Abstract
For over 150 years, researchers have investigated the anti-predator function of animal patterns. However, this work has mainly focused on when prey remain still, and has only recently started to incorporate motion into the study of defensive coloration. As motion breaks camouflage, a new challenge is to understand how prey avoid predators while moving around their environment, and if a moving prey can ever be camouflaged. We propose that there is a solution to this, in that a ‘flicker fusion effect’ can change the appearance of the prey in the eyes of their predators to reduce the chances of initial detection. This effect occurs when a high contrast pattern blurs at speed, changing the appearance of the prey, which may help them better match their background. Despite being widely discussed in the literature, the flicker fusion effect is poorly described, there is no clear theoretical framework for testing how it might reduce predation, and the terminology describing it is, at best, rather confusing. Our review addresses these three key issues to enable researchers to formulate precise predictions about when the flicker fusion effect occurs, and to test how it can reduce predation.
Keywords: flicker fusion effect, camouflage, motion, deimatic display, startle, dazzle coloration
1. Introduction
Prey use an incredible array of different strategies to avoid predators [1]. These include signalling defences to predators using warning coloration [2,3], avoiding detection by predators through camouflage [4], and mimicking inedible objects in the environment to avoid being recognized [5]. These defensive strategies have been largely studied in the context of how a prey's appearance enhances its survival when it is stationary. However, given that many prey need to move around their environment (e.g. to find resources and mates), or use movement as part of their defensive display, there is increasing interest in how defensive coloration and movement interact to reduce predation [6–10]. Incorporating motion into the study of defensive coloration is important because not only can it change the efficacy of a defensive strategy [6], but also it raises novel questions about how defensive strategies function and are defined [7–11].
One of the major challenges for understanding the anti-predator function of colour patterns of moving prey stems from the fact that ‘motion breaks camouflage’ [10]: if moving prey cannot conceal themselves through camouflage, what kind of patterns could help reduce predation? One possibility is that colour patterns elicit visual illusions in predators when prey are moving, making them hard to capture. For example, high contrast visible patterns could elicit ‘motion dazzle’, impairing predators' judgements of speed and/or trajectory of moving targets [12–16].
However, there is another visual illusion that has received much less attention, but which could in fact help moving prey defend and possibly conceal themselves rather than just make them tricky to catch. The ‘flicker fusion effect’ can cause a change in a prey's appearance if it moves sufficiently quickly that its pattern becomes blurred [17]. If that change in appearance enables prey to better match their background, it could reduce the chances that they are detected by a predator. Despite anecdotal reports by researchers of this change in appearance occurring in the wild [17,18], it is still not known if, or how, it might work to deter natural predators. As a consequence, the flicker fusion effect remains poorly defined and understood, despite it having the potential to be a unique way to reduce predation across a wide range of prey species [19].
Our review will address three main issues. The first is to explain the psychophysics behind the illusion of flicker fusion effect so that we can precisely predict when it is found in nature, and what factors affect its occurrence. The second is to disentangle the putative functions of the flicker fusion effect. Enhanced concealment through background matching is not the only possible function, and we discuss other functions suggested in the literature. Finally, we will clarify the terminology surrounding the flicker fusion effect to avoid confusion, particularly with other strategies involving movement and coloration. We aim to facilitate the study of the flicker fusion effect in the context of prey defences, and particularly, to highlight its potential role in enhancing concealment of moving prey.
2. What is the mechanism underlying the flicker fusion effect?
It was 40 years ago that Pough [17] wrote about prey changing their appearance when in motion compared with when they were static. He observed striped newborn northern water snakes (Nerodia sipedon) producing sudden bursts of rapid movement in response to a threat which meant that their stripes blurred together to make them appear uniformly coloured (figure 1). He suggested that this change in appearance, from striped to uniform, was due to the fact that snakes' pattern elements were alternating faster than the observer's critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF), hence the name ‘flicker fusion effect’ [18].
The CFF is a measure of a visual system's ability to resolve rapid stimulus change, and is defined as the maximum temporal frequency at which a light can flicker before being perceived as continuous [20]. But how does this relate to a predator's ability to resolve the stripes of a moving prey? When a striped prey moves across a predator's visual field, the pattern elements locally alternate between light and dark (figure 2a). If the prey moves fast enough, the frequency of alternation, known as the temporal frequency, will exceed the maximum frequency that the predator can temporally resolve, and the stripes will blur and no longer be perceived. The temporal frequency at which the stripes alternate depends on the stripe width and on the speed at which the prey moves, and increases as either the stripes get thinner or the speed increases.
In principle, by knowing the stripe width and the speed of the prey, along with the CFF of the predator (which varies across species, see [21]), it should be possible to predict when the primary visual effect of blurring will occur in the eyes of a predator [18,22]. However, it is not quite that simple. The CFF is generally measured using a whole field flickering stimulus, which means that it is measured with a visual stimulus that has no internal pattern. By contrast, Pough's striped water snakes represent patterned visual stimuli, which are characterized by their pattern spatial frequency: spatial frequency is the number of cycles of alternating dark and light stripes per degree of visual angle (figure 2a). For patterned stimuli, this is substantially above zero, while for uniform ones, it is equal to zero. Empirically, flicker fusion occurs at lower temporal frequency for patterns having higher spatial frequencies [23]; this means that the temporal frequency at which the stripes of a patterned prey will completely blur is not fixed, but decreases as stripe width decreases. We will call this the threshold for flicker fusion, or the TFF (shown by the red line in figure 2b). Complete blurring occurs above the TFF, although patterns will start to blur below it. The TFF is the same as the CFF only when the stimulus is uniform (i.e. where the TFF intersects the y-axis and spatial frequency is zero); the CFF is the upper limit of resolvable temporal frequency. To help visualize this, imagine a striped moving prey with a defined spatial (Flow) and temporal frequency (Wlow); when slow moving, its pattern remains visible to a predator (P; figure 2b). However, if the prey has thinner stripes (1; figure 2b) with a higher spatial frequency (Fhigh), or moves faster (2; figure 2b), resulting in a higher temporal frequency (Whigh), its stripes will alternate faster than the predator's threshold for flicker fusion (i.e. W > TFF). Consequently, it will no longer be possible for the predator to distinguish the pattern elements and the striped prey will appear uniform.
Besides the TFF, there are other factors that also influence the occurrence of the flicker fusion effect (table 1). In particular, the viewing conditions are critical. If the prey is further away, its stripes will appear thinner to the predator, i.e. the spatial frequency increases, and the flicker fusion effect will occur at lower prey speeds. In addition, as ambient luminance decreases, the TFF also decreases because animals' eyes visually sample their environment less frequently and integrate photon capture over longer periods in order to try and gather sufficient visual information from their environment [24]. When animal eyes become adapted to low lighting conditions, spatial acuity can also decrease [25]. This means that the flicker fusion effect is more likely to occur under dim compared with bright conditions [22], because prey do not need to be moving as fast for blurring to occur. While for a given speed, finer stripes will produce faster alternation (and more likely exceed the TFF), other things being equal, the speed necessary for the flicker fusion effect will always be lower for prey patterns of lower internal contrast. The same principles discussed so far can be applied to prey having non-striped patterns, e.g. zigzag or spotted, if the elements are repeated along the vector of motion. When the temporal frequency at which these elements alternate exceeds a predator's TFF, the prey will appear uniform or even differently patterned while moving.
Table 1.
factor | impact | |
---|---|---|
viewing conditions | distance from the prey | increasing viewing distance increases the spatial frequency of the pattern as seen by the predator, making the flicker fusion effect more likely to occur |
ambient light | at lower illumination, animals integrate visual information over longer times and TFFs decrease more rapidly: the flicker fusion effect can occur at lower speeds | |
prey pattern and movement | speed | adequate speed is required for blurring of pattern elements to occur |
stripe width | thinner stripes will blur at lower speeds as they produce more rapid temporal frequency | |
pattern internal contrast | low contrast patterns blur at lower speeds than high contrast ones | |
orientation of pattern elements | blurring occurs when elements are repeated along the vector of motion | |
predator vision | spatio-temporal acuity | increasing spatio-temporal acuity of the predator requires higher speeds for the flicker fusion effect to occur (in figure 2b, TFF will shift towards higher temporal and spatial frequencies) |
contrast sensitivity | the more sensitive the predator is to contrast at the relevant luminance level, the harder the flicker fusion effect is to achieve (in figure 2b, TFF shifts upwards and declines more steeply) | |
fixation | if the predator tracks the prey to stabilize it on the retina, the effective speed of the prey will be reduced, weakening or abolishing the flicker fusion effect |
Ultimately, the speed necessary for the flicker fusion effect to occur depends upon the predator's contrast sensitivity function. Contrast sensitivity is defined as 1/contrast threshold, where the threshold is the minimum contrast required for the predator to detect a pattern. The contrast sensitivity function describes how contrast sensitivity varies as a function of spatial and temporal frequency. The red ‘TFF’ line in figure 2b corresponds to a line of constant contrast sensitivity; for predators with more sensitive vision, the TFF line will be shifted upwards and decline less steeply with spatial frequency [23]. And of course, flicker fusion only occurs if the predator's eyes remain stationary as the prey moves: if it tracks the prey and stabilizes it on the retina, blurring will not occur.
Given this complex interaction of factors affecting the occurrence of the flicker fusion effect, how often might it occur in the wild? To date, the evidence is limited to striped coral snake mimics (Lampropeltis triangulum campbelli and L. elapsoides), which are calculated to move fast enough when in flight for their patterns to blur in the eyes of some potential predators (raptors), particularly in dim light [22]. Indeed, the effect may be particularly prevalent in low light intensity environments, including deep water or forest environments. However, while demonstrating the feasibility of the flicker fusion effect, these calculations are likely to be conservative as they were based on predators' CFF values, and prey do not need to move as fast to blur based on the TFF. Blurring through the flicker fusion effect may be occurring more often in nature than previously thought (e.g. [22]), and occur in slower moving prey, not just those performing a rapid escape.
Although calculations for the flicker fusion effect have been made predominantly with avian predators in mind, other species of predator (e.g. insects [26]) have lower visual acuities. This means that the flicker fusion effect could be occurring more often in the eyes of these predators. Based on praying mantis' visual acuity [26], we calculate that a bumblebee's pattern will certainly be perceived as blurred at a typical viewing and strike distance of 5 cm [26] when the insect flies at 0.25 m s−1 (Bombus terrestris maximum flying speed is 6 m s−1 [27]). Consequently, the flicker fusion effect could be more widespread than it initially would appear, and not restricted to striped snakes. In the same way that UV colours were ignored for a long time because we could not see them [28], blurring through the flicker fusion effect may also have been an underappreciated feature of animal coloration because of our own visual biases.
3. What defensive function might the flicker fusion effect have?
The second issue to address is what anti-predator function the flicker fusion effect may have. While changes in appearance caused by the flicker fusion effect have been widely assumed to be an adaptation to reduce predation (e.g. [29,30]), the problem is that there have been no tests with actual predators. So far, the evidence for the anti-predator function of the flicker fusion effect comes from indirect observations in snakes [22,31]. For example, the ‘zigzag’ morphs of Vipera berus appear to have a higher survival advantage compared with other morphs, but there is no evidence that this results from reduced predation, and if it does, how that occurs [31]. Therefore, it could be argued that the flicker fusion effect is simply the by-product of rapid movement that has been selected to escape a predator, rather than part of a defensive strategy. It is important to identify how the flicker fusion effect might work in order to conduct experiments with predators to distinguish among functional hypotheses. We have attempted to disentangle the proposed explanations to provide a theoretical framework for the future study of how the flicker fusion effect could help reduce predation (table 2).
Table 2.
function | how this is achieved |
---|---|
camouflage the moving prey | the uniform coloration from blurring helps prey match general features of their background, and enhances concealment [19,30] |
alter the perception of motion | the change in appearance during movement alters the prey's perceived speed or trajectory, making it difficult to capture [18,30] |
hide the final resting location | a sudden change in appearance from the moving to the static prey pattern makes it difficult for a predator to locate resting prey [17,22,29] |
deter predators | a sudden change in appearance caused by the flicker fusion effect may cause the attacking predator to show neophobia or hesitate, giving the prey an increased opportunity to escape |
As already mentioned, the flicker fusion effect could help prey to become more camouflaged during movement [19,30]. Pough's [17] original observations included how the uniform appearance generated by the flicker fusion effect in the escape responses of snakes made them appear to blend into their environment. This could happen, for example, if prey's coloration matches the mean luminance of the background, even though it has a high contrast visual texture (e.g. stripes); such prey might be highly conspicuous when remaining still but could become camouflaged when moving fast enough for the flicker fusion effect to occur [19]. If the flicker fusion effect does indeed improve background matching, it would be the basis for a unique form of camouflage in moving prey: rather than concealing the speed or trajectory of the prey's motion once detected (like motion dazzle; [13]), the flicker fusion effect would reduce the chances of initial detection.
However, it is possible that the flicker fusion effect could help prevent capture by ‘confusing’ predators, and making it difficult for them to track and effectively capture the prey; for example, pattern blurring could cause predators to lose internal reference points [15]. The effect of blurring could also lead to additional illusory effects, such as altering prey's perceived speed due to the loss of internal contrast in the pattern or reduced contrast against the background [30,32,33]. While this latter idea could be considered a form of motion dazzle [30], the idea and study of motion dazzle has thus far relied upon the prey's pattern being visible to the predator when it is moving [1,13–16]. Therefore, if the flicker fusion effect also changes the speed and/or trajectory of prey through pattern blurring, it must be due to different perceptual mechanisms than those already proposed (e.g. [13,16]) and not through the pattern ‘dazzling’ predators.
A third way that the flicker fusion effect could help reduce predation is by hiding the final resting place of a moving prey, making it difficult to locate once it becomes stationary again [17]. This idea is perhaps similar to the idea of ‘flash coloration’, where an otherwise camouflaged prey suddenly reveals a conspicuous body part when it flees a predator, only to hide it again before or as it comes to rest [1,34]. Although the benefits of flash coloration are not established, it is thought that if a predator tracks the moving prey using its conspicuous coloration, it will subsequently be less able to detect cryptic features of the prey's camouflage pattern (perhaps through loss of a search image; [35]). In the case of the flicker fusion effect, when the prey suddenly becomes stationary with a cryptic pattern, the predator would continue to look for the prey based on its appearance when moving. The problem of finding the stationary cryptic prey could be further exacerbated if the predator predicts the movement of the prey along the perceived trajectory, and searches in the wrong place, either because it looks further along the path than where the prey has actually stopped [17], or less far because of misjudging the speed [33].
Finally, the flicker fusion effect could simply be a way to deter predators: a novel dynamically changing appearance could elicit neophobia or an avoidance response, similar to that of warning signals (e.g. [36,37]). Alternatively, perhaps the sudden change in coloration is a deimatic display eliciting a startle or fear response in its predators [11], that gives prey an advantage to escape. In these cases, the flicker fusion effect simply performs a well-established defensive function.
We acknowledge that this may not be an exhaustive list, and of course, differences in hunting strategies or visual systems among predator species means that the flicker fusion effect could serve more than one defensive function, even for a single prey species. However, what is clear is that we need to know how it works, and particularly if it is a form of camouflage or a deterrent. We think it is particularly important to establish if it is a unique form of concealment, where blurring of an internal pattern at speed could reduce the initial detection of prey. The question of whether any pattern can reduce the detection of moving prey is one of the major unanswered questions in the study of defensive coloration. Currently, only the flicker fusion effect offers a possible solution.
4. How is confusion arising through current terminology?
To investigate the flicker fusion effect, we need to be clear about what it is we refer to when using this term. This is because the flicker fusion effect has not just been used to describe the mechanism by which the appearance of a prey's pattern changes (e.g. [14,30,31,38]), but has also given its name to a hypothesis [29–31], and been used to describe a camouflage strategy [4,12,30]. This has led to what we see as some confusion in the literature.
For example, some researchers refer to a ‘flicker fusion hypothesis'; however, it is not clear what this is. Sometimes, it refers to the mechanism and whether or not it is possible that blurring occurs through the flicker fusion effect [22,30], while other times it refers to whether or not the blurring could confer a survival advantage [29,31]. While this is confusing in itself, there is of course the additional problem that there are multiple functional hypotheses relating to how it might reduce predation (table 2). The use of the term ‘flicker fusion hypothesis’ has the potential to lead to considerable confusion about what the hypothesis actually is, and we suggest that it is abandoned altogether.
The flicker fusion effect has also been used to describe a specific defensive strategy, ‘flicker fusion camouflage’ [12,30], which describes the situation when the effect helps prey better match their backgrounds by making prey ‘uniformly camouflaged’ [12]. The problem with the use of this term is that it suggests that the function of the flicker fusion effect is to camouflage the prey, while several other possible functions exist (table 2). While calling motion dazzle a form of camouflage works because its only possible function is to hide the movement of the prey (‘dazzle camouflage’; [13,14]), the same logic cannot be applied to the flicker fusion effect, because it might instead deter predators. Unless we know that patterns have evolved to elicit the flicker fusion effect to enhance concealment, we suggest that it is best not to use this term.
However, we do still need terminology that allows us to study flicker fusion, so what terminology should we be using? Our view is that the flicker fusion effect should be limited to describing the visual illusion that alters the perceived pattern of a prey when it moves sufficiently quickly to exceed the predator TFF. This definition accurately describes how pattern and speed interact to produce a change in appearance in the eyes of the predator, and does not ascribe any particular function to the effect. Avoiding using flicker fusion effect in relation to any functional role reduces any implicit bias in understanding how it works. By clearly separating the mechanism (the perceptual effect) from the function (how it deters predators), our proposed terminology allows researchers to study one or the other, or both. Only once functions are better explored and identified should we start to use flicker fusion in ways that align it to particular defensive strategies.
5. Conclusion
For a long time, the flicker fusion effect has been thought to confer a selective advantage to several snake species fleeing from putative predators. By exploring the psychophysical principles behind the effect, we hope to have highlighted how widespread the effect could be. Striped patterns in particular, but also other patterns types, common across many taxa, could blur at speed given what we know about the visual capabilities of different species of predators.
It is clear that we need more studies of the flicker fusion effect in order to understand when it occurs, and what its effect(s) are on predators. Understanding how the flicker fusion effect works is likely to be solved by a combination of approaches. Field observations will be important for establishing how the effect might function and if it could be involved in contexts other than predation (e.g. signalling to mates). Nonetheless, psychophysics experiments in the laboratory are likely to provide valuable insights into its perceptual basis, and tests with computer generated targets can be readily conducted with predatory species, such as birds and mantids (e.g. [26,39]).
Despite these challenges to fully understand when and how it works in the wild, the study of the flicker fusion effect, to our eyes, offers an exciting opportunity to discover new ways in which a prey's appearance and behaviour have evolved to reduce predation. Notably, the flicker fusion effect fundamentally differs from other defensive strategies involving movement and patterning, because it allows prey to look different when moving and when stationary. Crucially, it has the potential to conceal an animal during motion, reducing the chances of it being detected by a predator. Perhaps, when combined with the right pattern, motion need not always break camouflage.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Christina Halpin, Matthew Wheelwright, Robert Elwood, Adam Kane and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Competing interests
We have no competing interests.
Funding
J.C.A.R. is supported by a Leverhulme Trust Research Leadership Award (RL-2012-019), and D.U. by a Best Erasmus Traineeship for Jobs Scholarship.
References
- 1.Cott HB. 1940. Adaptive coloration in animals. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mappes J, Marples N, Endler JA. 2005. The complex business of survival by aposematism. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 598–603. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2005.07.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rowe C, Halpin C. 2013. Why are warning displays multimodal? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1425–1439. ( 10.1007/s00265-013-1515-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stevens M, Merilaita S. 2009. Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 423–427. ( 10.1098/rstb.2008.0217) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Skelhorn J. 2015. Masquerade. Curr. Biol. 25, R643–R644. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.069) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stevens M, Searle WTL, Seymour JE, Marshall KLA, Ruxton GD. 2011. Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses. BMC Biol. 9, 81 ( 10.1186/1741-7007-9-81) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Umbers KDL, Mappes J. 2016. Towards a tractable working hypothesis for deimatic displays. Anim. Behav. 113, E5–E7. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.01.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bian X, Elgar MA, Peters RA. 2016. The swaying behavior of Extatosoma tiaratum: motion camouflage in a stick insect? Behav. Ecol. 27, 83–92. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv125) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Srygley RB. 1999. Incorporating motion into investigations of mimicry. Evol. Ecol. 13, 691–708. ( 10.1023/a:1011046202928) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hall JR, Cuthill IC, Baddeley R, Shohet AJ, Scott-Samuel NE. 2013. Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 1785 ( 10.1098/Rspb.2013.0064) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Skelhorn J, Holmes GG, Rowe C. 2016. Deimatic or aposematic? Anim. Behav. 113, E1–E3. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Stevens M, Yule DH, Ruxton GD. 2008. Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2639–2643. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.0877) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Scott-Samuel NE, Baddeley R, Palmer CE, Cuthill IC. 2011. Dazzle camouflage affects speed perception. PLoS ONE 6, e20233 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0020233) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Von Helversen B, Schooler LJ, Czienskowski U. 2013. Are stripes beneficial? Dazzle camouflage influences perceived speed and hit rates. PLoS ONE 8, e61173 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0061173) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hughes AE, Troscianko J, Stevens M. 2014. Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success. BMC Evol. Biol. 14, 1 ( 10.1186/S12862-014-0201-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kelley LA, Kelley JL. 2014. Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. Behav. Ecol. 25, 450–463. ( 10.1093/beheco/art118) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pough FH. 1976. Multiple cryptic effects of crossbanded and ringed patterns of snakes. Copeia 4, 834–836. ( 10.2307/1443481) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jackson JF, Ingram W, Campbell HW. 1976. Dorsal pigmentation pattern of snakes as an anti-predator strategy. Am. Nat. 110, 1029–1053. ( 10.1086/283125) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Endler JA. 1978. A predator's view of animal color patterns. In Evolutionary biology (eds Hecht MK, Steere WC, Wallace B), pp. 319–364. Boston, MA: Springer US. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Talbot HF. 1834. XLIV. Experiments on light. Phil. Mag. Series 3 5, 321–334. ( 10.1080/14786443408648474) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Healy K, McNally L, Ruxton GD, Cooper N, Jackson AL. 2013. Metabolic rate and body size are linked with perception of temporal information. Anim. Behav. 86, 685–696. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.06.018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Titcomb GC, Kikuchi DW, Pfennig DW. 2014. More than mimicry? Evaluating scope for flicker-fusion as a defensive strategy in coral snake mimics. Curr. Zool. 60, 123–130. ( 10.1093/czoolo/60.1.123123-130) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Watson AB, Ahumada AJ.2016. The pyramid of visibility. In IS&T International Symposium on Electronic Imaging, 2016, Human Vision and Electronic Imaging 2016 , pp. HVEI-102.1–HVEI-102.6. Springfield, VA: Society for Imaging Science and Technology. ( ) [DOI]
- 24.Tyler CW, Hamer RD. 1990. Analysis of visual modulation sensitivity. IV. Validity of the Ferry–Porter law. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 7, 743–758. ( 10.1364/JOSAA.7.000743) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Van Nes FL, Koenderink JJ, Nas H, Bouman MA. 1967. Spatiotemporal modulation transfer in the human eye. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 57, 1082–1088. ( 10.1364/JOSA.57.001082) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Nityananda V, Tarawneh G, Jones L, Busby N, Herbert W, Davies R, Read JCA. 2015. The contrast sensitivity function of the praying mantis Sphodromantis lineola. J. Comp. Physiol. A 201, 741–750. ( 10.1007/s00359-015-1008-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ellington CP. 1999. The novel aerodynamics of insect flight: applications to micro-air vehicles. J. Exp. Biol. 202, 3439–3448. (doi:202:3439-3448) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bennett ATD, Cuthill IC. 1994. Ultraviolet vision in birds: what is its function? Vision Res. 34, 1471–1478. ( 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90149-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed M. 2004. Avoiding attack. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stevens M. 2007. Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 1457–1464. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.0220) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lindell LE, Forsman A. 1996. Sexual dichromatism in snakes: support for the flicker-fusion hypothesis. Can. J. Zool. 74, 2254–2256. ( 10.1139/Z96-256) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Blakemore MR, Snowden RJ. 2000. Textured backgrounds alter perceived speed. Vision Res. 40, 629–638. ( 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00214-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Thompson P. 1982. Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast. Vision Res. 22, 377–380. ( 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90153-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Edmunds M. 2005. Flash colors. In Encyclopedia of entomology, p. 1466 Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Troscianko J, Lown AE, Hughes AE, Stevens M. 2013. Defeating crypsis: detection and learning of camouflage strategies. PLoS ONE 8, 8 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0073733) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Roper TJ, Cook SE. 1989. Response of chicks to brightly colored insect prey. Behaviour 110, 276–293. () [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Roper TJ, Wistow R. 1986. Aposematic coloration and avoidance-learning in chicks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 141–149. ( 10.1080/14640748608402225) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Niskanen M, Mappes J. 2005. Significance of the dorsal zigzag pattern of Vipera latastei gaditana against avian predators. J. Anim. Ecol. 74, 1091–1101. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2005.01008.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dittrich W, Gilbert F, Green P, McGregor P, Grewcock D. 1993. Imperfect mimicry—a pigeon perspective. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 251, 195–200. ( 10.1098/rspb.1993.0029) [DOI] [Google Scholar]