Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Mar 6.
Published in final edited form as: Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2016 Nov 15;74:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2016.11.002

Neurocognitive predictors of treatment response to randomized treatment in adults with tic disorders

Amitai Abramovitch a,b,*, Lauren S Hallion b,c,d, Hannah E Reese b,e, Douglas W Woods f, Alan Peterson g, John T Walkup h, John Piacentini i, Lawrence Scahill j,k, Thilo Deckersbach b,, Sabine Wilhelm b,
PMCID: PMC5330153  NIHMSID: NIHMS833739  PMID: 27864156

Abstract

Tourette’s disorder (TS) and chronic tic disorder (CTD) are neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by involuntary vocal and motor tics. Consequently, TS/CTD have been conceptualized as disorders of cognitive and motor inhibitory control. However, most neurocognitive studies have found comparable or superior inhibitory capacity among individuals with TS/CTD relative to healthy controls. These findings have led to the hypothesis that individuals with TS/CTD develop increased inhibitory control due to the constant need to inhibit tics. However, the role of cognitive control in TS/CTD is not yet understood, particularly in adults. To examine the role of inhibitory control in TS/CTD, the present study investigated this association by assessing the relationship between inhibitory control and treatment response in a large sample of adults with TS/CTD. As part of a large randomized trial comparing behavior therapy versus supportive psychotherapy for TS/CTD, a battery of tests, including tests of inhibitory control was administered to 122 adults with TS/CTD at baseline. We assessed the association between neuropsychological test performance and change in symptom severity, as well as compared the performance of treatment responders and non-responders as defined by the Clinical Global Impression Scale. Results indicated that change in symptoms, and treatment response were not associated with neuropsychological performance on tests of inhibitory control, intellectual ability, or motor function, regardless of type of treatment. The finding that significant change in symptom severity of TS/CTD patients is not associated with impairment or change in inhibitory control regardless of treatment type suggests that inhibitory control may not be a clinically relevant facet of these disorders in adults.

1. Introduction

Tourette’s disorder (TS) and persistent (chronic) motor or vocal tic disorder (CTD), are neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by multiple vocal and motor tics (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The worldwide prevalence of tic disorders in children is estimated at 0.8%, with higher prevalence in boys (1.1%), whereas the prevalence of tic disorders in adults is estimated at 1:2000 (Knight et al., 2012). Tic disorders usually onset in childhood and their severity tend to decrease with age. However, it has been estimated that 11% of individuals with tic disorders continue to experience moderate to severe tics resulting in daily life functional impairments into adulthood (Bloch et al., 2006; Leckman et al., 1998).

Compared to controls, individuals diagnosed with tic disorders exhibit different patterns of brain activity in the cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) neural circuitry (Leckman, Bloch, Smith, Larabi, & Hampson, 2010). The prominent role of the CSTC system in executive and inhibitory functions, together with the clinical presentation of tics, led to the hypothesis that tic disorders are disorders of motor disinhibition, wherein patients experience difficulties suppressing tics (Jung, Jackson, Parkinson, & Jackson, 2013). Indeed, imaging studies reveal increased activation of the CSTC network in patients with tic disorders during attempts to inhibit eye blinking (Mazzone et al., 2010).

1.2 Inhibitory control in tic disorders

Studies investigating executive function in tic disorders, particularly tasks of inhibitory control (including response inhibition, response suppression, and interference control), reveal mixed results (Kalsi, Tambelli, Aceto, & Lai, 2015). In fact, the majority of studies utilizing the gold standard tests of inhibitory control have revealed intact performance amongst adults with tic disorders. These include research utilizing Go/No-Go tasks (GNG; Serrien, Orth, Evans, Lees, & Brown, 2005; Thomalla et al., 2014; Watkins et al., 2005) and the Stroop task (Eddy & Cavanna, 2014; Thibault, O’Connor, Stip, & Lavoie, 2009). Very few studies found underperformance on tasks of behavioral inhibition in adults with TS/CTD (Jackson, Draper, Dyke, Pépés, & Jackson, 2015), but these results were found on tasks such as Sentence Completion, or the Simon task (Dursun, Burke, & Reveley, 2000; Georgiou, Bradshaw, Phillips, Bradshaw, & Chiu, 1995). In addition, it has been argued that such studies tend to include participants with comorbid disorders such as Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and OCD, and that inhibitory deficits may be found mainly in individuals diagnosed with tic disorders concomitant with OCD and/or ADHD (Jung et al., 2013). Notably, recent reviews of the literature highlight findings indicating a paradoxical superior behavioral control among adolescents and adults diagnosed with tic disorders compared to controls—hypothetically due to years of experience attempting to inhibit tics (Jackson et al., 2015; Jung et al., 2013). However, more research is required to support this notion, and the role of behavioral inhibition in adult TS/CTD remains unclear.

1.3 Neuropsychological functions and treatment response in tic disorders

Behavioral interventions for tic disorders, such as Habit Reversal Therapy (HRT) and its newer version called Comprehensive Behavioral Intervention for Tics (CBIT), are found to be effective for the treatment of tic disorders, (Piacentini et al., 2010; Wilhelm et al., 2012) yielding large effect sizes in adult samples (McGuire et al., 2014). However, very little is known about predictors of treatment response to behavior therapy for tic disorders. Neuropsychological predictors of treatment response may be important in informing treatment selection, as well as informing treatment development. To our knowledge, there are only three studies that examined changes in cognitive function following cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for adult TS/CTD. Lavoie and colleagues (2011) reported improved performance on the Purdue Pegboard Test following treatment. However, this test assesses basic motor functions, and the authors did not include executive function or other neuropsychological tests in their study. In another study (Deckersbach, Rauch, Buhlmann, & Wilhelm, 2006) the authors compared a small sample of individuals diagnosed with TS receiving habit reversal therapy (n=15) to a sample (n=15) receiving supportive psychotherapy. The authors found that aspects of performance on a visuospatial priming task had predictive value for treatment response. Notably, the authors used this task to assess response inhibition, although the task was visuospatial in nature which poses difficulties in differentiating between the predictive value of visuospatial function versus response inhibition. In a recent study, however, Morand-Beaulieu and colleagues (2015) employed a stimulus-response compatibility inhibition task in a sample of 20 TS/CTD adult patients and 20 controls and found no performance difference between pre- and post-treatment. Thus, the goal of this study was to utilize gold standard tasks of inhibitory control—namely, the GO/No-Go (GNG) test assessing response inhibition, and the Stroop test, assessing interference control—to predict treatment response to CBIT among adults with TS and CTD. In light of the mixed literature and the novelty of this study, our investigation is exploratory.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1 Study Design

Participants were recruited as part of a large-scale, randomized controlled trial comparing 10 weeks (8 sessions) of comprehensive behavioral intervention for tics (CBIT) to psychoeducation and supportive therapy (PST). See Wilhelm et al. (2012) for a detailed description of the study procedures. Neuropsychological measures were administered at baseline. Clinical severity was assessed by an independent evaluator (a clinician blind to treatment condition) at baseline and at post-treatment.

Participants were recruited at three sites: Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Yale University, and University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio.

2.2 Participants

Adult participants (n = 122) were included in the present study. Inclusion criteria were age ≥16 years, a diagnosis of TS or CTD of moderate severity or greater based on the Clinical Global Impression-Severity Score (CGI-S ≥ 4), and a Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS; Leckman et al., 1989) total score ≥ 14 (> 10 for those with only motor or vocal tics). Additional inclusion criteria were fluency in English, IQ > 80 on a standardized intelligence test, no history of schizophrenia or pervasive developmental disorder, and no current substance use disorder. Other comorbidities (bipolar disorder, depression, anxiety and related disorders, and ADHD) were permitted provided that the co-occurring disorder was stable and not of sufficient severity to require clinical attention. Participants were excluded if they previously completed a course (> 4 sessions) of CBT for tics. Medications for tics were permitted provided the dose was stable for at least 6 weeks with no planned changes for the duration of the study. Fifty-one participants (41.8%) were medicated—out of which 8 participants were on tic medication only (e.g., alpha agonists, neuroleptics), 23 were on other medications in addition to tic medication and 20 participants were on non-tic medication only (e.g., SSRIs).

2.3 Treatments

A comprehensive description of the study treatments can be found elsewhere (Wilhelm et al., 2012). Briefly, both treatments consisted of eight 60–90 minute sessions administered over 10 weeks. CBIT comprised psychoeducation, tic awareness training, competing response training, relaxation training, and functional analysis. PST comprised disorder-specific psychoeducation and supportive therapy. Therapists had at least a master’s degree in clinical psychology, followed detailed treatment manuals, and were specifically trained on both treatments for this study. Treatment sessions were videotaped and randomly selected for fidelity ratings. Fidelity was good or better for 75.7% of CBIT tapes and 87.7% of PST tapes.

2.4 Measures

2.4.1 Clinical measures

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Patient Version (SCID-P)

Diagnostic status was assessed via the SCID-P (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002), a widely-used and well-validated semi-structured interview developed to establish past and current DSM-IV diagnoses.

Clinical Global Impression-Improvement scale (CGI-I)

The CGI-I (Guy & Bonato, 1970) is a single-item standard global assessment used to assess changes in severity of the target disorder. The CGI-I scores range between 1 (very much improved) to 7 (very much worse). Positive response to treatment in the present study was defined as a score of 2 or 1 (much improved, or very much improved). The CGI-I score in each time point was assessed by an evaluator that was blind to treatment assignment.

Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS)

The YGTSS (Leckman et al., 1989) is a clinician-rated scale used to assess tic severity and was the primary clinical outcome measure in this study. Motor and phonic tics are rated separately from 0 to 5 on several scales including: number, frequency, intensity, complexity, and interference. Thus, Motor and Phonic Tic scores can range from 0 to 25; the combined Total Tic score ranges from 0 to 50. There is also an Impairment score that rates the overall burden due to tics. The Impairment scale yields a single score from 0 to 50 with higher scores indicating higher levels of overall impairment associated with tics. The YGTSS has demonstrated strong psychometric properties with good internal consistency, excellent inter-rater reliability, and strong convergent and divergent validity (Leckman et al., 1989). The primary outcome measure in this study was the Total Tic score, as it shows the greatest sensitivity to change in tic severity over brief periods of time (Lin et al., 2002).

2.4.2 Neuropsychological Tests

2.4.2.1 General Tests
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR)

The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001) provides an estimate of premorbid IQ. The WTAR shows a strong (r > .7) correlation with IQ as assessed by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Subjects are to read a list of words and receive a point for each correctly pronounced word.

Purdue Pegboard Test

The Purdue Pegboard Test (Tiffin & Asher, 1948) measures manual dexterity. The task consists of a board with holes and a set of pegs. Subjects are instructed to place the pegs in the holes (one at a time) as quickly and correctly as possible. Outcomes can be presented separately for left, right, and both hands. For the purposes of the present paper, we report only the results for both hands. Tiffin & Asher (1948) provide a means to calculate scaled scores; however, the normative samples are comprised of adults who applied for factory and production jobs and therefore may not be representative of the general population. We therefore use raw scores in analyses and present descriptive data from a large general sample of Canadian adults (Yeudall, Fromm, Reddon, & Stefanyk, 1986) for comparison.

2.4.2.2 Inhibitory control tasks
Stroop Color & Word Test

The Stroop task is a classic cognitive interference task (Stroop, 1935). It assesses inhibition of a dominant response (reading) in favor of an alternative competing response (color naming). In the first condition, participants are presented with color words (written in black) and are asked to name the color as quickly as possible. In the second condition, subjects are asked to name the color of a nonsense stimulus (XXXXX) as quickly as possible. In the Interference condition, participants are presented with color words (e.g. red) written in a color that is inconsistent with the word’s meaning (e.g. red written in blue). Participants are required to name the color the word is printed in as quickly as possible. An interference (color-word) score was calculated as a standardized score based on age and education-corrected normative data.

Go/No-Go Task (GNG)

The GNG task (Serrien et al., 2005) is a computerized measure of motor response inhibition. In the task, participants view a cue stimulus (an arrow pointing to the right or left) that signaled them to get ready, followed by a target stimulus (the figure 0 or the letter S) that indicated the correct response. Participants were seated in front of a desk with a custom-built button-press device held in each hand and faced a computer screen at a distance of 80 cm. A fixation cross was visible continuously in the center of the screen. During each trial, an arrow was presented that pointed to the right or to the left and served as a cue for a following target. On “go” trials (75% of trials), the target stimulus was the figure 0. On “No-Go” (response inhibition) trials (25% of trials), the target signal was the letter S, and indicated that the planned response needed to be withheld (inhibited).

The cue and target stimuli appeared on the right or left side of the fixation cross to enhance stimulus-response compatibility and remained visible on the screen for 500 ms; a fixed time interval of 3 s occurred between the onsets of cue and target. Participants responded to the target signal by pressing the right-sided or left-sided button as fast as possible with the thumb of the right or left hand (as instructed by the direction of the cue arrow and the asymmetry of the target signal with respect to the central fixation cross on screen). Commission errors were calculated as the percent of false alarms (incorrect positive responses) divided by the total number of No-Go trials (20 trials). Omission errors were calculated as the percent of misses (incorrect negative responses) divided by the total number of go trials (60 trials).

2.5 Statistical analyses

Clinical improvement was computed as the change in YGTSS total score from baseline to post-treatment (week 10). Each neuropsychological measure was examined in a separate regression model as a predictor of treatment outcome with treatment condition (CBIT vs. PST) included as a moderator. These analyses were conducted using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2013). Multivariate analysis of variance was conducted in order to assess performance differences between treatment responders and non-responders on neuropsychological tasks.

3. Results

Participants were 122 adults (M age = 31.55, SD = 13.72; 36% female) with an average total tic severity of 22.91 (SD = 6.60) as assessed by the YGTSS. Participants were generally of average intelligence (WTAR FSIQ M = 106.02, SD = 9.56). Motor functioning was somewhat deficient: The average Purdue Pegboard performance for both hands was approximately one standard deviation below the mean reported in a large normative adult sample (M = 11.32, SD = 2.02 in this sample, versus norms M = 12.69, SD = 1.55; Yeudall et al., 1986), and was equivalent to scores obtained in other adult CTD and TS samples (e.g., Lavoie, Thibault, Stip, & O’Connor, 2007).

3.1 Neuropsychological predictors of treatment response

3.1.1 General measures

No significant association was found between neuropsychological test performance and change in symptoms (i.e., pre – post total YGTSS score) for any of the tests administered, nor did performance interact with treatment condition to predict improvement. FSIQ did not significantly predict change in symptoms (B = −0.09, p = .24), nor did FSIQ interact with treatment condition to predict change (B = −0.08, p = .47). Purdue Pegboard Task performance similarly failed to predict change in symptoms (B = −0.17, p = .62), nor did performance interact with treatment condition (B = −0.50, p = .32). No significant differences were found on these two tasks between treatment responders and non-responders, and no significant interaction effect was found between the two treatment conditions (Table 3). Medication status added to the regression and MANOVA models did not alter results.

Table 3.

Neuropsychological Test Performance as a Function of Treatment Response and Treatment Type

Responders Non-responders Group Comparison Interaction Tx Type

M SD M SD F η2 F
WTAR FSIQ 106.04 8.85 107.5 9.13 .44 .01 .12
Purdue Pegboard 11.00 2.08 11.61 11.61 3.89 .05 .75
Stroop Word T score 44.69 12.99 47.40 12.66 .41 .01 .42
Stroop Color T score 43.81 11.53 46.74 10.89 1.30 .02 .11
Stroop Interference T score 52.12 5.69 52.37 9.39 .37 .01 .45
Go/NoGo Commissions (#) 2.50 2.73 2.43 3.45 .58 .02 1.74
Go/NoGo Omissions (#) 1.08 1.60 2.44 5.31 1.75 .05 .19

Note. WTAR = Wechsler Adult Test for Reading. FSIQ = Full Scale IQ. η2 = unadjusted Eta squared. Interaction Tx Type = difference between responders and non-responders as a function of treatment type.

#

= raw number of hits.

Comparison sample sizes: WTAR - 75, 27; Purdue – 76, 27; Stroop – 77, 27; Go/NoGo – 73, 26.

3.1.2 Inhibitory control tasks

Stroop interference did not predict change in symptoms (B = 0.02, p = .86) or interact with treatment condition to predict change (B = 0.10, p = .44). Similarly, GNG commission errors did not predict change in YGTSS (B = −0.01, p = 0.85) or interact with treatment condition to predict change (B = 0.02, p − .76). GNG omission errors did not predict change in YGTSS (B = −0.02, p = .66) or interact with treatment condition to predict change (B = 0.03, p = .79). Finally, no significant differences were found on inhibitory tasks between treatment responders and non-responders, and no significant interaction effect was found between the two treatment conditions (Table 3). Medication status added to the regression and MANOVA models did not alter results.

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first large scale study to investigate the predictive validity of neuropsychological tests of response inhibition on treatment response for tic disorders in adults. Our results show no significant association between performance on the Stroop or GNG tests with treatment response regardless of the type (i.e., CBIT and PST). Moreover, no association was found between general intellectual ability or psychomotor functioning and treatment response. These null findings were evident when assessed using change in symptoms severity as a continuous dependent variable, as well when assessed using the binary CGI-I criterion. Two previous studies examined the association between neuropsychological test performance and treatment response in TS/CTD samples. In contrast to our findings, Lavoie and colleagues (2011) reported improved performance on the Purdue Pegboard test following CBT in a sample of TS patients. However, the sample size in their study was very small (n = 10). Similarly, Deckersbach and colleagues (2006) found some predictive validity of a Visuospatial Priming Task for treatment response in participants with TS. However, the study had a small sample, and it utilized a visuospatial response inhibition task that is heavily influenced by visuospatial abilities that may mask inhibitory function. In contrast, our results are in accord with a more recent study that reported no association between treatment repsonse and performance on a stimulus-response compatibility inhibition task in a sample of 20 TS/CTD patients (Morand-Beaulieu et al., 2015).

The unique clinical presentation of patients with tic disorders could be perceived as a control deficit, and one might assume that these individuals suffer from substantial cognitive deficits, particularly in executive function such as inhibitory control. The results of the relatively small body of research are inconsistent, however. In fact, the majority of studies utilizing well-validated measures of executive function in TS/CTD samples indicated intact performance compared to controls (e.g., Eddy & Cavanna, 2014; Serrien et al., 2005; Thibault et al., 2009; Thomalla et al., 2014). Indeed, recent reviews of inhibitory control in TS conclude that there is no convincing evidence of deficits in inhibitory control (or executive functions) in TS/CTD (Jackson et al., 2015; Jung et al., 2013). In light of studies exhibiting enhanced motor inhibition in TS/CTD, it has been suggested that —This finding is consistent with the proposal that the frequent need to actively suppress tics leads to a generalised enhancement in the efficacy of volitional control mechanisms in TS that extends to laboratory tasks of cognitive control of motor output.” (Jung et al., 2013, p. 1017).

It should be noted, however, that it has been suggested that prevalent comorbid conditions such as ADHD act as protective factors against meaningful cognitive deficits in individuals diagnosed with TS (for a review see Kalsi et al., 2015). However, this notion—that has been based on one study assessing pediatric samples (Sukhodolsky, Landeros-Weisenberger, Scahill, Leckman, & Schultz, 2010), is theoretically problematic given the strong association between ADHD and cognitive deficits, and due to the need for more research directly examining this hypothesis, particularly in adults. Although more research is needed, if executive functions are indeed intact among individuals diagnosed with TS/CTD, while tic severity can be substantial and even impairing, it would be reasonable to assume that these cognitive functions would be ineffective predictors of treatment response. In fact, our results of lack of an association between change in the core symptoms of TS/CTD and baseline response inhibition lends support to the notion that tics are not a direct result of failure of motor nor cognitive inhibition. It is important to note, however, that research into neuropsychological performance as predictors of treatment response in other disorders where neuropsychological deficits are more pronounced is inconsistent, and overall neurocognitive functions do not appear to be a promising domain in terms of predictions of treatment response (e.g., Braga et al., 2016; Douglas, Porter, Knight, & Maruff, 2011; Granholm et al., 2008; Moritz et al., 2005; Wild & Gur, 2008).

The current study has several strengths. First, to our knowledge, this study is the largest to date to provide neuropsychological data on adults with TS/CTD. Second, the present sample underwent a rigorous screening procedure, and was provided with a disorder-specific evidence based treatment. Third, examination of the association between neuropsychological test performance and treatment response, may provide more meaningful insight compared to a cross-sectional study. Nevertheless, the present study is not without limitations. First, the present study did not include a non-clinical control sample that hinders direct conclusions concerning an objective deficit on neuropsychological tasks. However, the WTAR and the Stroop tests produced scaled scores indicating performance on the normative range, and participants’ performance on the Purdue Pegboard was found to be one standard deviation below the norms and similar to performance scores reported in other TS/CTD studies. In addition, the present study focused on specific executive function indices tapping response inhibition (GNG) and interference control (Stroop), and no other executive function, for which results may be theoretically different.

5. Conclusion

In the largest sample to date to assess neuropsychological predictors of treatment response in adult individuals diagnosed with TS/CTD, we found no association between neurocognitive functioning and treatment response, regardless of treatment type. These results were similar when treatment response was assessed as change in symptom severity, and when assessed using the definition of treatment response according the CGI-I. In addition, scaled scores derived from test means indirectly indicated that participants largely performed in the normative range. These results support recent research suggesting that inhibitory control deficits do not play a central role in TS/CTDs psychopathology. However, neuropsychological investigations in adult TS/CTD are limited, and more research is needed in order to provide clearer insight into neurocognitive functioning in these disorders.

Table 1.

General Neuropsychological Functioning as a Predictor of Treatment Response

B SE (B) t p 95% CI R-sq F
WTAR FSIQ .18 6.95***
 Condition 12.66 12.10 1.05 .30 −11.34 – 36.67
 WTAR FSIQ −0.09 0.07 −1.18 .24 −0.24 – 0.06
 Condition × FSIQ −0.08 0.11 −0.73 .47 −0.31 – 0.14
Purdue Pegboard Test .17 6.54***
 Condition 9.66 5.72 1.68 .09 −1.69 – 21.01
 Both Hands −0.17 0.34 −0.50 .62 −0.85 – 0.51
 Condition × Both Hands interaction −0.50 0.50 −1.01 .32 −1.48 – 0.49

Note. WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. FSIQ = Full Scale IQ. WTAR analysis n=102. Purdue analysis n=103.

***

p <.001.

Table 2.

Inhibition as a Predictor of Treatment Outcome

B SE (B) t p 95% CI R2 F
Stroop Task
 Stroop Word Score 0.14 5.38***
  Condition 2.53 3.90 0.65 .52 −5.21 – 10.28
  Word t-score −0.06 0.06 −1.03 .31 −0.18 – 0.06
  Condition × word t-score 0.03 0.08 0.33 .74 −0.13 – 0.19
 Stroop Color Score 0.15 5.83***
  Condition 4.09 4.36 0.94 .35 −4.55 – 12.74
  Color t-score −0.07 0.07 −1.06 .29 −0.21 – 0.06
  Condition × color t-score −0.01 0.09 −0.07 .95 −0.19 – 0.18
 Stroop Interference .14 5.52***
  Condition 8.88 6.47 1.37 .17 −3.95 – 21.72
  Interference t-score −0.02 0.09 −0.18 .86 −0.19 – 0.16
  Condition × interference −0.10 0.12 −0.78 .44 −0.34 – 0.15
Go/NoGo Task
 Commission errors 0.14 5.07***
  Condition 4.20 1.29 3.27*** .00 1.65 – 6.75
  Commission errors 0.01 0.04 0.19 .85 −0.07 – 0.09
  Condition × commission errors −0.02 0.06 −0.30 .76 −0.14 – 0.11
 Omission errors 0.14 5.11***
  Condition 4.08 1.10 3.71*** .00 1.90 – 6.26
  Omission errors 0.02 0.05 0.45 .66 −0.08 – 0.12
  Condition × omission errors −0.03 0.10 −0.26 .79 −0.21 – 0.16

Note.

***

p <.001.

Stroop analyses n=104. Go/NoGo analyses n=100.

Highlights.

  • The role of inhibitory functions in adult TS/CTD is unclear

  • Adolescents and adults with TS/CTD may possess intact or superior inhibitory control

  • We examined the association between baseline inhibitory function and treatment response in adults TS/CTD

  • Neuropsychological tests did not predict treatment response regardless of treatment type

  • Results support the notion that inhibitory deficits are not core facets of TS/CTD in adults

Acknowledgments

Funding: This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to Drs. Wilhelm (5R01MH069877), Scahill (R01MH069874), and Peterson (RO1MH069875) with subcontracts to Drs. Piacentini and Woods.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5. Arlington, Va: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bloch MH, Peterson BS, Scahill L, Otka J, Katsovich L, Zhang H, Leckman JF. Adulthood outcome of tic and obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in children with Tourette syndrome. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2006;160(1):65–69. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Braga DT, Abramovitch A, Fontenelle LF, Ferrao YA, Gomes JB, Vivan AS, … Cordioli AV. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PREDICTORS OF TREATMENT RESPONSE TO COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL GROUP THERAPY IN OBSESSIVE-COMPULSIVE DISORDER. Depress Anxiety. 2016 doi: 10.1002/da.22509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Deckersbach T, Rauch S, Buhlmann U, Wilhelm S. Habit reversal versus supportive psychotherapy in Tourette’s disorder: A randomized controlled trial and predictors of treatment response. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(8):1079–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Douglas KM, Porter RJ, Knight RG, Maruff P. Neuropsychological changes and treatment response in severe depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198(2):115–122. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Dursun SM, Burke JG, Reveley MA. Antisaccade eye movement abnormalities in Tourette syndrome: evidence for cortico-striatal network dysfunction? J Psychopharmacol. 2000;14(1):37–39. doi: 10.1177/026988110001400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE. Set-Shifting Deficits: A Possible Neurocognitive Endophenotype for Tourette Syndrome Without ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2014 doi: 10.1177/1087054714545536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. New York: Biometrics Research; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  9. Georgiou N, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG, Bradshaw JA, Chiu E. The Simon effect and attention deficits in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome and Huntington’s disease. Brain. 1995;118(Pt 5):1305–1318. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.5.1305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Granholm E, McQuaid JR, Link PC, Fish S, Patterson T, Jeste DV. Neuropsychological predictors of functional outcome in Cognitive Behavioral Social Skills Training for older people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;100(1–3):133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Guy W, Bonato RR. CGI: Clinical Global Impression. Chavy Chase, MD: National Institute of Mental Health; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  12. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  13. Jackson GM, Draper A, Dyke K, Pépés SE, Jackson SR. Inhibition, Disinhibition, and the Control of Action in Tourette Syndrome. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2015;19(11):655–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Jung J, Jackson SR, Parkinson A, Jackson GM. Cognitive control over motor output in Tourette syndrome. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37(6):1016–1025. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kalsi N, Tambelli R, Aceto P, Lai C. Are Motor Skills and Motor Inhibitions Impaired in Tourette Syndrome? A Review. Journal of Experimental Neuroscience. 2015;9:57–65. doi: 10.4137/JEN.S25095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Knight T, Steeves T, Day L, Lowerison M, Jette N, Pringsheim T. Prevalence of Tic Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Pediatric Neurology. 2012;47(2):77–90. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.05.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lavoie ME, Imbriglio TV, Stip E, O’Connor KP. Neurocognitive Changes Following Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment in Tourette Syndrome and Chronic Tic Disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy. 2011;4(1):34–50. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2011.4.1.34. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lavoie ME, Thibault G, Stip E, O’Connor KP. Memory and executive functions in adults with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12(2):165–181. doi: 10.1080/13546800600826371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, Smith ME, Larabi D, Hampson M. Neurobiological substrates of Tourette’s disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2010;20(4):237–247. doi: 10.1089/cap.2009.0118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, Ort SI, Swartz KL, Stevenson J, Cohen DJ. The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28(4):566–573. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198907000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Leckman JF, Zhang H, Vitale A, Lahnin F, Lynch K, Bondi C, … Peterson BS. Course of Tic Severity in Tourette Syndrome: The First Two Decades. Pediatrics. 1998;102(1):14–19. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lin H, Yeh CB, Peterson BS, Scahill L, Grantz H, Findley DB, … Leckman JF. Assessment of symptom exacerbations in a longitudinal study of children with Tourette’s syndrome or obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(9):1070–1077. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200209000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mazzone L, Yu S, Blair C, Gunter BC, Wang Z, Marsh R, Peterson BS. An FMRI study of frontostriatal circuits during the inhibition of eye blinking in persons with Tourette syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(3):341–349. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08121831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McGuire JF, Piacentini J, Brennan EA, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, Small BJ, Storch EA. A meta-analysis of behavior therapy for Tourette Syndrome. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2014;50:106–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Morand-Beaulieu S, O’Connor KP, Sauve G, Blanchet PJ, Lavoie ME. Cognitive-behavioral therapy induces sensorimotor and specific electrocortical changes in chronic tic and Tourette’s disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2015;79(Pt B):310–321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Moritz S, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Fricke S, Cutler C, Brassen S, Hand I. Neurocognitive impairment does not predict treatment outcome in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43(6):811–819. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Piacentini J, Woods DW, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Chang S, … Walkup JT. Behavior therapy for children with Tourette disorder: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2010;303(19):1929–1937. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Serrien DJ, Orth M, Evans AH, Lees AJ, Brown P. Motor inhibition in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: functional activation patterns as revealed by EEG coherence. Brain. 2005;128(Pt 1):116–125. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Strauss S, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  30. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  31. Sukhodolsky DG, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Scahill L, Leckman JF, Schultz RT. Neuropsychological functioning in children with Tourette syndrome with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(11):1155–1164. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Thibault G, O’Connor KP, Stip E, Lavoie ME. Electrophysiological manifestations of stimulus evaluation, response inhibition and motor processing in Tourette syndrome patients. Psychiatry Research. 2009;167(3):202–220. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.03.021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Thomalla G, Jonas M, Baumer T, Siebner HR, Biermann-Ruben K, Ganos C, … Munchau A. Costs of control: decreased motor cortex engagement during a Go/NoGo task in Tourette’s syndrome. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 1):122–136. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Tiffin J, Asher EJ. The Purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. J Appl Psychol. 1948;32(3):234–247. doi: 10.1037/h0061266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Watkins LH, Sahakian BJ, Robertson MM, Veale DM, Rogers RD, Pickard KM, … Robbins TW. Executive function in Tourette’s syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 2005;35(4):571–582. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Wechsler D. WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  37. Wild J, Gur RC. Verbal memory and treatment response in post-traumatic stress disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(3):254–255. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.045922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Piacentini J, Woods DW, Deckersbach T, Sukhodolsky DG, … Scahill L. Randomized trial of behavior therapy for adults with Tourette syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(8):795–803. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Yeudall LT, Fromm D, Reddon JR, Stefanyk WO. Normative data stratified by age and sex for 12 neuropsychological tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1986;42(6):918–946. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198611)42:6&#x0003c;918::AID-JCLP2270420617&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES