data showed that the Atrx-cHet mice weighed significantly less than control mice over this time period (F=17.87, P=0.0003; Fig. 2A,B). Alcian Blue and Alizarin Red skeletal staining of P17 mice reveal that the Atrx-cHet skeletons were smaller than those of the control mice (Fig. 2C). Tibia, femur and humerus bones were also measured and found to be significantly shorter in the Atrx-cHet mice compared with littermate controls (Fig. 2D).
We previously reported that deletion of Atrx in the developing mouse forebrain and anterior pituitary leads to low circulating concentrations of IGF-1 and thyroxine (T4) (Watson et al., 2013). Some evidence suggests that T4 regulates the prepubertal concentrations of insulin growth factor 1 (IGF-1), whereas after puberty this regulation is largely mediated by growth hormone (GH) (Xing et al., 2012). Given that the Atrx-cHet mice were smaller than control mice, we examined the concentrations of T4, IGF-1 and GH in the blood by enzyme-linked immunosorbent assasy (ELISAs). We observed no significant difference in T4 and GH concentrations between P17 Atrx-cHet mice and control littermates. However, there was a large (80%) and significant decrease in IGF-1 concentrations (Fig. 2E). Thus, the reduced body size of the Atrx-cHet mice was correlated with low circulating IGF-1 concentrations.
Hindlimb-clasping phenotype in Atrx-cHet mice
The Atrx-cHet mice displayed increased hindlimb clasping compared with control mice, with >90% exhibiting limb clasping by 3 months of age (F=20.78, P<0.0001; Fig. 3A). In the open-field test, the distance travelled was not significantly different between control and Atrx-cHet mice, indicating that activity and locomotion were normal (F=0.20, P=0.66; Fig. 3B). Anxiety levels were also normal, based on the time spent in the centre of the open-field apparatus (F=0.84, P=0.44; Fig. 3C). Likewise, their performance in the elevated plus maze revealed no significant difference in the amount of time that control and Atrx-cHet mice spent in the open versus closed arms (F=0.68, P=0.41; Fig. 3C,D). We concluded that the Atrx-cHet mice were not hyper- or hypo-active and did not exhibit excessive anxiety, but the increased level of hindlimb-clasping behaviour was suggestive of neurological defects.
Atrx-cHet mice have normal working memory but deficits in object recognition memory
Given that ATRX mutations are linked to ID, we next evaluated memory in Atrx-cHet mice using various established paradigms. We first tested short-term working memory in the Y-maze task (de Castro et al., 2009). No difference was detected between control and Atrx-cHet mice in the percentage alternation or in the number of entries into the arms, suggesting that working memory was normal in Atrx-cHet mice (t=0.05, P=0.96; Fig. 4A). We then tested the Atrx-cHet mice in the spontaneous novel object recognition task that mainly involves the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (Ennaceur and Delacour, 1988). In rodents, the natural tendency to seek out and explore novelty leads to a preference for the novel over the familiar object, indicating recognition memory of the familiar object (Bevins and Besheer, 2006). During the habituation period, both control and Atrx-cHet mice spent ∼50% of the allotted time with each individual object (Fig. 4B). In the course of the short-term memory test (1.5 h), control mice spent ∼70% of their time with the novel object, whereas Atrx-cHet mice still spent ∼50% of their time with each object, suggesting an inability to remember the familiar object (Fig. 4B). Similar results were obtained in the long-term memory test (24 h). The total amount of time spent interacting with the objects was unchanged between control and Atrx-cHet mice during all three tests, ruling out visual or tactile impairment.
Atrx-cHet mice display deficits in contextual fear and spatial memory
To evaluate contextual fear memory, mice were placed in a box with distinctive black and white patterns on the sides for 3 min and shocked after 2.5 min. Twenty-four hours later, they were placed back into the same box with the same contextual cues, and the time spent freezing was measured at 30 s intervals. The data showed that the Atrx-cHet mice spent less time freezing than control mice (F=28.57, P<0.0001), and the total percentage of immobility time was significantly lower for Atrx-cHet mice, indicating impaired fear memory in these mice (t=5.35, P<0.0001; Fig. 4C).
The Morris water maze was next used to evaluate hippocampus-dependent spatial memory (Morris, 1984). During the 4 days of training, the Atrx-cHet mice took significantly more time finding the target platform while swimming a longer distance compared with control mice (latency F=31.44, P<0.0001; distance F=12.29, P<0.01; Fig. 5A). The Atrx-cHet mice also swam more slowly than control mice (F=15.40, P<0.001; Fig. 5A). During testing on the fifth day, the platform was removed and the time spent in each quadrant recorded as a measure of spatial memory. Whereas control mice spent significantly more time in the target quadrant than the left or opposite quadrant (F=4.70, P<0.01), Atrx-cHet mice showed no preference for the target quadrant (F=0.75, P=0.53; Fig. 5B). The results suggested that spatial learning and memory might be impaired in the Atrx-cHet mice. The cued Morris water maze was used to determine whether motivational or sensorimotor defects contributed to the phenotype seen in the noncued version of the test. Whereas the control mice quickly learned to correlate the cue with the platform, the Atrx-cHet mice were unable to do so (F=14.09, P<0.01; Fig. 5C). We noticed that the mice failed to show normal signs of aversion to water during this task, with a preference to be swimming rather than to climb on the platform during training, even jumping back into the water after being placed on the platform.
Atrx-cHet mice have normal motor endurance and motor memory
Given that the Atrx-cHet mice swam more slowly than control animals in the Morris water-maze task, we considered that perhaps the test was confounded by deficits in motor skills. To clarify this issue, we examined endurance and motor skills further in the mutant mice. We found that motor function and balance measured in the Rotarod task were not significantly different in Atrx-cHet mice during any of the trials (F=3.02, P=0.09; Fig. 6A). Atrx-cHet mice also performed similarly to control animals in the treadmill task (t=0.34, P=0.73; Fig. 6B). By contrast, Atrx-cHet mice exhibited decreased forelimb grip strength, normalized to body weight (t=2.80, P<0.05; Fig. 6C).
DISCUSSION
This study demonstrates that deletion of Atrx in the CNS leads to endocrine defects and behavioural abnormalities. Specifically, we see impairments in spatial learning and memory in the Morris water maze, in contextual fear conditioning and in novel object recognition.
We previously reported that mice lacking ATRX expression in the embryonic mouse forebrain have an average lifespan of 22 days (Watson et al., 2013). It is thus not surprising that inactivation of Atrx using the Nestin-Cre driver (which mediates deletion in the majority of CNS cells) is neonatal lethal. By contrast, the Atrx-cHet female mice survived to adulthood, probably because roughly half of all Nestin-expressing cells and their progeny are spared. Mosaic loss of ATRX in Atrx-cHet female mice still negatively affects development, as the mice are smaller compared with littermate controls, and the length of long bones is decreased. As the Nestin-Cre driver does not promote Cre expression in bone progenitors (Wiese et al., 2004), this phenotype might result from the low concentration of circulating IGF-1 in these mice. The reason for low IGF-1 is difficult to pinpoint in our mice. It has been shown that mice expressing Cre under the control of the Nestin promoter are smaller as a result of a decrease in mouse GH (Declercq et al., 2015). However, in our hands, GH concentrations are normal in the Atrx-cHet mice. Given the normal concentrations of both T4 and GH, there could be unanticipated expression of Cre in the liver that affects IGF-1 production. Examining the potential off-target expression of Cre will be required to elucidate the mechanism of IGF-1 downregulation in these mice.
The Atrx-cHet mice displayed a variety of behavioural abnormalities. We initially noticed that the mice displayed excessive hindlimb clasping, which could indicate neurological impairment (Guy et al., 2001). This prompted us to perform additional tests to assess neurobehaviour of the mice. We observed no change in general activity or anxiety using the open-field test and elevated plus maze, respectively, and no change in working memory in the Y-maze task. The Atrx-cHet mice exhibited increased latency to reach the platform in the Morris water-maze task, which might indicate a defect in spatial memory. However, the findings are difficult to interpret because Atrx-cHet mice swam at a lower speed, which could indicate a problem with their ability to swim rather than with memory. It was previously reported that mice lacking MeCP2 protein, an established interactor with ATRX in the brain, exhibit navigational difficulties in the Morris water maze (Stearns et al., 2007). Significant differences in swimming ability made it difficult to conclude whether the increased latency to the platform was attributable to motor or cognitive deficits, similar to our findings with the Atrx-cHet mice. Although we did not observe defective motor skills in the Rotarod or treadmill tests or decreased activity in the open-field test, we noticed that the mice failed to show normal signs of aversion to water during this task and preferred to be swimming rather than to climb on the platform during training, even jumping back into the water after being placed on the platform. We attempted to test the mice in the Barnes maze, another spatial learning and memory test, but the heterozygote mice tended to jump off the edge of the maze. Based on these observations, it will be important in the future to perform additional tests that gauge the level of motivation in these mice.
Despite these issues, which might require further experimentation for a full understanding, we obtained supporting evidence that memory is affected in the Atrx-cHet mice in the contextual fear and the novel object recognition tasks. Additional support comes from a previous study done in a mouse model of Chudley–Lowry syndrome associated with reduced expression of ATRX (Nogami et al., 2011). The authors of that study reported an impairment in contextual fear memory and suggested that ATRX might play a role in regulation of adult-born neurons. Our results show defects not only in contextual fear memory, but also in novel object recognition and, potentially, the Morris water-maze task. This might indicate a role for ATRX not only in adult-born neurons, but perhaps also in the amygdala, hippocampus and the rest of the medial temporal lobe, structures which are vital for the tasks impaired in the Atrx-cHet mice (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Logue et al., 1997; Wan et al., 1999).
The DAXX protein is a well-established interactor with ATRX. Although the behaviour of DAXX knockout mice has not yet been investigated, a study previously demonstrated that DAXX binds with ATRX to the promoters of several immediate-early genes upon activation of cortical neuronal cultures (Michod et al., 2012). DAXX was also shown to be crucial for the incorporation of histone H3.3 at these gene promoters, supporting a potential role for DAXX and ATRX the initial steps of memory consolidation. EZH2, a member of the PRC2 complex that mediates H3K27 trimethylation, is another putative binding partner of ATRX (Cardoso et al., 1998; Margueron et al., 2009). Inducible deletion of the Ezh2 gene in neural progenitor cells in the adult brain caused impaired spatial learning and memory and contextual fear memory, suggesting that EZH2 (potentially with ATRX) provides important cues in adult neural progenitor cells (Zhang et al., 2014).
We emphasize that these mice do not model the ATR-X syndrome, where only males are affected and females exhibit 100% skewing of X-chromosome inactivation and are therefore largely unaffected. Rather, the Atrx-cHet mice are a useful tool to probe ATRX function in the CNS. Overall, our study presents compelling evidence that ATRX is required in the mouse CNS for normal cognitive processes and sets the stage for additional investigations delving into the mechanisms by which it regulates chromatin structure and gene expression in neurons in the context of learning and memory.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animal care and husbandry
Mice were exposed to a 12 h:12 h light–dark cycle and with water and chow ad libitum. The AtrxloxP mice have been described previously (Bérubé et al., 2005). AtrxloxP mice (129svj) were mated with mice expressing Cre recombinase under the control of the Nestin gene promoter (Bl6) (Tronche et al., 1999). The progeny include hemizygous male mice that produce no full-length ATRX protein in the CNS (Atrxf/y Cre+) and heterozygous female mice with approximately half the cells lacking ATRX protein as a result of the random pattern of X-inactivation (Atrxf/+ Cre+). Male and female littermate floxed mice lacking the Cre allele were used as controls. Genotyping of tail biopsies for the presence of the floxed and Cre alleles was performed as described previously (Bérubé et al., 2005; Seah et al., 2008). All procedures involving animals were conducted in accordance with the regulations of the Animals for Research Act of the province of Ontario and approved by the University of Western Ontario Animal Care and Use Committee (2008-041). Behavioural assessments started with less-demanding tasks (grip force, open-field tests), followed by more-demanding ones (Morris water maze). Experimenters followed ARRIVE guidelines: mouse groups were randomized, they were blind to the genotypes, and software-based analysis was used to score mouse performance in most of the tasks. All experiments were performed between 09.00 and 16.00 h.
Immunofluorescence staining
Mice were perfused and the brains fixed for 72 h with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS and cryopreserved in 30% sucrose/PBS. Brains were flash frozen in Cryomatrix (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and sectioned as described previously (Ritchie et al., 2014). For immunostaining, antigen retrieval was performed by incubating slides in 10 mM sodium citrate at 95°C for 10 min. Cooled slides were washed and incubated overnight in anti-ATRX rabbit polyclonal antibody (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, SC-15408; 1:200, H-300) (Watson et al., 2013) diluted in 0.3% Triton-X100 in PBS. Sections were washed and incubated with goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 594 (Life Technologies) for 1 h. Sections were counterstained with DAPI and mounted with SlowFade Gold (Invitrogen). Cell counts were done in three control–KO littermate-matched pairs in a blinded manner.
Microscopy
All images were captured using an inverted microscope (DMI 6000b; Leica) with a digital camera (ORCA-ER; Hamamatsu). Openlab image software was used for manual image capture, and images were processed using the Volocity software (PerkinElmer).
Haematoxylin and Eosin staining
Brain cryosections (8 μm thick) from 3-month-old mice were rehydrated in 70% ethanol for 2 min followed by tap water for 5 min. They were then placed in CAT Haematoxylin (Biocare) for 2 min, placed under running tap water for 1 min, and set in filtered Tasha's Bluing Solution (Biocare) for 30 s. The slides were placed under running tap water for 10 min and set in filtered Eosin Y (Fisher Scientific) for 2 min. Immediately afterwards, the cells were dehydrated in 70% ethanol for 30 s each, then 90% ethanol for 1 min and 100% ethanol for 2 min each. The slides were placed in xylene 3× for 5 min and mounted with Permount (Fisher Scientific) immediately after.
RT-qPCR
Total RNA was isolated from control and Atrx-cHet rostral cortex and hippocampus using the RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen) and reverse transcribed to cDNA using 1 μg RNA and SuperScript II Reverse Transcriptase (Invitrogen). cDNA was amplified in duplicate using primers in the following conditions: 95°C for 10 s, 55°C for 20 s and 72°C for 30 s for 35 cycles. Primers detected Atrx exons 17 and 18. Standard curves were generated for each primer pair. Primer efficiency (E) was calculated as E=(10−1/slope−1)×100%, where a desirable slope is −3.32 and R2>0.990. All data were corrected against β-actin.
ELISAs
Blood was collected from the inferior vena cava of P17 mice. One hundred microlitres of 0.5 M EDTA pH 7.0 per millilitre of blood collected was added to the blood sample and centrifuged at 21,000 g for 10 min at 4°C. Plasma supernatant was collected and kept frozen at −80°C. Plasma IGF-1 concentration was measured using a mouse IGF-1 ELISA kit (R&D Systems, MG100). Plasma GH (Millipore, EZRMGH-45K) and T4 (Calbiotech, T4044T) were also measured by ELISA according to the manufacturers' instructions.
Bone staining and measurements
Skinned and eviscerated P17 mouse carcasses were fixed overnight in 95% ethanol and transferred to acetone overnight (Ulici et al., 2009). Fixed skeletons were stained in a 0.05% Alizarin Red, 0.015% Alcian Blue, 5% acetic acid in 70% ethanol solution for 7-14 days. Stained skeletons were cleaned in decreasing concentrations of potassium hydroxide (2, 1 and 0.5%) for several days and stored in 50:50 70% ethanol/glycerol solution. Alcian Blue and Alizarin Red-stained skeletons were imaged using an Olympus SP-57OUZ digital camera. The lengths of the tibia, femur and humerus, the width of the skull and the length of the foot from at least four different littermate pairs from both mouse models were imaged using the Zeiss Stereo Zoom Microscope Stemi SV6 and measured with a ruler accurate to 0.1 mm.
Hindlimb clasping, grip force, Rotarod, treadmill and open-field tests
Hindlimb clasping was measured by lifting mice up by the base of the tail. Clasping was scored on a scale of 0 (no clasping, limbs splayed) to 2 (clasping, wringing paws).
Grip force, an indicator of muscular strength, was measured using a Grip Strength Meter (Columbus Instruments) (Solomon et al., 2013). The meter was positioned horizontally, and the mice were held by the base of the tail and lowered towards the triangular pull bar. Once the mice had gripped the bar, the meter was calibrated, and the mice were gently pulled away from the apparatus. The force applied to the bar as the mice released it was recorded as peak tension (in newtons). This test was repeated five times, with the highest and lowest value being removed for user error, and the remaining three values were averaged for the final grip strength.
For the Rotarod task, mice were placed on the Rotarod apparatus (San Diego Instruments) and rotation was increased from 5 to 35 rpm over 5 min. Latency to fall was recorded automatically. Ten trials were performed on the first day and four were performed on the second day. There was an inter-trial period of 10 min, during which the mice were placed in their home cage.
Training for the treadmill test occurred over 4 days (3 min day−1). On the first day, the incline was set to 5° and increased by 5° every day to a maximum of 20°. The initial speed was 8 m min−1, and the treadmill was accelerated by 1 m min−2. On the subsequent training days 2, 3 and 4, the initial speed was increased to 10, 11 and 12 m min−1, respectively, with constant acceleration. During testing on the fifth day, the initial speed was 12 m min−1 and accelerated to 20 m min−1 over the course of 15 min. Distance to exhaustion was measured, and the work performed (W, in joules) was calculated using the formula: W (J)=body weight (kg)×cos20°×9.8 (J kg−1×m)×distance (m).
In the open-field test, locomotor activity was automatically recorded (AccuScan Instrument). The mice were placed in an arena with an area of 20 cm×20 cm with 30-cm-high walls. Mice were acclimated to the locomotor room for 10 min before testing. Locomotor activity was measured in 5 min intervals over 2 h, as previously described (Martyn et al., 2012).
Elevated plus maze, Y-maze, fear conditioning and novel object recognition
Animals were placed in the centre of the elevated plus maze (Med Associate) and their activity was recorded over 5 min. The total time spent in the open and closed arms was recorded using computer software (AnyMaze). The centre of the mouse body was used as an indicator of which zone they were in.
Spontaneous Y-maze alternation was measured using a symmetrical three-armed Y-maze as described (de Castro et al., 2009). Video tracking was performed using computer software (AnyMaze) and the order and number of entries into each arm recorded. Each mouse underwent one trial lasting 5 min. Spontaneous alternation was counted when a mouse entered all three arms in a row without visiting a previous arm.
To measure contextual fear, mice were placed in a 20 cm×10 cm clear acrylic enclosure with a metal grid floor and one wall distinct from the others (stripes were drawn on one of the walls). The chamber was equipped with an electric shock generator. Videos were recorded using the AnyMaze video tracking software. On Day 1, mice were allowed to explore the enclosure freely, and at 150 s the mice were given a shock (2 mA, 180 V, 2 s). Shock sensitivity was confirmed by vocalization of the mice. Thirty seconds later the mice were returned to their home cage. After 24 h, the mice were placed back into the enclosure for 6 min and freezing time was measured in 30 s intervals. Freezing was defined as immobility lasting >0.5 s.
To test novel object recognition, mice were habituated with no objects in an open arena (40 cm×40 cm) for 5 min on both Day 1 and Day 2. On Day 3, mice were placed in the arena with two identical objects (A; a red plastic ball attached on top of a yellow cube base) and allowed to explore for 10 min. Video tracking was used (AnyMaze). To test short-term memory, 1.5 h after training the mice were placed back in the arena for 5 min with one previous object (A) and one novel object (B; a blue plastic pyramid attached on top of a green prism base). To test long-term memory, 24 h after training the mice were placed back in the arena for 5 min with one previous object (A) and one novel object (B). Recognition of previous and novel objects was expressed as the percentage of time spent with each object compared with the total time interacting. Interaction with the object was defined as sniffing or touching the object, but not leaning against or standing on the object.
Morris water maze
The Morris water-maze test was conducted as described previously (Vorhees and Williams, 2006). Mice were given four trials (90 s) a day for 4 days consecutively, with a 15 min inter-trial period. The latency to find the platform was recorded using video-tracking software (AnyMaze). If the mice did not find the platform during the 90 s, they were gently guided onto the platform. On the 5th and the 12th day, the platform was removed and time spent in each quadrant of the maze recorded using the video software. The task was performed in a pool 1.5 m in diameter with 25°C water, and the platform was submerged 1 cm beneath the water surface. Spatial cues (shapes printed in black and white) were distributed around the pool. For the cued version of the Morris water maze, mice were subjected to four trials (90 s) per day for 7 days consecutively, with a 30 s inter-trial period. If the mouse did not find the platform after 90 s, it was gently guided onto the platform. The visible platform and the mouse starting location changed with each trial, so they were unique between trials. The platform was made visible by placing a red plastic cube on top of the platform, which was wiped with ethanol between each trial.
Statistical analyses
All data were analysed using GraphPad Prism software or SPSS, with Student's t-test (unpaired, two tailed) or one- or two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni or Benjamini–Hochburg correction where indicated. All results are depicted as means±s.e.m. unless indicated otherwise. Values of P≤0.05 were considered to indicate significance.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Matthew Cowan at the Robarts Neurobehavioral facility for technical assistance. We thank Dr Michael Miller for his help with statistical analyses. We are also grateful to Doug Higgs and Richard Gibbons for the gift of the Atrxf/f mice.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.
Author contributions
R.J.T.: design, interpretation of data, execution of experiments, writing of article. J.R.S.: execution of experiments, interpretation of data. Y.J.: execution of experiments. M.A.M.P.: interpretation of data, editing of article. F.B.: design, interpretation of data, editing of article. N.G.B.: conception, design, interpretation of data, writing of article.
Funding
This work was supported by a studentship from the Department of Paediatrics at the University of Western Ontario to R.J.T., a Canadian Institutes for Health Research Masters Scholarship to J.R.L. and a Canadian Institutes for Health Research operating grant to N.G.B. (MOP142369).
References
- Aapola U., Kawasaki K., Scott H. S., Ollila J., Vihinen M., Heino M., Shintani A., Kawasaki K., Minoshima S., Krohn K. et al. (2000). Isolation and initial characterization of a novel zinc finger gene, DNMT3L, on 21q22.3, related to the cytosine-5-methyltransferase 3 gene family. Genomics 65, 293-298. 10.1006/geno.2000.6168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bérubé N. G., Smeenk C. A. and Picketts D. J. (2000). Cell cycle-dependent phosphorylation of the ATRX protein correlates with changes in nuclear matrix and chromatin association. Hum. Mol. Genet. 9, 539-547. 10.1093/hmg/9.4.539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bérubé N. G., Mangelsdorf M., Jagla M., Vanderluit J., Garrick D., Gibbons R. J., Higgs D. R., Slack R. S. and Picketts D. J. (2005). The chromatin-remodeling protein ATRX is critical for neuronal survival during corticogenesis. J. Clin. Invest. 115, 258-267. 10.1172/JCI200522329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevins R. A. and Besheer J. (2006). Object recognition in rats and mice: a one-trial non-matching-to-sample learning task to study ‘recognition memory’. Nat. Protoc. 1, 1306-1311. 10.1038/nprot.2006.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso C., Timsit S., Villard L., Khrestchatisky M., Fontès M. and Colleaux L. (1998). Specific interaction between the XNP/ATR-X gene product and the SET domain of the human EZH2 protein. Hum. Mol. Genet. 7, 679-684. 10.1093/hmg/7.4.679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Castro B. M., Pereira G. S., Magalhães V., Rossato J. I., De Jaeger X., Martins-Silva C., Leles B., Lima P., Gomez M. V., Gainetdinov R. R. et al. (2009). Reduced expression of the vesicular acetylcholine transporter causes learning deficits in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 8, 23-35. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00439.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Declercq J., Brouwers B., Pruniau V. P. E., Stijnen P., de Faudeur G., Tuand K., Meulemans S., Serneels L., Schraenen A., Schuit F. et al. (2015). Metabolic and behavioural phenotypes in Nestin-Cre mice are caused by hypothalamic expression of human growth hormone. PLoS ONE 10, e0135502 10.1371/journal.pone.0135502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhayalan A., Tamas R., Bock I., Tattermusch A., Dimitrova E., Kudithipudi S., Ragozin S. and Jeltsch A. (2011). The ATRX-ADD domain binds to H3 tail peptides and reads the combined methylation state of K4 and K9. Hum. Mol. Genet. 20, 2195-2203. 10.1093/hmg/ddr107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drane P., Ouararhni K., Depaux A., Shuaib M. and Hamiche A. (2010). The death-associated protein DAXX is a novel histone chaperone involved in the replication-independent deposition of H3.3. Genes Dev. 24, 1253-1265. 10.1101/gad.566910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennaceur A. and Delacour J. (1988). A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 31, 47-59. 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrick D., Sharpe J. A., Arkell R., Dobbie L., Smith A. J. H., Wood W. G., Higgs D. R. and Gibbons R. J. (2006). Loss of Atrx affects trophoblast development and the pattern of X-inactivation in extraembryonic tissues. PLoS Genet. 2, e58 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbons R. J., Picketts D. J., Villard L. and Higgs D. R. (1995). Mutations in a putative global transcriptional regulator cause X-linked mental retardation with alpha-thalassemia (ATR-X syndrome). Cell 80, 837-845. 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90287-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbons R. J., Bachoo S., Picketts D. J., Aftimos S., Asenbauer B., Bergoffen J. A., Berry S. A., Dahl N., Fryer A., Keppler K. et al. (1997). Mutations in transcriptional regulator ATRX establish the functional significance of a PHD-like domain. Nat. Genet. 17, 146-148. 10.1038/ng1097-146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grozeva D., Carss K., Spasic-Boskovic O., Tejada M. I., Gecz J., Shaw M., Corbett M., Haan E., Thompson E., Friend K. et al. (2015). Targeted next generation sequencing analysis of 1000 individuals with intellectual disability. Hum. Mutat. 36, 1197-1204. 10.1002/humu.22901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guy J., Hendrich B., Holmes M., Martin J. E. and Bird A. (2001). A mouse Mecp2-null mutation causes neurological symptoms that mimic Rett syndrome. Nat. Genet. 27, 322-326. 10.1038/85899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Law M. J., Lower K. M., Voon H. P. J., Hughes J. R., Garrick D., Viprakasit V., Mitson M., De Gobbi M., Marra M., Morris A. et al. (2010). ATR-X syndrome protein targets tandem repeats and influences allele-specific expression in a size-dependent manner. Cell 143, 367-378. 10.1016/j.cell.2010.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung J. W.-C., Ghosal G., Wang W., Shen X., Wang J., Li L. and Chen J. (2013). Alpha thalassemia/mental retardation syndrome X-linked gene product ATRX is required for proper replication restart and cellular resistance to replication stress. J. Biol. Chem. 288, 6342-6350. 10.1074/jbc.M112.411603 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy M. A., Kernohan K. D., Jiang Y. and Berube N. G. (2015). ATRX promotes gene expression by facilitating transcriptional elongation through guanine-rich coding regions. Hum. Mol. Genet. 24, 1824-1835. 10.1093/hmg/ddu596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis P. W., Elsaesser S. J., Noh K.-M., Stadler S. C. and Allis C. D. (2010). Daxx is an H3.3-specific histone chaperone and cooperates with ATRX in replication-independent chromatin assembly at telomeres. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 14075-14080. 10.1073/pnas.1008850107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logue S. F., Paylor R. and Wehner J. M. (1997). Hippocampal lesions cause learning deficits in inbred mice in the Morris water maze and conditioned-fear task. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 104-113. 10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margueron R., Justin N., Ohno K., Sharpe M. L., Son J., Drury W. J. III, Voigt P., Martin S. R., Taylor W. R., De Marco V. et al. (2009). Role of the polycomb protein EED in the propagation of repressive histone marks. Nature 461, 762-767. 10.1038/nature08398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martyn A. C., De Jaeger X., Magalhaes A. C., Kesarwani R., Goncalves D. F., Raulic S., Guzman M. S., Jackson M. F., Izquierdo I., MacDonald J. F. et al. (2012). Elimination of the vesicular acetylcholine transporter in the forebrain causes hyperactivity and deficits in spatial memory and long-term potentiation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 17651-17656. 10.1073/pnas.1215381109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michod D., Bartesaghi S., Khelifi A., Bellodi C., Berliocchi L., Nicotera P. and Salomoni P. (2012). Calcium-dependent dephosphorylation of the histone chaperone DAXX regulates H3.3 loading and transcription upon neuronal activation. Neuron 74, 122-135. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris R. (1984). Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 11, 47-60. 10.1016/0165-0270(84)90007-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nan X., Hou J., Maclean A., Nasir J., Lafuente M. J., Shu X., Kriaucionis S. and Bird A. (2007). Interaction between chromatin proteins MECP2 and ATRX is disrupted by mutations that cause inherited mental retardation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 2709-2714. 10.1073/pnas.0608056104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nogami T., Beppu H., Tokoro T., Moriguchi S., Shioda N., Fukunaga K., Ohtsuka T., Ishii Y., Sasahara M., Shimada Y. et al. (2011). Reduced expression of the ATRX gene, a chromatin-remodeling factor, causes hippocampal dysfunction in mice. Hippocampus 21, 678-687. 10.1002/hipo.20782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips R. G. and LeDoux J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 274-285. 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picketts D. J., Higgs D. R., Bachoo S., Blake D. J., Quarrell O. W. and Gibbons R. J. (1996). ATRX encodes a novel member of the SNF2 family of proteins: mutations point to a common mechanism underlying the ATR-X syndrome. Hum. Mol. Genet. 5, 1899-1907. 10.1093/hmg/5.12.1899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritchie K., Watson L. A., Davidson B., Jiang Y. and Berube N. G. (2014). ATRX is required for maintenance of the neuroprogenitor cell pool in the embryonic mouse brain. Biol. Open 3, 1158-1163. 10.1242/bio.20148730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seah C., Levy M. A., Jiang Y., Mokhtarzada S., Higgs D. R., Gibbons R. J. and Berube N. G. (2008). Neuronal death resulting from targeted disruption of the Snf2 protein ATRX is mediated by p53. J. Neurosci. 28, 12570-12580. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4048-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon L. A., Russell B. A., Watson L. A., Beier F. and Berube N. G. (2013). Targeted loss of the ATR-X syndrome protein in the limb mesenchyme of mice causes brachydactyly. Hum. Mol. Genet. 22, 5015-5025. 10.1093/hmg/ddt351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stearns N. A., Schaevitz L. R., Bowling H., Nag N., Berger U. V. and Berger-Sweeney J. (2007). Behavioral and anatomical abnormalities in Mecp2 mutant mice: a model for Rett syndrome. Neuroscience 146, 907-921. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tronche F., Kellendonk C., Kretz O., Gass P., Anlag K., Orban P. C., Bock R., Klein R. and Schütz G. (1999). Disruption of the glucocorticoid receptor gene in the nervous system results in reduced anxiety. Nat. Genet. 23, 99-103. 10.1038/12703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulici V., Hoenselaar K. D., Agoston H., McErlain D. D., Umoh J., Chakrabarti S., Holdsworth D. W. and Beier F. (2009). The role of Akt1 in terminal stages of endochondral bone formation: angiogenesis and ossification. Bone 45, 1133-1145. 10.1016/j.bone.2009.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorhees C. V. and Williams M. T. (2006). Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat. Protoc. 1, 848-858. 10.1038/nprot.2006.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wan H., Aggleton J. P. and Brown M. W. (1999). Different contributions of the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex to recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 19, 1142-1148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson L. A., Solomon L. A., Li J. R., Jiang Y., Edwards M., Shin-ya K., Beier F. and Bérubé N. G. (2013). Atrx deficiency induces telomere dysfunction, endocrine defects, and reduced life span. J. Clin. Invest. 123, 2049-2063. 10.1172/JCI65634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiese C., Rolletschek A., Kania G., Blyszczuk P., Tarasov K. V., Tarasova Y., Wersto R. P., Boheler K. R. and Wobus A. M. (2004). Nestin expression--a property of multi-lineage progenitor cells? Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 61, 2510-2522. 10.1007/s00018-004-4144-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing W., Govoni K. E., Donahue L. R., Kesavan C., Wergedal J., Long C., Bassett J. H. D., Gogakos A., Wojcicka A., Williams G. R. et al. (2012). Genetic evidence that thyroid hormone is indispensable for prepubertal insulin-like growth factor-I expression and bone acquisition in mice. J. Bone Miner. Res. 27, 1067-1079. 10.1002/jbmr.1551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xue Y., Gibbons R., Yan Z., Yang D., McDowell T. L., Sechi S., Qin J., Zhou S., Higgs D. and Wang W. (2003). The ATRX syndrome protein forms a chromatin-remodeling complex with Daxx and localizes in promyelocytic leukemia nuclear bodies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 10635-10640. 10.1073/pnas.1937626100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J., Ji F., Liu Y., Lei X., Li H., Ji G., Yuan Z. and Jiao J. (2014). Ezh2 regulates adult hippocampal neurogenesis and memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 5184-5199. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4129-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]