Abstract
Recent epidemiological data suggest that the lifetime prevalence of gambling problems differs depending on race-ethnicity. Understanding variations in disease presentation in blacks and whites, and relationships with biological and sociocultural factors, may have implications for selecting appropriate prevention strategies. 62 non-treatment seeking volunteers (18-29 years, n=18 [29.0%] female) with gambling disorder were recruited from the general community. Black (n=36) and White (n=26) participants were compared on demographic, clinical and cognitive measures. Young black adults with gambling disorder reported more symptoms of gambling disorder and greater scores on a measure of compulsivity. In addition they exhibited significantly higher total errors on a set-shifting task, less risk adjustment on a gambling task, greater delay aversion on a gambling task, and more total errors on a working memory task. These findings suggest that the clinical and neurocognitive presentation of gambling disorder different between racial-ethnic groups.
Keywords: addiction, cognition, gambling, impulsivity, phenomenology, race
1.0. Introduction
Gambling is a commonplace activity across cultures, and in extreme forms, can evolve into Gambling Disorder, a behavioral problem characterized by persistent, recurrent maladaptive patterns of gambling behavior and functional impairment (APA, 2013). Recent research that examined racial-ethnic differences in lifetime prevalence of problem gambling found higher rates among black (2.2%) compared to white (1.2%) respondents (Alegria et al., 2009). Health initiatives have highlighted the importance of understanding racial-ethnic differences (http://minorityhealth.hhs.gov). In terms of clinical presentation, there is considerable heterogeneity across individuals with gambling problems (Hodgins et al., 2011), and a small but growing number of studies have found that significant clinical differences exist between African-American and White gamblers (Barry et al., 2008, 2011; Sacco et al., 2011). For example, African-American low- or at-risk gamblers appear more likely to report a co-occurring mood disorder, hypomania, or any substance use disorder (Barry et al., 2011). Other research has shown that African-American gamblers appear more likely to endorse trying to cut back on their gambling and more likely to endorse the suffering of losses (Sacco et al., 2011). Telephone callers to a gambling helpline who were African-American were more likely than white callers to report longer durations of gambling problems but less likely to report daily tobacco use or mental health treatment (Barry et al., 2008).
Several factors may influence race or ethnic differences in the development or presentation of gambling disorder, including environmental and clinical factors. Previous research on race-ethnicity and gambling has shown that environmental factors such as low-income and living in a disadvantaged neighborhood influence the development of psychiatric problems, including problematic gambling behavior, in children and adolescents (Martins et al., 2013). In addition to exploring these sociodemographic variables or overt clinical presentations with respect to race or ethnicity, however, it is potentially important to also consider the underpinning neurobiological factors such as cognition. This accords with the recent shift towards Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), proposed by the National Institute for Mental Health (NIMH), which seeks to improve classification and treatment of mental disorders by considering dimensions including cognition (Insel et al., 2010). Previous research demonstrated that individuals with gambling disorder demonstrate cognitive impairments relating to several aspects of decision-making (van Holst et al., 2010a). The finding of decision-making deficits in people with gambling disorder fits with neurobiological models of gambling disorder and findings from the broader cognitive literature (Clark, 2010; van Holst et al., 2010a; van Holst et al., 2010b). Unfortunately, racial-ethnic influences over cognitive function in relation to gambling disorder have yet to be explored. Common cognitive problems between races-ethnicities may be suggestive of common neural dysfunction while distinct deficits may be suggestive of differential pathophysiology.
Given the unknown influence of cognition on gambling pathophysiology, we sought to examine potential cognitive differences between young black and white adults with gambling disorder. Based on previous research suggesting higher rates of gambling problems among African-Americans, as well as greater comorbidity with mood and substance use disorder (Barry et al., 2011), our hypothesis was that black gamblers would report more severe gambling symptoms and relative impairment with respect to inhibitory control, working memory, and decision-making.
2.0. Methods
2.1. Subjects
Participants comprised non-treatment-seeking young adults aged 18-29 years, recruited as part of a longitudinal study of impulsive behaviors. Subjects were self-selected in response to media announcements in a metropolitan area (“have you ever gambled?”). Inclusion criteria were: gambling in any form at least five times during the preceding 12-months, and presence of subsyndromal gambling disorder (for definition see below). Exclusion criteria included an inability to understand/undertake the procedures and to provide written informed consent, and presence of formal gambling disorder (as opposed to at-risk gambling disorder – see below). Since we sought to examine a naturalistic sample, subjects with psychiatric and substance use comorbidity, as well as those subjects currently taking psychotropic medications, were all allowed to participate.
The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Institutional Review Board of the University of Chicago approved the study and the consent statement. After all study procedures were explained, subjects provided voluntary written informed consent. Participants were compensated with a $50 gift card for a local department store.
2.2. Clinical Assessments
Raters assessed each participant using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) to examine psychiatric morbidity; and the modified Structured Clinical Interview for Gambling Disorder (SCI-GD) (Grant et al., 2004), which is a nine-item instrument assessing symptoms of gambling disorder (consistent with DSM-5): a score of ≥4 indicates current gambling disorder (Grant et al., 2011). Participants were assessed for the frequency of gambling behavior as well as money lost gambling using a timeline follow-back method for gambling (Weinstock et al., 2004). In addition, participants completed the Padua Inventory, a 60-item questionnaire originally developed to study obsessive-compulsive thoughts and behaviors in the general population (Sanavio, 1988). It yields a total score, derived on the basis of factor analysis.
2.3. Cognitive Assessments
Cognitive testing was undertaken in quiet room using a touch-screen computer, with a trained assessor present. We utilized tests selected from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB). The cognitive domains of interest were response inhibition, working memory, and decision-making. We focused on these areas after considering the existing literature, these being domains commonly implicated in the pathophysiology of gambling symptomatology (e.g. Clark, 2010; van Holst et al., 2010a; van Holst et al., 2010b; Grant et al., 2011; Odlaug et al., 2011).
We assessed response inhibition using the Stop-Signal Task (Aron et al., 2004), a paradigm in which the subject viewed a series of directional arrows appearing one per time on-screen, and made speeded motor responses depending on the direction of each arrow (left button for a left-facing arrow, and vice versa). On a subset of trials, an auditory stop-signal occurred (‘beep’) to indicate to volunteers that response suppression was needed for the given trial. This task uses a dynamic tracking algorithm to calculate the ‘stop-signal reaction time’, which is an estimate of the time taken by the given volunteer’s brain to suppress a response that would normally be undertaken. The task also recorded median ‘go’ reaction times, which is the average response latency for trials not involving a stop-signal; this is a measure of general ‘response speed’ rather than inhibitory control.
All subjects completed the Spatial Working Memory task (SWM) (Owen et al., 1990). On the SWM test (8-box version), participants attempt to locate tokens hidden underneath boxes on-screen and try to avoid returning to boxes that previously yielded such tokens. The key outcome measures include the “total number of errors” (inappropriately returning to boxes that previously yielded tokens) and “strategy score” (lower score equates to more optimal strategy use).
Different aspects of decision-making were quantified using the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) (Rogers et al., 1999). There were four practice trials followed by eight blocks of nine trials. At the start of each block, the ‘cumulative points’ setting on the task was reset to 100. On each trial, subjects were shown a set of red and blue boxes, totaling ten. The ratio of red:blue boxes were varied over the course of the task pseudo-randomly (box-ratios: 9_1, 8_2, 7_3, 6_4). Subjects were informed that for each trial, the computer had hidden a ‘token’ inside one of the boxes, and that they had to indicate whether they felt the token would be hidden behind a red or a blue box. This choice was made by selecting ‘red’ or ‘blue’ using the touch-screen interface. After making this judgment, subjects were required to gamble a proportion of their points on whether their color choice was correct. The key outcome measures were (i) mean proportion of points gambled; (ii) quality of decision-making (the proportion of trials where the volunteer chose red when red boxes were in the majority and vice versa – i.e. made the logical color choice); and (iii) risk adjustment (tendency to adjust how many points are gambled depending on the degree of risk).
2.4. Data Analysis
Subjects meeting criteria for gambling disorder were grouped into two categories based on race-ethnicity: blacks and whites. Potential differences in demographic, clinical and cognitive variables between the groups were explored using three separate multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs); this approach provided protection against false positive errors, in that individual variables were only explored when the broader category of interest (demographic, clinical, or cognitive) showed a main effect of group. Where a composite MANOVA test obtained statistical significance, differences between groups were explored for individual variables within the given analysis. Demographic variables found to differ significantly between groups were entered as covariates into subsequent MANOVAs. Statistical significance was defined as p<0.05 uncorrected. IBM SPSS Software, Version 19 was used for the analyses. For MANOVA, data were checked for normality and other assumptions using Box’s M, tests for linearity (inter-variable correlations), Levine’s tests, and Mahalanobis distances. At the level of individual test measures, data were inspected for violation of assumptions including normality using Shapiro Wilk tests and visual displays; any significant findings at the level of individual variables, where there was any evidence of violation of model assumptions, were confirmed or refuted using non-parametric tests as appropriate.
We also undertook secondary analysis using correlation (Spearman’s) tests, to explore possible relationships between clinical measures and cognitive measures; because of the number of tests entailed, and the exploratory nature of these tests, we corrected for multiplicity by using a significance threshold of 0.05/42 = 0.0012 (42 correlational tests).
3.0. Results
The sample included 62 (18 [29.0%] female) young adults with gambling disorder. A total of 36 black and 26 white gamblers were included. MANOVA for demographic variables indicated that the two groups differed overall (F=2.725, df 1,55, p=0.022). As can be seen in Table 1 (top section), this was due to the black gamblers earning significantly more dollars in the preceding year than the white gamblers. This variable was taken forward as a covariate into subsequent analyses.
Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Blacks and Whites with Gambling Disorder, Results from MANOVAs.
Blacks n=36 | Whites n=26 | Statistic | df | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic variables | |||||
Age, years | 24.9 (2.9) | 23.9 (3.7) | 1.409 | 1,58 | .240 |
Female, n (%) | 13 (36.1) | 5 (19.2) | 2.088c | 1 | .148 |
Education, n (%) | 5.057c | 4 | .281 | ||
Less than high school | 1 (2.8) | 2 (7.7) | |||
High school degree or GED | 10 (27.8) | 1 (3.8) | |||
Some college | 19 (52.8) | 15 (57.7) | |||
College Graduate | 5 (13.8) | 8 (30.8) | |||
Post-Graduate | 1 (2.8) | 0 (0) | |||
Marital Status, n (%) | 4.285c | 3 | 0.232 | ||
Single | 31 (86.1) | 21 (80.8) | |||
Married | 3 (8.3) | 0 (0) | |||
Divorced/Separated | 0 (0) | 4 (15.4) | |||
Living together or engaged | 2 (5.6) | 1 (3.8) | |||
Employment, n (%) | 6.029c | 4 | .197 | ||
Works full-time | 11 (30.6) | 6 (23.1) | |||
Works part-time | 10 (27.8) | 3 (11.5) | |||
Student | 4 (11.1) | 6 (23.1) | |||
Student and employed | 1 (2.8) | 6 (23.1) | |||
Unemployed | 10 (27.8) | 5 (19.2) | |||
Income for the past year, dollars | 27779.08 (18866.58) | 18096.15 (14271.66) | 4.839& | 1,58 | .032 |
Clinical Variables | |||||
Age at onset of first gambling, years | 13.9 (4.0) | 14.8 (4.7) | 1.181 | 1, 58 | .282 |
Gambling frequency per week, times per week | 5.2 (3.7) | 3.3 (2.3) | 5.884& | 1, 58 | .018 |
Amount lost to gambling for the past year, dollars | 5777.08 (6694.99) | 5130.77 (8705.33) | 0.361 | 1, 58 | .550 |
SCI-PG scores# | 6.4 (1.7) | 5.0 (1.2) | 11.250@ | 1,58 | .001 |
PG-YBOCS total scores | 18.7 (8.1) | 15.7 (7.7) | 1.312 | 1,58 | .257 |
QOLI t-score | 40.7 (15,6) | 39.9 (17.8) | 0.258 | 1,58 | .613 |
Padua total score## | 40.3 (32.6) | 18.3 (11.0) | 9.928@ | 1,58 | 0.003 |
SCI-PG=Structured Clinical Interview for Pathological Gambling (modified for gambling disorder and DSM-5); PG-YBOCS=Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale modified for pathological gambling; QOLI=Quality of Life Inventory
All values are Mean (± SD) unless otherwise stated
Statistical tests reported in table are F tests, except where indicated: c=chi-square, with Yates’ correction where appropriate.
@ homogeneity of variance violated; group differences remain significant with Welch’s test for SCI-PG scores (Welch=14.269, df 1,59.988, p<0.001) and Padua total scores (Welch=14.163, df 1,45.355, p<0.001); & normality assumptions violated, remains significant with Mann-Whitney for past year income (p=0.034); gambling frequency per week (p=0.015).
The MANOVA for the clinical variables demonstrated that the groups differed on these measures overall (F=3.270, df 1,52, p=0.006). As shown in Table 1 (bottom section), that was due to the black gamblers showing significantly higher gambling frequency per week, higher SCI-PG scores, and higher Padua total scores, as compared to the white gamblers.
In terms of cognitive measures, the two study groups differed overall on the MANOVA (F=2.881, df 1,49, p=0.010). This was due to the black gamblers, as compared to white gamblers, exhibiting significantly higher IDED total errors (adjusted), less CGT risk adjustment, higher CGT delay aversion, and more SWM Total errors (Table 2).
Table 2. Cognitive Characteristics of Blacks and Whites with Gambling Disorder, Results from MANOVA.
Blacks n=36 |
Whites n=26 |
Statistic | df | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
IED Total errors | 43.25 (30.22) | 26.0 (21.03) | 6.210& | 1,59 | .016 |
SST SSRT | 176.30 (65.57) | 192.14 (61.40) | 0.163 | 1,59 | .688 |
SST median go reaction times | 576.14 (210.3) | 484.10 (207.6) | 1.770 | 1,59 | 0.189 |
CGT Quality of decision making | .905 (.091) | .907 (.106) | 0.009 | 1, 59 | .923 |
CGT Risk adjustment | .577 (.806) | 1.358 (1.149) | 7.495& | 1, 59 | .008 |
CGT Delay aversion | .454 (.246) | .236 (.340) | 8.341& | 1, 59 | .006 |
SWM Total errors | 34.36 (21.39) | 22.73 (22.82) | 6.621& | 1, 59 | .013 |
IED=Intradimensional/Extradimensional Set Shift task; CGT=Cambridge Gambling
Task; SWM=Spatial Working Memory task
Statistical tests are F tests.& normality assumptions violated, group difference remains significant with Mann-Whitney for IED Total errors (p=0.013), risk adjustment (p=0.003), delay aversion (p=0.002), and SWM Total errors (p=0.020).
The two study groups did not differ in terms of occurrence of mainstream mental disorders, as shown in Table 3. In the exploratory correlational analysis, when the whole sample was considered, there was a significant negative correlation between CGT risk adjustment and SCIPG total scores (r=-0.452, p<0.0001 uncorrected). This relationship did not remain statistically significant at the corrected threshold when each group was considered separately.
Table 3. Psychiatric Comorbidity in Blacks and Whites with Gambling Disorder.
Blacks n=36 |
Whites n=25 |
Statistic | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Comorbid Current Disorders, n (%) | ||||
Any mood disorder | 7 (19.4) | 2 (8.0) | 1.536 | .215 |
Any anxiety disorder | 9 (25.0) | 7 (28.0) | .069 | .793 |
Post-traumatic stress disorder | 2 (5.6) | 0 (0) | 1.436 | .231 |
Any eating disorder | 0 (0) | 2 (8.0) | 2.978 | .084 |
Any psychotic disorder | 2 (5.6) | 0 (0) | 1.436 | .231 |
Any alcohol use disorder | 15 (41.7) | 10 (40.0) | .017 | .896 |
Any substance use disorder | 15 (41.7) | 6 (24.0) | 2.040 | .153 |
Any current disorder | 24 (66.7) | 14 (56.0) | .715 | .395 |
Statistic is chi-square; df=1
4.0. Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to assess clinical and cognitive differences between black and white young adults with gambling disorder. We found important clinical and cognitive differences between groups which merit further exploration.
We found some evidence that black gamblers exhibited higher levels of gambling symptomatology compared to whites: they met more diagnostic criteria for gambling disorder, and had higher gambling frequency per week. However, groups did not differ significantly in terms of PG-YBOCS total scores, or the amount of money lost to gambling per year, suggesting that these clinical differences did not map onto worse longer-term symptom severity or outcomes. Contrary to other research which has found that black people report longer duration of gambling problems prior to seeking help (Barry et al., 2008), measures of current gambling severity in our study showed no differences. Whether race-ethnicity may play a larger role in symptom severity in those with lower levels of gambling behavior, however, awaits further research.
We found that black gamblers in our sample exhibited higher scores on the measure of obsessive-compulsiveness (Padua) compared to white gamblers; these findings were contrary to our a priori predictions.
From a cognitive point of view, people with gambling disorder have been found to experience impairments across a range of neuropsychological domains. In recent reviews of the literature, cognitive domains often implicated in gambling disorder have included measures of impulsivity (e.g. stop-signal, go/no-go, and temporal discounting), decision-making, and cognitive flexibility (e.g. set-shifting) (Goudriaan et al., 2004; Clark 2010; Goudriaan et al., 2014; Chamberlain et al., 2015). It is interesting to consider, therefore, whether these types of cognitive functions differ between racial-ethnic groups in people with gambling disorder.
In terms of impulsivity, our two racial-ethnic groups did not differ on the SST, a task of motor impulsivity. Previous research, using different methodologies to assess impulsivity, reported higher ratings of impulsivity in African-American youth gamblers (Martins et al., 2008). The Martins et al. (2008) study, however, used parental and teacher report of childhood impulsivity in a sample of adolescence. The impulsive behaviors they witnessed may have little if any relationship to the types of impulsivity we measured in this study. We did not measure temporal discounting in our study as such.
On other cognitive tests, we found an intriguing dissociation: the black gamblers showed relative impairment, versus white gamblers, in terms of cognitive flexibility (set-shifting task), aspects of decision-making (gambling task), and spatial working memory. In addition, lower risk adjustment correlated with higher SCI-PG scores across the whole sample, suggesting that this parameter may be particularly sensitive to the state versus trait aspects of the disorder. The prognostic value of such neurocognitive functions in gambling disorder has received limited research attention to date. In one study (n=22 cases), people with gambling disorder who relapsed over the course of one year did not differ from non-relapsers in terms of baseline neurocognitive performance (Iowa Gambling, Stroop, and Delay Discounting paradigms) (De Wilde et al., 2013). In another 1-year study, in 157 participants with subsyndromal gambling problems, baseline cognitive function (measured using the same tests used in the current study) did not differ significantly between those individuals whose symptoms remitted versus persisted (Grant et al., 2014). In a study involving 113 patients with gambling disorder assigned to psychological or psychoeducational treatment, reassessed 6-months later, improvements in decision-making (Iowa Gambling task) performance appeared related to higher chance of symptomatic recovery (Rossini-Dib et al., 2015). Lastly, in a study of 46 pathological gamblers, relapse by approximately one-year was significantly predicted by higher baseline disinhibition (Stop-Signal task), worse decision-making (Card Playing task), and longer duration of disorder; the combination of these variables explained 53% of variance in relapse (Goudriaan et al., 2008). Collectively, these studies suggest that decision-making problems, and potentially other types of cognitive impairment, may have predictive value in terms of outcomes in gambling disorder. As such, our finding that decision-making differs as a function of racial-ethnic differences in gambling disorder may well have differential prognostic implications. This needs to be studied in future work.
While the extant research has not examined race as it relates to potential cognitive markers in young adults with gambling disorder, previous research has looked at risk factors associated with cognitive dysfunction in children. In a sample of 109 children employing cognitive testing, Martell (2013) found that race-ethnicity and low income were risk factors for ADHD and executive function deficits (Martell, 2013). Research into racial-ethnic differences in older adults, however, is conflicting with one study noting ethnic differences in terms of risk-taking (or ‘delay’) tasks (Sloan & Wang, 2005) while another study noted no significant neurocognitive differences between African-American and European-American participants (Baird et al., 2007). Coupled with the current study, these studies all had significantly different age cohorts and methodologies, limiting comparisons.
Thus, we emphasize the need to consider race research relating to gambling disorder, including treatment studies. If one regards these neurocognitive markers as candidate ‘treatment targets’ for people with gambling disorder, it is likely that they could be ameliorated via different psychological and pharmacological interventions.
There are several limitations to the current study. The sample size was relatively small – however, it was sufficiently powered to detect important clinical and cognitive differences; we feel that the study also has merit in view of the lack of data in this field, and we hope it will provide a springboard for more substantive work. Because of the sample size, and since post hoc tests were protected at the level of composite multivariate analysis of variance tests, we did not correct for multiple comparisons for our primary analyses. As such, some significant findings may reflect false positives. We did not assess all socio-economic variables in the current study, although education, employment and income were examined. The white gamblers had lesser income than black gamblers, but the groups did not differ on other socio-economic measures, and income per year was controlled for in the statistical analyses for clinical and cognitive measures.
Environmental factors such living in a disadvantaged neighborhood may have influenced gambling behavior (Martins et al., 2013). The longitudinal study conducted by Martins and colleagues (2013), however, reported on a slightly younger population, including children and adolescents, and may not be directly comparable to our sample of young adult gamblers. The current study did not include a non-gambling control group; as such, it is possible (although in our view unlikely) that group differences could be due to general race-ethnic related effects, rather than different presentation of clinical features as a function of race-ethnicity in people with gambling disorder. Information on medication status was not collected, nor was intelligence quotient. Future studies, using a longitudinal design and variety of socio-demographic, clinical, and cognitive paradigms, may further our understanding of the influence of environmental and neurobiological factors in the development of gambling addiction.
These findings indicate that clinical and cognitive factors associated with gambling disorder may differ depending on race-ethnicity. Future work should address whether group differences are pre-disposing or rather a consequence of gambling disorder, an issue that cannot be addressed within the confines of the current study design. Understanding the potential differences in decision-making may aid in the development of early intervention for individuals with gambling disorder pathology. These initial data emphasize the need to further explore race-ethnicity in relation to neurobiological models for gambling.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Center of Excellence in Gambling Research grant from the National Center for Responsible Gaming to Dr. Grant. Dr. Chamberlain’s involvement in this project was supported by a grant from the Academy of Medical Sciences (AMS, United Kingdom).
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest: Dr. Chamberlain consults for Cambridge Cognition; his involvement in this research was funded by a grant from the Academy of Medical Sciences (AMS, UK). Dr. Odlaug has received research grants from the Trichotillomania Learning Center and receives royalties from Oxford University Press. Since these data were collected, Dr. Odlaug has taken a position with Lundbeck Pharmaceuticals. Dr. Grant has received research grants from NIMH, National Center for Responsible Gaming, and Forest and Roche Pharmaceuticals. He receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, and McGraw Hill. Mr. Leppink and Ms. Redden report no financial relationships with commercial interests.
Contributors
Samuel Chamberlain: Dr. Chamberlain undertook statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript.
Sarah Redden: Ms. Redden collected the data and assisted in drafting the manuscript.
Eric Leppink: Mr. Leppink collected the data and assisted in drafting the manuscript.
Brian Odlaug: Dr. Odlaug collected the data and assisted in drafting the manuscript.
Jon Grant: Dr. Grant designed the study, collected the data and drafted the manuscript.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Alegria AA, Petry NM, Hasin DS, Liu SM, Grant BF, Blanco C. Disordered gambling among racial and ethnic groups in the US: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. CNS Spectrums. 2009;14:132–142. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900020113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2004;8:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baird AD, Ford M, Podell K. Ethnic differences in functional and neuropsychological test performance in older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22:309–318. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry DT, Steinberg MA, Wu R, Potenza MN. Characteristics of black and white callers to a gambling helpline. Psychiatric Services. 2008;59:1347–1350. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.59.11.1347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry DT, Stefanovics EA, Desai RA, Potenza MN. Differences in the associations between gambling problem severity and psychiatric disorders among black and white adults: findings from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. American Journal on Addictions. 2011;20:69–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2010.00098.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cambridge Cognition Limited. CANTABeclipse. version 3. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge Cognition Limited; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain SR, Lochner C, Stein DJ, Goudriaan AE, van Holst RJ, Zohar J, Grant JE. Behavioural addiction-A rising tide? Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015 Aug 24; doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark L. Decision-making during gambling: an integration of cognitive and psychobiological approaches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:319–330. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desai RA, Maciejewski PK, Dausey DJ, Caldarone BJ, Potenza MN. Health correlates of recreational gambling in older adults. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161:1672–1679. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Primate analogue of the Wisconsin card sorting test: Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1996;110:872–886. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.5.872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Wilde B, Goudriaan A, Sabbe B, Hulstijn W, Dom G. Relapse in pathological gamblers: A pilot study on the predictive value of different impulsivity measures. J Behav Addict. 2013 Mar;2(1):23–30. doi: 10.1556/JBA.2.2013.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, Van den Brink W. Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004 Apr;28(2):123–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, De Beurs E, Van Den Brink W. The role of self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity versus neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making in the prediction of relapse in pathological gamblers. Psychol Med. 2008 Jan;38(1):41–50. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudriaan AE, Yücel M, van Holst RJ. Getting a grip on problem gambling: what can neuroscience tell us? Front Behav Neurosci. 2014 May 20;8:141. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Steinberg MA, Kim SW, Rounsaville BJ, Potenza MN. Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling (SCI-PG) Psychiatry Research. 2004;128:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Chamberlain SR, Schreiber LR, Odlaug BL, Kim SW. Selective decision-making deficits in at-risk gamblers. Psychiatry Research. 2011;189:115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Derbyshire K, Leppink E, Chamberlain SR. One-year follow-up of subsyndromal gambling disorder in non-treatment-seeking young adults. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2014 Aug;26(3):199–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgins DC, Stea JN, Grant JE. Gambling disorders. Lancet. 2011;378:1874–1884. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)62185-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, Sanislow C, Wang P. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. (4th ed.) New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:276–291. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martel MM. Individual differences in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and associated executive dysfunction and traits: sex, ethnicity, and family income. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2013;83:165–175. doi: 10.1111/ajop.12034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins SS, Storr CL, Ialongo NS, Chilcoat HD. Gender differences in mental health characteristics and gambling among African-American adolescent gamblers. American Journal on Addictions. 2008;17:126–134. doi: 10.1080/10550490701861227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins SS, Storr CL, Lee GP, Ialongo NS. Environmental influences associated with gambling in young adulthood. Journal of Urban Health. 2013;90:130–140. doi: 10.1007/s11524-012-9751-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, Kim SW, Schreiber LR, Grant JE. A neurocognitive comparison of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition in gamblers with varying degrees of clinical severity. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:2111–2119. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:1021–1034. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Semple J, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:1–24. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00098-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Muir JL. A specific form of cognitive rigidity following excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain in marmosets. Neuroscience. 1992;47:251–264. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90241-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Williams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:9029–9038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-20-09029.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossini-Dib D, Fuentes D, Tavares H. A naturalistic study of recovering gamblers: What gets better and when they get better. Psychiatry Res. 2015 May 30;227(1):17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacco P, Torres LR, Cunningham-Williams RM, Woods C, Unick GJ. Differential item functioning of pathological gambling criteria: an examination of gender, race/ethnicity, and age. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2011;27:317–330. doi: 10.1007/s10899-010-9209-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanavio E. Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1988;26:169–177. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(88)90116-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloan FA, Wang J. Disparities among older adults in measures of cognitive function by race or ethnicity. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60:P242–250. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.5.p242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Holst RJ, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE. Why gamblers fail to win: a review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews. 2010a;34:87–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Holst RJ, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE. Brain imaging studies in pathological gambling. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2010b;12:418–425. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0141-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstock J, Whelan JP, Meyers AW. Behavioral assessment of gambling: An application of the timeline followback method. Psychological Assessments. 2004;16(1):72–80. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]