Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 28.
Published in final edited form as: CNS Spectr. 2016 Sep 29;22(1):22–30. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000875

ADHD symptoms in non-treatment seeking young adults: relationship with other forms of impulsivity

Samuel R Chamberlain 1,2, Konstantinos Ioannidis 1,2, Eric W Leppink 3, Faiza Niaz 1,2, Sarah A Redden 3, Jon E Grant 3
PMCID: PMC5330410  EMSID: EMS71674  PMID: 27680974

Abstract

Objective

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has been associated with various manifestations of impulsivity in adults, including elevated rates of other impulsive disorders, substance use, questionnaire-based impulsivity scores, and inhibitory dysregulation on neurocognitive tests. The relationship between ADHD and all these other forms of impulsivity have yet to be explored within the context of a single comprehensive study.

Methods

423 young adults, who gambled ≥5 times in the preceding year, were recruited using media advertisements and undertook detailed assessment including structured psychiatric interview, questionnaires, and neurocognitive tests. Participants with ADHD symptoms were identified using the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale Screener (ASRS-V1.1) and were compared to controls using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).

Results

ADHD symptoms were found in 20.3% of the sample, but only 7.3% of these subjects had ever received a formal diagnosis. ADHD symptoms were associated with significantly lower quality of life, lower self-esteem, higher emotional dysregulation, higher impulsivity-compulsivity questionnaire scores, more problematic internet use, greater occurrence of psychiatric disorders, and impaired stop-signal reaction times. Of these variables, stop-signal reaction times and Barratt attentional impulsiveness were the strongest predictors of group classification.

Conclusions

ADHD symptoms are common and under-diagnosed in young adults who gamble, and are most strongly linked with certain other types of impulsivity (questionnaire- and cognitive-based measures) and with emotional dysregulation, suggesting that these are each important considerations in understanding the pathophysiology of the disorder, but also potential treatment targets. It is necessary to question whether treatment for adult ADHD could be enhanced by considering self-esteem, emotional reactivity, and impaired inhibitory control as specific treatment targets, in addition to the core diagnostic symptoms of the disorder.

Keywords: impulsivity, ADHD, cognition, gambling, inhibition

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neuropsychiatric disorder characterized by impulsivity-hyperactivity and/or inattention, and marked functional impairment.1 ADHD is the most common psychiatric condition in children, and persists into adulthood in approximately 60% of cases.2 Adult ADHD is associated with a variety of untoward consequences including unemployment, criminality, interpersonal problems, and driving accidents.24 The prevalence of ADHD in adults globally is estimated to be 3.4%, with some variability across geographical locations (range 1.2-7.3%),5 but considerably higher rates have been reported in young adults. For example, one study in university students reported ADHD symptoms in 21.8% of the sample.6

ADHD is considered by many to represent an archetypal disorder of impulsivity as impulsive behaviors are central to the diagnostic criteria, and in terms of understanding the disorder’s pathophysiology.7 The term impulsivity refers broadly to behaviors or acts that are inappropriate, premature, and that often result in un-desirous outcomes.810 Thus, an individual with ADHD may be more prone to interrupting others or undertaking reckless acts without due consideration of the consequences. Impulsivity can also manifest in other ways, such as predisposition towards substance misuse (or higher frequency of substance use), co-occurrence with other disorders of impulsivity e.g. impulse control disorders, impulsivity on self-report questionnaires, and on neurocognitive tests. Despite its relative importance in ADHD phenomenology, only three out of 18 diagnostic symptoms are explicitly directed in assessing impulsivity in the current diagnostic and classification systems and the full-blown disorder can be present despite the absence of all these three symptoms, both in children and adults.1

Studies conducted in adults with ADHD have found higher occurrence of substance use disorders (including alcohol) compared to control samples.2,11 In a meta-analysis, one in four patients with a substance use disorder met criteria for comorbid ADHD.12 Interestingly, some data indicate that cocaine use is associated with a lower rate of ADHD as compared to other drugs of abuse.13

In addition to substance use disorders, ADHD may also show co-morbid overlap with other disorders relating to impulsivity.14 Adolescents screening positive for ADHD had higher rates of gambling behavior and problem gambling,15 and similar findings were reported in young adults longitudinally.16 In a sample of adults with pathological/problem gambling, 1 in 4 reported a history of ADHD.17 Internet addiction also appears to be strongly associated with ADHD.18 In a meta-analysis, compulsive internet use was associated with elevated risk of co-morbid ADHD, with an odds ratio of 2.9.19 In a survey of sexual behaviors and their consequences, elevated rates of ADHD were found in subjects with compulsive sexual behavior, as compared to those without.20 ADHD has also been linked with eating disorders. One large scale study reported that 5.7% of people with a lifetime diagnosis of ADHD had a lifetime diagnosis of bulimia nervosa, and the figure for binge-eating disorder was higher still at 9.3%.21 It has been postulated that binge-eating may represent a mediating ‘missing link’ between co-occurring ADHD and obesity.22,23

Considerable research has been conducted in ADHD in relation to impulsivity as measured using self-report questionnaires and neuropsychological tasks. Of those questionnaires developed in order to quantify maladaptive impulsivity, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) is one of the most widely used.24, 25 Compared to controls, adults with ADHD typically show heightened scores on all three subscales of the Barratt Impulsivity Scale.26 Similarly, while various cognitive tasks have been developed to explore impulsivity, the stop-signal task is one of the most widely used.27, 28, 29 Multiple meta-analyses confirm the existence of stop-signal inhibitory control deficits in patients with ADHD as compared to controls, albeit the majority of data are from clinical settings.3032 Studies in healthy controls indicate that Barratt impulsiveness and stop-signal impulsiveness are partially overlapping constructs..33,34

Adults with ADHD frequently experience difficulties in regulating their emotions35. Emotional dysregulation contributes to functional impairment in ADHD above and beyond impairment attributable to the core classic symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention.36 Data from a study in ADHD sibling pairs indicated that ADHD plus emotional dysregulation may represent a familial subtype of the disorder.37 In one adults study, 55% of ADHD patients showed emotional dysregulation that was of greater severity than >95% of control subjects.38, 39

Much of the above research investigating relationships between ADHD and other aspects of impulsivity has tended to focus on restricted aspects, rather than comprehensively examining a broad range of impulsive variables, using validated instruments, within the confines of a single study. This has made it difficult to ascertain those measures of impulsivity most strongly related with ADHD, and the inter-relationships between different types of impulsivity in this disorder.

Therefore, we quantified multiple types of impulsivity in young adults with ADHD symptoms recruited from the community, compared to controls recruited from the same source using the same methodology. All participants gambled five times or more in the preceding year, hence this was an enriched sample for studying impulsivity. We hypothesized that ADHD symptoms would be associated with higher rates of impulse control disorders, substance use (alcohol and nicotine), problem gambling, problem internet use, heightened questionnaire-based impulsivity scores, emotional dysregulation, and impaired response inhibition. We further predicted that heightened questionnaire-based impulsivity and cognitive impulsivity (stop-signal performance) would be particularly strongly related to ADHD symptoms.

Methods

Subjects

423 non-treatment-seeking young adults aged 18-29 years were recruited from the general community using media advertisements. The only inclusion criterion was that participants had gambled at least five times in the past year (i.e. proxy for some level of baseline impulsive behavior); this nominal inclusion criterion was used as the funding for this study was from the National Center for Responsible Gaming, and the research was conducted as part of a broader program focusing on gambling behaviors. Gambling was defined very broadly, as an episode of putting something of value at risk in the hope of obtaining reward (adapted from72). Large-scale US studies suggest that around 70-80% of young adults gamble at least once in a given year73,74. According to the University of Calgary Addiction Behaviors Laboratory (Canada), 51% of males and 54% of females gamble more than 5 times per year (www.addiction.ucalgary.ca). Thus, our threshold of gambling 5 or more times per year is likely to reasonably representative of most adults, but with some degree of sample enrichment.

Exclusion criteria were an inability to understand/undertake the procedures and an inability to provide written informed consent (or refusal of consent).

Individuals with ADHD symptoms were identified using the six-item Adult ADHD Self Report Scale (ASRS v1.1) developed by the World Health Organization.2,40 The ASRS six-item screen was developed for community-based studies and exhibits strong concordance with clinician diagnoses as well as sound psychometric properties. For each item, the individual rates the frequency of a given difficulty or behavior (e.g. difficulty wrapping up the final details of a project) on a scale of 0 (never) to 4 (very often) based on their experiences over the preceding 6 months. Each response of sometimes or greater (2 or more) on screening items 1-3 equates to one point; each response of often or greater (3 or more) on screening items 4-6 results in a point. A total score of four or more indicates that ADHD is highly likely. We therefore used this recommended definition to identify probable ADHD cases in our sample. Previous data suggest that this approach has a sensitivity of 68.7% and specificity of 99.5% for detection of true ADHD cases2.

Individuals with psychiatric and substance use comorbidity, as well as those taking psychotropic medications, were allowed to participate since we wished this to be a naturalistic sample.

The Institutional Review Board of the University of Chicago approved the study, and all procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. After an explanation of procedures, participants provided written informed consent, and were compensated with a $50 gift card for a local department store after participation.

Clinical and Cognitive Assessments

Psychiatric assessment included the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview41 to identify major psychiatric disorders, the ASRS (described above) to identify ADHD symptoms, and the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview42 to screen for impulse control disorders (gambling disorder, trichotillomania, kleptomania, pyromania, intermittent explosive disorder, compulsive buying disorder, compulsive sexual behavior, skin picking disorder, and binge eating disorder). As part of the interview, participants were asked about the number of alcoholic beverages they consumed per week, and the frequency of cigarette smoking (packs per day equivalent). Problem gambling behaviors were quantified using the Structured Clinical Interview for Gambling Disorder (SCI-GD; adapted from Grant et al., 2004 to consider DSM-5 gambling disorder)43 and problem internet behaviors with Young’s Internet Addiction Test.44

The Barratt Impulsivity Scale, Version 11 (BIS-11),25 was used to measure personality facets relating to impulsivity: attentional impulsivity (inability to concentrate attention), motor impulsivity (acting without thinking), and non-planning impulsivity (being present in the moment, lack of future thinking). Quality of life was indexed using the Quality of Life Inventory (QOLI),45 and self-esteem using the Rosenberg Self Esteem questionnaire (RSE).46 We also included the Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale (DERS)47 because problems regulating emotion are commonly implicated in the pathophysiology of ADHD.48

Neurocognitive testing was undertaken using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB),49 in a quiet testing room supervised by a trained assessor. We focused on response inhibition, working memory, and set-shifting, these being commonly affected cognitive domains across clinical ADHD studies.8,50

Response inhibition was measured with the Stop-Signal Task.51 Participants observed a series of directional arrows appearing one per time on-screen, and made quick motor responses depending on the direction of each arrow (left button was pressed for a left arrow, and vice versa). On a minority of the trials, an auditory stop-signal occurred (‘beep’), which indicated to the participant that they should attempt to suppress their button press response for the given trial. By using a tracking algorithm the task estimated the ‘stop-signal reaction time’ for each subject, which is a measure of the average time taken to suppress a response that would normally be undertaken (longer values equated to worse inhibition control). Median reaction times for ‘go’ trials were also recorded (longer values indicate relative psychomotor slowing).

On the Spatial Working Memory task,52 participants attempted to locate tokens hidden underneath boxes on-screen whilst avoiding returning to boxes that previously yielded such tokens. The key outcome measure was the total number of errors made on the task, with higher scores equating to worse memory performance.

On the set-shifting task, for each trial two pictorial stimuli were displayed on-screen, and volunteers attempted to work out an underlying rule about which stimulus was ‘correct’. After making a choice, the computer provided feedback (‘correct’ or ‘incorrect’). Once the individual had learnt the underlying rule governing the correct stimulus, the rule was changed by the computer. The key outcome measure on the task was the number of errors made at the crucial extra-dimensional set-shifting stage, in which it was necessary to shift attention to a previously irrelevant stimulus dimension.

Data analysis and power estimate

Salient demographic, clinical, and cognitive variables were tabulated for the two groups: ADHD symptom group and controls. We first considered whether these variables differed significantly between the groups overall using multivariate analysis of variance test (mANOVA). If this global test was significant, individual results were explored using follow-up ANOVA tests (or equivalent non-parametric tests were used as appropriate, where indicated in the text). Where significant group differences were found, effect sizes were reported (Cohen’s D, or equivalent non-parametric statistic as indicated in the text)

Binary logistic regression was used to identify significant independent predictors of group classification (method ‘enter’, probability for stepwise entry p=0.05, removal p=0.10, maximum iterations 20).

In secondary analysis, possible influences of gender on variables that differed significantly between ADHD symptom group and controls were examined using t-tests (for each group, and in all subjects pooled). Statistical significance was defined as p<0.05 two-tailed, uncorrected.

Our original target sample size of 400 participants was based on the assumption of approximately 20% prevalence of ADHD symptoms in the general community; this would have yielded group sizes of 80 and 320 respectively. We calculated that such group sizes would yield >95% power to detect a statistically significant difference between groups on each measure of interest, assuming medium effect size (Cohen’s D of 0.5 or greater), and alpha=0.05 uncorrected, two-tailed.

Results

Demographic, clinical, and cognitive measures of the two groups are presented in Table 1. The composite MANOVA test was significant (p<0.001) indicating that the two groups differed significantly on these variables overall; post-hoc ANOVA tests are displayed. ADHD symptoms were identified in 20.3% of the total sample, and only 7 individuals (8.3%) within the ADHD symptom group had ever received a diagnosis of ADHD.

Table 1. Comparison of the Study Groups on Demographic and Clinical Variables.

Variable ADHD symptoms (N=86) Controls (N=337) p-value Effect Size
Age, years 22.3 (3.7) 22.3 (3.6) 0.942
Gender, male, N [%] 54 [62.8%] 213 [63.2%] 0.920c
Education Score # 3.3 (0.7) 3.3 (0.9) 0.662
Quality of life t-score 42.7 (11.7) 46.9 (11.7) 0.003 -0.36 **
Problem gambling score (SCI-SG) 1.5 (2.0) 1.2 (1.8) 0.181
Internet addiction score (IAT) 2.9 (2.0) 2.2 (2.1) 0.031 0.34 *
Alcohol consumption, drinks per week 1.5 (1.2) 1.3 (1.4) 0.360
Nicotine consumption, packs per day (equivalent) 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.3) 0.639
Self-esteem (RSE) total score 20.2 (6.0) 22.4 (6.3) 0.005 -0.36 **
Emotional dysregulation (DERS) total score 81.1 (20.0) 71.3 (17.3) <0.001 0.52 ***
Any current MINI psychiatric disorder, excepting impulse control disorders, N [%] 45 [52.3%] 109 [32.3%] <0.001c 0.17 ***
Any current impulse control disorder, N [%] # 14 [16.3%] 33 [9.8%] 0.0875c
BIS-11
  Attention
  Motor
  Non-planning
19.9 (3.9)
26.0 (5.1)
26.8 (4.4)
16.2 (3.9)
23.1 (4.7)
23.3 (5.6)
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
0.95
0.59
0.70
***
***
***

All scores are mean ± SD unless otherwise noted. Statistic: ANOVAs except where indicated with ‘c’ for chi-square. y: Yates’ correction applied. Effect sizes are Cohen’s D except for chi-square, which are phi. # education scores: 1 = did not complete high school, 2 = high school graduate, 3 = some college, 4 = college graduate, 5 = beyond college level education. SCI-GD = Structured Clinical Interview for Gambling Disorder; IAT = Young’s Internet Addiction Test; RSE = Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale; DERS = Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale; MINI = Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory; BIS-11 = Barratt Impulsivity Scale; *p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001.

MINI: the count N [%] of each type of disorder in the ADHD and control groups respectively were: any mood disorder: 6 [7.0%], 10 [3.0%]; any anxiety disorder: 10 [20.9%], 44 [13.1%]; any substance use disorder: 32 [37.2%], 60 [17.8%]; any eating disorder: 4 [4.7%], 7 [2.1%]. MIDI: the count N [%] of each disorder, in the ADHD and control groups respectively were: Compulsive buying disorder: 5 [5.8%], 14 [4.2%]; Kleptomania: 1 [1.2%], 0 [0%]; Trichotillomania: 0 [0%], 2 [0.6%]; Intermittent explosive disorder: 3 [3.5%], 6 [1.8%]; Pyromania: 0 [0%], 1 [0.3%]; pathological gambling: 16 [18.6%], 64 [19%]; Compulsive sexual behavior: 5 [5.8%], 7 [2.1%]; Binge-eating disorder: 3 [3.5%], 4 [1.2%]; Skin picking disorder: 3 [3.5%], 3 [0.9%].

The ADHD symptom group did not differ from controls in terms of age, education, gender, problem gambling behaviors (SCI-GD) or amounts of nicotine and alcohol consumed. Compared to controls, the ADHD symptom group showed significantly lower quality of life, lower self-esteem (RSE questionnaire), higher emotional dysregulation (DERS), more psychiatric disorders besides impulse control disorders, and higher questionnaire-rated impulsivity (Barratt questionnaire). The ADHD symptom group also had higher problem internet use scores, albeit with marginal statistical significance. Although occurrence of one or more impulse control disorders was numerically higher in the ADHD symptom group, the difference versus controls did not obtain statistical significance.

In terms of neuropsychological test performance, the ADHD symptom group was impaired on stop-signal reaction times, but intact on the other cognitive domains that were examined.

The logistic regression model was statistically significant (Chi-square = 49.936, p<0.001); group status was significantly predicted by stop-signal reaction times (B=0.221, Wald=13.514, p<0.001), and Barratt attentional impulsivity (B=0.005, Wald=4.063, p=0.044). The classification model exhibited overall correct classification of 79.6%, with high specificity (94.6%) but limited sensitivity (34.7%).

Secondary analysis of variables that differed between the ADHD symptom group and controls (identified in Tables 1 & 2) indicated that there were no statistically significant effects of gender on these variables in either group (all p>0.10), nor in the whole study population (all p>0.10).

Table 2. Comparison of the Study Groups on Cognitive Variables.

Variable ADHD symptoms (N=86) Controls (N=337) p-value Effect Size
SST SSRT 205.1 (78.1) 176.1 (58.0) <0.001 0.42 ***
SST Median Correct RT on GO Trials 515.3 (196.9) 482.8 (176.4) 0.138
SWM total errors 18.9 (18.4) 18.2 (19.1) 0.763
IED ED errors 9.1 (9.7) 10.2 (9.9) 0.366

All scores are mean ± SD unless otherwise noted. Statistic: ANOVAs.Effect sizes are Cohen’s D. SST = Stop Signal Task; SSRT = Stop-Signal Reaction Time; IDED = Intra-Dimensional/Extra-Dimensional set-shift Task; SWM = Spatial Working Memory task *p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001.

Discussion

Despite ADHD symptoms being relatively common, the majority of studies exploring different types of impulsivity in relation to ADHD have focused on just one or two aspects of impulsivity. We explored the relationship between ADHD symptoms and various forms of impulsivity in a sample of young adults recruited from the general community, with controls also recruited using the same methodology. All participants were selected on the basis of gambling 5 or more times in the preceding year. The key findings were that ADHD symptoms were significantly associated with worse quality of life, lower self-esteem, more emotional dysregulation, higher questionnaire-based measures of impulsivity (Barratt questionnaire), more psychiatric disorders (besides impulse control disorders), higher problem internet use, and worse response inhibition. When binary logistic regression was used to identify independent predictors of group classification, stop-signal reaction times and Barratt attentional impulsiveness were significantly predictive variables, albeit the model displayed limited fit (high specificity but relatively low sensitivity).

Contrary to our expectations, ADHD symptoms were not significantly associated with higher rates of impulse control disorders, more problem gambling, or cognitive impairments in domains other than inhibitory control (working memory, set-shifting).

Quality of life, self-esteem, and emotional dysregulation

The finding that the group with ADHD symptoms manifested lower quality of life than controls (small-medium effect size) confirms that our method of identifying this group was clinically meaningful; i.e. the participants were significantly negatively impacted by the symptoms. Beyond the lower quality of life, the ADHD symptom group also showed lower self-esteem (small-medium effect size). Reduced self-esteem has been reported both in child/adolescent ADHD53,54 and in adult patients,55 recruited from clinical settings. Problems with self-esteem in ADHD play an important role of how these individuals adapt psychosocially in adolescence and in later life.5558The association we found between emotional dysregulation and ADHD symptoms (medium effect size) supports the view that the former is an important component in understanding ADHD in adults.

Co-morbidity, including with other impulsive symptom types

ADHD symptoms were associated with greater risk of psychiatric disorders in general as quantified by the MINI assessment, but with small effect size, and this appeared to have been mostly driven by substance use disorders, suicidality, and antisocial personality disorder (see footer, Table 1), which are indeed common in clinical ADHD settings likewise.2,6063 Interestingly, the ADHD symptom group showed higher occurrence of problem internet use. Previous research has found that, in a sample of outpatients with internet addiction, ADHD occurred in 14% of the sample.18 Compulsive internet use was associated with elevated risk of co-morbid ADHD in a meta-analysis, with an odds ratio of 2.9.19 Contrary to expectation, however, our study groups did not differ on overall occurrence of impulse control disorders as indexed by the MIDI, nor in terms of problem gambling scores. This may in part reflect the relative scarcity of impulse control disorders, but could also suggest that relationships between ADHD symptoms and formal impulse control disorders are quite subtle in young adults, recruited from the community.

Questionnaire and cognitive types of impulsivity

Questionnaire based composite measures of impulsivity and compulsivity are useful in that they are quick to administer, convenient, and well-suited for population based studies. We found that ADHD symptoms were associated with higher scores on all three subscales of the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS), with medium-large effect sizes. Consistent with our data, elevated BIS scores have been previously documented in ADHD.26

The identification of impaired response inhibition in people with ADHD (small-medium effect size) accords well with previous meta-analyses, which reported impaired response inhibition across ADHD clinical studies.32,67 Weak relationships have been found between neuropsychological functioning and severity of ADHD symptoms (e.g. 75, 76). Our findings suggest that impaired response inhibition extends to individuals with ADHD symptoms who have not generally sought treatment, likely reflecting the milder end of the disease spectrum. Stop-signal impairment represents a particularly promising cognitive marker for ADHD risk given that other cognitive domains we assessed (working memory, set-shifting) were intact in our sample. Impaired response inhibition appears to represent a trait marker for ADHD, which is significantly heritable.68, rather than being particularly related to current symptom severity. Response inhibition is dependent on the integrity of neural circuitry including the right inferior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex.51 Patients with ADHD show hypoactivation of fronto-striatal circuitry when undertaking functional imaging versions of such inhibitory control tasks.69,70

Limitations

While this study has positive features, such as inclusion of a broad set of impulsivity measures, and the use of a community sample, several limitations should be considered. All participants were recruited on the basis of gambling 5 or more times in the preceding year according to self-report. While it could be argued that our findings may not therefore generalize to the background population, it is likely that the majority of individuals in the background population gamble at least five times or more per year (see www.addiction.ucalgary.ca), and so we believe our sample – while somewhat enriched in terms of impulsivity – is informative. We intentionally focused on ADHD symptoms rather than formally diagnosed ADHD. It should be noted that the clinical diagnosis of ADHD requires detailed assessment including use of gold-standard measures besides the ASRS. As such, our results may differ from those found in formally diagnosed patients, who are likely to present with a more severe form of pathology, and hence may well present with higher rates of psychiatric co-morbidities including impulse control disorders and substance use than observed here. According to previous data, the method of identifying ADHD symptoms we utilized detects true ADHD cases with around 68.7% sensitivity and 99.5% specificity2. Thus, while nearly all of the ‘ADHD symptom’ group would have been expected to actually have ADHD, some cases of underlying clinical ADHD would have been overlooked and included in the control group. Missed cases in the control group are likely to be on the lesser severity end of the spectrum and this is also supported by.3 where those cases that screened positive on ASRS were likely to have a higher average symptom severity score compared to those who scored negative. This being an exploratory study, and based on our a priori power calculation, we did not correct our findings for multiple comparisons; as such, replication studies would be warranted before firm conclusions are drawn. That being said, the overall composite MANOVA test was highly significant, and the majority of significant group differences would have survived correction for multiple comparisons. While our sample size had >95% power to detect a statistically significant difference between study groups of medium (0.5) or greater effect size, ability to detect smaller effects would have been more limited. We did not record current medication usage e.g. stimulants; however, the overwhelming majority of subjects with ADHD symptoms in our study (91.7%) had never been diagnosed for ADHD, and therefore are unlikely to have been receiving treatment for their symptoms. Lastly, we did not record information about neurological symptoms or history of head trauma.

Conclusion

In summary, self-report ADHD symptoms appear to be very common in young adults who gamble 5 or more times per year, and are associated with highly specific cognitive impairment in terms of inhibitory dyscontrol, higher than expected questionnaire-based impulsivity-compulsivity scores, and high emotional dysregulation, along with increased prevalence of axis-I disorders in general. The majority of people with these ADHD symptoms by self-report had never received a formal diagnosis: in part this will reflect ‘false positives’ of this method of ADHD screening, but it is also likely that many cases of clinically significant ADHD are being overlooked. Future work should further evaluate the functional consequences of these findings relating to impulsivity longitudinally, and consider whether the specificity and sensitivity of ADHD screening instruments, could be improved by incorporating cognitive or questionnaire measures. It would also be important for future studies to confirm the generalizability of these findings, and to further explore the impact of ADHD treatments on domains of emotional dysregulation, self-esteem, and inhibitory control: these aspects of ADHD are not well-reflected in the current core diagnostic criteria of the disorder.

Funding

This study was funded by a Center in Research Excellence in Gambling grant from the National Center for Responsible Gaming (NCRG to Dr. Grant). Dr. Chamberlain’s involvement in this research was supported by a grant from the Academy of Medical Sciences (AMS, UK).

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, Howes MJ, Jin R, Secnik K, Spencer T, Ustun TB, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):245–56. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, Faraone SV, Greenhill LL, Howes MJ, Secnik K, Spencer T, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):716–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Nigg JT. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and adverse health outcomes. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(2):215–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.005. Epub 2012 Dec 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fayyad J, De Graaf R, Kessler R, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Demyttenaere K, De Girolamo G, Haro JM, Karam EG, Lara C, Lépine JP, et al. Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:402–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Atwoli L, Owiti P, Manguro G, Ndambuki D. Self-reported attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder symptoms among university students in Eldoret, Kenya. East Afr Med J. 2010;87(5):187–91. doi: 10.4314/eamj.v87i5.63072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121(1):65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. Review. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(3):255–61. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Daruna JH, Barnes PA. "A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity". In The Impulsive Client: Theory, Research, and Treatment. Am Psychol. 1993:23–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Vingilis E, Mann RE, Erickson P, Toplak M, Kolla NJ, Seeley J, Jain U. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, other mental health problems, substance use, and driving: examination of a population-based, representative canadian sample. Traffic Inj Prev. 2014;15(Suppl 1):S1–9. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2014.926341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, van de Glind G, van den Brink W, Smit F, Crunelle CL, Swets M, Schoevers RA. Prevalence of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in substance use disorder patients: a meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;122(1-2):11–9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, Crunelle CL, Carpentier PJ. Substance use disorders and ADHD: an overview of recent Dutch research. Tijdschr Psychiatr. 2013;55(11):861–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kooij JJ, Huss M, Asherson P, Akehurst R, Beusterien K, French A, Sasané R, Hodgkins P. Distinguishing comorbidity and successful management of adult ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2012;16(5 Suppl):3S–19S. doi: 10.1177/1087054711435361. Epub 2012 Apr 12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Faregh N, Derevensky J. Gambling behavior among adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Gambl Stud. 2011;27(2):243–56. doi: 10.1007/s10899-010-9211-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Breyer JL, Botzet AM, Winters KC, Stinchfield RD, August G, Realmuto G. Young adult gambling behaviors and their relationship with the persistence of ADHD. J Gambl Stud. 2009;25(2):227–38. doi: 10.1007/s10899-009-9126-z. Epub 2009 Mar 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grall-Bronnec M, Wainstein L, Augy J, Bouju G, Feuillet F, Vénisse JL, Sébille-Rivain V. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among pathological and at-risk gamblers seeking treatment: a hidden disorder. Eur Addict Res. 2011;17(5):231–40. doi: 10.1159/000328628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bernardi S, Pallanti S. Internet addiction: a descriptive clinical study focusing on comorbidities and dissociative symptoms. Compr Psychiatry. 2009;50(6):510–6. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Carli V, Durkee T, Wasserman D, Hadlaczky G, Despalins R, Kramarz E, Wasserman C, Sarchiapone M, Hoven CW, Brunner R, Kaess M. The association between pathological internet use and comorbid psychopathology: a systematic review. Psychopathology. 2013;46(1):1–13. doi: 10.1159/000337971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Odlaug BL, Lust K, Schreiber LR, Christenson G, Derbyshire K, Harvanko A, Golden D, Grant JE. Compulsive sexual behavior in young adults. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2013;25(3):193–200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kessler RC, Berglund PA, Chiu WT, Deitz AC, Hudson JI, Shahly V, et al. The prevalence and correlates of binge eating disorder in the world health organization world mental health surveys. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:904–914. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cortese S, Isnard P, Dalla Bernardina B, Mouren MC. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, binge eating, and obesity. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68(6):976. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v68n0624h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cortese S, Castellanos FX. The relationship between ADHD and obesity: implications for therapy. Expert Rev Neurother. 2014;14(5):473–9. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2014.904748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Congdon E, Canli T. A neurogenetic approach to impulsivity. J Pers. 2008;76(6):1447–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00528.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–74. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Malloy-Diniz L, Fuentes D, Leite WB, Correa H, Bechara A. Impulsive behavior in adults with attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: characterization of attentional, motor and cognitive impulsiveness. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(4):693–8. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070889. Epub 2007 May 18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Winstanley CA, Eagle DM, Robbins TW. Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26(4):379–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(4):177–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10(2):276–91. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Sarver DE, Kofler MJ. ADHD and behavioral inhibition: a re-examination of the stop-signal task. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36(7):989–98. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9230-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control? J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(2):216–22. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):1064–76. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Aichert DS, Wöstmann NM, Costa A, Macare C, Wenig JR, Möller HJ, Rubia K, Ettinger U. Associations between trait impulsivity and prepotent response inhibition. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34(10):1016–32. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.706261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Broos N, Schmaal L, Wiskerke J, Kostelijk L, Lam T, Stoop N, Weierink L, Ham J, de Geus EJ, Schoffelmeer AN, van den Brink W, et al. The relationship between impulsive choice and impulsive action: a cross-species translational study. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e36781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Corbisiero S, Stieglitz RD, Retz W, Rösler M. Is emotional dysregulation part of the psychopathology of ADHD in adults? Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2013;5(2):83–92. doi: 10.1007/s12402-012-0097-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Barkley RA, Fischer M. The unique contribution of emotional impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as adults. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(5):503–13. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201005000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Yorks D, Miller CA, Petty CR, Faraone SV. Deficient emotional self-regulation and adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a family risk analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(6):617–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10081172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Miller CA, McDermott KM, Faraone SV. Understanding deficient emotional self-regulation in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a controlled study. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2013a;5(3):273–81. doi: 10.1007/s12402-012-0100-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Miller CA, Petty CR, Faraone SV. Neuropsychological Deficits Are Not Predictive of Deficient Emotional Self-Regulation in Adults With ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2013b doi: 10.1177/1087054713476548. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rösler M, Retz W, Thome J, Schneider M, Stieglitz RD, Falkai P. Psychopathological rating scales for diagnostic use in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(Suppl 1):i3–11. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-1001-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Grant JE. Impulse control disorders: a clinician’s guide to understanding and treating behavioral addictions. Norton W. W. and Company; New York: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Grant JE, Steinberg MA, Kim SW, Rounsaville BJ, Potenza MN. Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling (SCI-PG) Psychiatry Res. 2004;128:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Young KS. Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyber Psychol Behav. 1998;1:237–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Frisch MB, Clark MP, Rouse SV, Rudd MD, Paweleck JK, Greenstone A, Kopplin DA. Predictive and treatment validity of life satisfaction and the quality of life inventory. Assessment. 2005;12(1):66–78. doi: 10.1177/1073191104268006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol & Behav. 2004;26(1):41–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(3):276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cambridge Cognition Limited. CANTABeclipse, version 3. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge Cognition Limited; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336–46. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:170–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:1021–34. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kurman J, Rothschild-Yakar L, Angel R, Katz M. How Good Am I? Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem as a Function of Perceived Parenting Styles Among Children With ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2015 Feb 11; doi: 10.1177/1087054715569599. pii: 1087054715569599. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Yen CF, Chou WJ, Liu TL, Yang P, Hu HF. The association of Internet addiction symptoms with anxiety, depression and self-esteem among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(7):1601–8. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cook J, Knight E, Hume I, Qureshi A. The self-esteem of adults diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a systematic review of the literature. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2014;6(4):249–68. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0133-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Newark PE, Elsȁsser M, Stieglitz R. Self-esteem, self-efficacy, and resources in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2012 doi: 10.1177/1087054712459561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Shaw-Zirt Barbara, Popali-Lehane Leelawatte, Chaplin William, Bergman Andrea. Adjustment, Social Skills, and Self-Esteem in College Students with Symptoms of ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2005;8(3):109–20. doi: 10.1177/1087054705277775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Slomkowski C, Klein RG, Mannuzza S. Is Self-Esteem an Important Outcome in Hyperactive Children? J Abnorm Child Psych. 1995;23(3):303–15. doi: 10.1007/BF01447559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Knouse LE, Safren SA. Current status of cognitive behavioral therapy for adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Clin N Am. 2010;33(3):497–509. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2010.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Edwards AC, Kendler KS. Twin study of the relationship between adolescent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and adult alcohol dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73(2):185–94. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Impey M, Heun R. Completed suicide, ideation and attempt in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;125(2):93–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01798.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sundquist J, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, Kendler KS. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and risk for drug use disorder: a population-based follow-up and co-relative study. Psychol Med. 2015;45(5):977–83. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001986. Epub 2014 Aug 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wilens TE, Upadhyaya HP. Impact of substance use disorder on ADHD and its treatment. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68(8):e20. doi: 10.4088/jcp.0807e20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Subramaniam M, Wang P, Soh P, Vaingankar JA, Chong SA, Browning CJ, Thomas SA. Prevalence and determinants of gambling disorder among older adults: A systematic review. Addict Behav. 2015;41C:199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Villemonteix T, Purper-Ouakil D, Romo L. [Is emotional dysregulation a component of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)?] Encephale. 2015;41(2):108–14. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2013.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Reimherr FW, Marchant BK, Gift TE, Steans TA, Wender PH. Types of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): baseline characteristics, initial response, and long-term response to treatment with methylphenidate. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2015 May 19; doi: 10.1007/s12402-015-0176-z. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Chamberlain SR, Robbins TW, Winder-Rhodes S, Müller U, Sahakian BJ, Blackwell AD, Barnett JH. Translational approaches to frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using a computerized neuropsychological battery. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):1192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Crosbie J, Arnold P, Paterson A, Swanson J, Dupuis A, Li X, Shan J, Goodale T, Tam C, Strug LJ, Schachar RJ. Response inhibition and ADHD traits: correlates and heritability in a community sample. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41(3):497–507. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9693-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Morein-Zamir S, Dodds C, van Hartevelt TJ, Schwarzkopf W, Sahakian B, Müller U, Robbins T. Hypoactivation in right inferior frontal cortex is specifically associated with motor response inhibition in adult ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(10):5141–52. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Sebastian A, Gerdes B, Feige B, Klöppel S, Lange T, Philipsen A, Tebartz van Elst L, Lieb K, Tüscher O. Neural correlates of interference inhibition, action withholding and action cancelation in adult ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202(2):132–41. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Wodushek TR, Neumann CS. Inhibitory capacity in adults with symptoms of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;18(3):317–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wilber MK, Potenza MN. Adolescent gambling: research and clinical implications. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 2006 Oct;3(10):40–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Barnes GM, Welte JW, Hoffman JH, Tidwell MC. Gambling, alcohol, and other substance use among youth in the United States. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009 Jan;70(1):134–42. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Welte JW, Barnes GM, Wieczorek WF, Tidwell MC, Parker J. Gambling participation in the U.S.: Results from a national survey. J Gambl Stud. 2002;18:313–337. doi: 10.1023/a:1021019915591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Barkley RA, Fischer M. Predicting impairment in major life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children as adults: self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF tests. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(2):137–61. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010 May;25(3):157–73. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES