Skip to main content
Journal of Physical Therapy Science logoLink to Journal of Physical Therapy Science
. 2017 Feb 24;29(2):221–223. doi: 10.1589/jpts.29.221

Effects of visual fatigue caused by smartphones on balance function in healthy adults

Young-Hyun Park 1,3, Chang-Man An 1,3,*, Sung-Jun Moon 2,3
PMCID: PMC5332974  PMID: 28265143

Abstract

[Purpose] The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of visual fatigue caused by smartphone use on balance function. [Subjects and Methods] The participants consisted of 22 healthy male and female adults. Their postural stability, limit of stability, and limit of stability running time were evaluated using a computerized posturography apparatus before and after inducing visual fatigue. Postural stability and the limit of stability were divided into static and dynamic conditions. [Results] There were significant differences between the dynamic postural stability, the static and dynamic limit of stability, and both the static and dynamic limit of stability running times after the induction of visual fatigue. [Conclusion] The results showed that visual fatigue caused by smartphone use has a negative effect on balance function. Therefore, reducing visual fatigue through proper rest is necessary.

Key words: Smartphone, Visual fatigue, Balance function

INTRODUCTION

Other than its primary purpose of telecommunication, smartphones have advantages as portable devices allowing connection to the internet, games, and online communities1). However, because smartphone technology is based on blue light, there is a high probability of damaging vision due to the shorter wavelength of light when used for a long period of time2). From the research on ocular fatigue induced by multimedia aimed toward the general public, it has been discovered that liquid crystal displays induce dry eye symptoms more frequently than ordinary books and the displays increase the level of visual fatigue from feeling dazzled3). If the visual fatigue continues in everyday life, it can affect visual processing and can cause various problems such as ocular pain, damaged corneal epithelial cells, conjunctival hyperemia, and decreased visual acuity. This continued visual fatigue also has a negative effect on visual feedback processing that integrates the vestibular organ system with the somatosensory network and can further cause an interruption in the body’s postural control system4,5,6). Vision as part of the postural control system may vary depending on ground stability and visual task. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to determine the effects of visual fatigue from smartphones on balance function depending on ground stability and visual task level and to provide smartphone users basic vision care information for preventive measures.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

This study included 22 young healthy adults (11 males and 11 females with an average height of 169.9 ± 7.8 cm and an average weight of 62.7 ± 13.8 kg) after they provided informed consent to participate in this study. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the local research ethics committee approved the study protocol.

Individuals with a history of ophthalmological injury or disease that may affect tear secretion, who have had surgery or with a disorder that may affect postural control, have had LASIK surgery, were diagnosed with xerophthalmia, or who wear contact lenses were excluded. The size of the smartphones was limited to 4.7–5.7 inches and the equipment type was limited to either the GALAXY NOTE 5 (SM-N920S), the LG G5 (F-700S), or the iPhone 6S (A-1688).

After sufficient rest without visual fatigue was given, subjects were positioned upright with their arms on a table and the smartphones were consistently held 40 cm away from the eyes7). Viewing time to induce the visual fatigue was limited to 40 minutes8).

Computerized posturography balance data were collected using the Biodex Balance System SD (BBS, Biodex Medical System, Inc., New York, USA). The BBS was used to measure the postural stability (PS) and the limit of stability (LOS). Subjects were positioned on the apparatus with their two hands together and their feet apart to adjust the center of pressure (COP) so that it was at the center of the concentric circle. Measurements were calculated from the process of the participants maintaining or moving their COP.

The subjects were instructed to maintain their COP at the center of the concentric circle which was indicated on the monitor in order to measure static and dynamic PS before and after the induction of visual fatigue. Three trials where two measurements were made during a 20 s period at 10 s intervals were completed by the subjects and the measurements were averaged over the three trials. The dynamic PS was measured while the participant controlled the unfixed footboard that provided 12 different levels of stability, levels 12 to 1, decreasing from 12, which was the most stable, to 1, the least stable. The apparatus was set at level 5 for the static PS experiment. The stability index scores were calculated from the degree of tilt deviation from horizontal; the lower postural stability index score indicated superior balance function.

For static LOS measurements before and after the induction of visual fatigue, the footboard was fixed and eight randomly positioned markings flashed around the reference point in the center of the monitor. Measurements were made as the participants changed the position of the COP in response to the eight signals; the return time was calculated. The dynamic LOS was measured while controlling the unfixed footboard at 12 different levels, level 12 to 1, with decreasing stability. The apparatus was set at level 5 for assessment of static LOS, and measurements were calculated using the same method. The static and dynamic LOS running time were recorded. The higher LOS score and shorten running time indicated superior balance function.

The general characteristics of the participants were characterized as the average ± standard deviation (SD). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to analyze the differences in PS, LOS, and LOS running time before and after the induction of visual fatigue. The paired t-test was used for comparisons of normally distributed data sets. The data were analyzed with PASW statistical software ver. 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) using p<0.05 as the level of significance.

RESULTS

Patient characteristics are listed in Table 1. The overall stability index (OSI) assessing the changed COP from all directions was used, with the lowest score referring to good postural balance control and sufficient PS. The OSI of static PS before inducing visual fatigue (0.36 ± 0.17) was not significantly different than PS after inducing visual fatigue (0.38 ± 0.2). However, the OSI of dynamic PS significantly increased from before (0.82 ± 0.4) to after (1.02 ± 0.59) the induction of visual fatigue (p<0.01) (Table 2).

Table 1. General patient characteristics (N=22).

Characteristics Mean ± SD Range
Gender (M/F) 11/11
Age (years) 25.3 ± 3.8 22–29
Height (cm) 169.9 ± 7.8 185–153
Weight (kg) 62.7 ± 13.8 90–46

Values are expressed as mean ± SD. M: male; F: female

Table 2. Comparison of postural control before and after inducing visual fatigue (N=22).

Value Before visual fatigue After visual fatigue
Static PS 0.36 ± 0.2 0.38 ± 0.2
Dynamic PS 0.82 ± 0.4 1.02 ± 0.6**
Static LOS (%) 60.32 ± 13.9 51.96 ± 15.7**
Dynamic LOS (%) 35.68 ± 11.5 29.55 ± 12.2**
Static LOS running time (sec) 35.55 ± 8.4 40.5 ± 14.2*
Dynamic LOS running time (sec) 47.36 ± 9.5 58.1 ± 20.7*

Values are expressed as mean ± SD. PS: postural stability; LOS: limit of stability. *p<0.05, **p<0.01

The directional control score (DCS) assessed the degree of COP control with a higher DCS indicating better performance during directional COP control. The DCS of static LOS significantly decreased from 60.32 ± 13.88 before visual fatigue induction to 51.96 ± 15.68 after induction (p<0.01). The DCS of dynamic LOS changed significantly from 35.68 ± 11.51 to 29.55 ± 12.19 before and after the visual fatigue induction, respectively (p<0.01) (Table 2).

The static LOS running time increased significantly after visual fatigue induction from 35.55 ± 8.38 s before to 40.5 ± 14.19 s after (p<0.05). The dynamic LOS running time also significantly increased from 47.36 ± 9.5 s to 58.05 ± 20.7 s before and after inducing visual fatigue, respectively (p<0.05) (Table 2).

DISCUSSION

This study investigated the effects of visual fatigue from using smartphones on postural control in 22 healthy adults. Visual fatigue was induced by placing smartphones 40 cm from the eyes for 1 hr. Static PS, dynamic PS, static LOS, dynamic LOS, static LOS running time, and dynamic LOS running time before and after the induction of visual fatigue were measured using BBS. Postural control ability during dynamic PS (p<0.01), static LOS (p<0.01), and dynamic LOS (p<0.01) after induction of visual fatigue significantly decreased but there were no significant changes in static PS. This can indicate that the static PS results in this study might have been affected by the measurement duration of 20 s, which may not be long enough to effectively modulate visual fatigue. Shu et al.9) showed that the maximum time a person can hold their blinking eyes with no visual fatigue was 23.12 s, but it was shortened to 17.09 s after 1 hr of computer work, reducing the ocular protection index. For LOS running time in this study, both static LOS and dynamic LOS running time significantly increased (p<0.05), confirming that postural control ability was diminished after the induction of visual fatigue. Therefore, visual fatigue may also substantially affect other studies such as the visual perception feedback study, where data is obtained regarding visual focusing ability, or in the mobility induced setting or the visual searching study which measured the level of attention10). Won et al.11) showed that changes in visual processing can affect the degree of dependence on proprioception and postural sway can be increased if visual processing is obstructed. The limitation of this study was the limited number of participants who had no balance deficits (22 young adults aged in the twenties). Also, It was unable to control the visual fatigue that may occur in the evaluation process BBS. Further studies should be carried with a wide age range of subject through an objective indicator of the visual fatigue index. In conclusion, the results of this study demonstrated that visual fatigue caused by smartphone use interferes with the visual ability to control posture and possibly reduces the ability to balance. Thus, this study suggests that it is necessary for smartphone users to reduce visual fatigue by taking appropriate rest. Also, proper guidelines on the use of smartphones should be proposed on various symptoms caused by visual fatigue from smartphones.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Park YM: A study on adult’s smartphone addiction and mental health [dissertation]. Sanji University, 2011. (in Korean).
  • 2.Kang SY, Hong JE, Choi EJ, et al. : Blue-light induces the selective cell death of photoreceptors in mouse retina. J Korean Ophthalmic Opt Soc, 2016, 21: 69–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jang YS, Kim US, Baek SH, et al. : Research on ocular fatigability induced by multimedia and instrument features. J Korean Ophthalmol Soc, 2012, 53: 689–693. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Park HJ, Yi K: Relationship between middle school student’s computer using time and dry eye. J Korean Ophthalmol Soc, 2002, 43: 449–454. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lateiner JE, Sainburg RL: Differential contributions of vision and proprioception to movement accuracy. Exp Brain Res, 2003, 151: 446–454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M: Motor control: translating research into clinical practice, 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lee SH: Evaluating the influence of smart phone use on the psycho-physiological condition [dissertation]. Seoul: Konkuk University, 2010. (in Korean).
  • 8.Park JS, Choi MJ, Ma JE, et al. : Influence of cellular phone videos and games on dry eye syndrome in university students. J Korean Acad Community Health Nurs, 2014, 25: 12–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Suh YW, Kim KH, Kang SY, et al. : The objective methods to evaluate ocular fatigue associated with computer work. J Korean Ophthalmol Soc, 2010, 51: 1327–1332. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Shen J, Reingold EM, Pomplun M: Guidance of eye movements during conjunctive visual search: the distractor-ratio effect. Can J Exp Psychol, 2003, 57: 76–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Won MH, Kim MC, Kim SJ, et al. : The effect of visual information provision on the changes of electromyogram activity in trunk and lower leg muscles during dynamic balance control. Korean J Sports Med, 2014, 32: 44–54. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Physical Therapy Science are provided here courtesy of Society of Physical Therapy Science

RESOURCES