Skip to main content
Medical Science Monitor Basic Research logoLink to Medical Science Monitor Basic Research
. 2017 Feb 22;23:31–35. doi: 10.12659/MSMBR.902385

Effect of Smoking on Cognitive Functioning in Young Saudi Adults

Shahid Bashir 1,2,A,B,C,D,E,F,, Faisal Alghamdi 3,A,B,D,E,F, Ahmed Alhussien 3,A,B,C,D,F, Meshal Alohali 3,A,B,C,D,E,F, Abdullah Alatawi 3,A,B,E,F, Tariq Almusned 3,B,E, Syed Shahid Habib 1,D,E
PMCID: PMC5333712  PMID: 28223681

Abstract

Background

Smoking is the predominant form of tobacco consumption and is growing worldwide, particularly in the younger generation in the Middle-East. We aimed to determine the effects of tobacco smoking on cognitive functions among young Saudi adults.

Material/Methods

We recruited a group of cigarette smokers (N=22) and a group of controls (non-smokers) (N=30) from apparently healthy male volunteers aged 18–29 years. Cognitive function was assessed by using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Automated Battery (CANTAB). The cognitive functions outcome variables were the response time (attention-switching task [AST]), and the percentage of correct response (pattern recognition memory [PRM] task). Clinical, demographic, blood markers (brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and apolipoprotein E) were assessed between groups.

Results

The 2 groups were matched for age and educational status. In comparison to the control group, smokers showed significant cognitive impairments in AST-Latency (p=0.001), AST-Congruent (p=0.001), and AST-Incongruent condition (p=0.001). There was not significant difference in BDNF APOE serum level between the 2 groups.

Conclusions

These results indicate that attention and alertness were significantly impaired in smokers compared to non-smokers.

MeSH Keywords: Memory, Mild Cognitive Impairment, Nicotine, Smoking Cessation

Background

Health authorities report that 6 million smokers in the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia (KSA) spend SR11 billion annually to satisfy their urge for nicotine [1]. Smokers in the KSA are mainly 17–40 years old [13]. Researchers report that smoking causes lung cancer and chronic respiratory and cardiovascular diseases, and is a risk factor for infections of the respiratory tract, adverse postoperative events, osteoporosis, delayed wound healing, diabetes, reproductive disorders, and duodenal and gastric ulcers [46]. All these characteristics of smoking show that it is a serious and chronic condition that occurs in a substantial part of the population.

Nicotine dependence is currently included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition (DSM-IV-TR) as a ‘substance use disorder’ [7].

The nicotine in cigarettes has a function similar to that of acetylcholine, which is a neurotransmitter found in most brain areas, modulating cognition via cortico-subcortical circuitry [8]. Due to the function similarity, nicotine has an effect on these receptors, including prefrontal cortices, amygdalo-hippocampal formation, and nucleus accumbens [810], which support daily cognitive tasks, including memory, attention, decision-making, and reward systems [1113]. However, the role of nicotine in cognition is controversial [14]. Chronic use of nicotine could be detrimental to cognitive functions. We used the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB), which is a computerized standard software, to assess cognitive functions according to the age and sex of participants. We used Attention-Switching Task (AST) and Pattern Recognition Memory (PRM) to assess cognitive functions, because memory and attention are main parameters used in assessing cognition in smokers [15].

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is one of the factors affecting the growth and survival of neurons; it is correlated with the number of cigarettes smoked, and has pro-atherothrombotic effects [16]. Apolipoprotein E (APOE) is reported to be elevated in people who smoke heavily and is a predictor of squamous metaplasia in the lung [17]. Another study found that elevated levels of APOE in smokers is correlated with amyloid levels in the brain, which increases the risk for Alzheimer disease [18].

Based on what is known about modulatory influences of nicotine on cognition, and on the above-described studies in young smokers, we hypothesized that smoking is associated with deficits in cognitive function among young people.

Material and Methods

Subjects

This cross-sectional study was conducted in the Department of Physiology, College of Medicine, King Saud University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia during the period Nov 2015 to May 2016. We matched subjects by age, sex, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status to estimate the impact of cigarette smoking on cognitive functions.

A detailed interview of the participants was conducted for study enrollment. All participants were questioned regarding cigarette smoking and use of other tobacco products. We excluded subjects based on a detailed history obtained regarding any drug intake, diseases such as seizures, and psychiatric problems.

Our study had 2 groups. The smoking group consisted of 22 male volunteers, with mean age 24.4±5.30 (Mean ±SD) years who were considered as regular cigarette smokers for at least 1 year and who smoked at least 1 cigarette per day. These 22 cigarette smokers were matched with a group of 30 non-cigarette smokers (control) who had never smoked, who were healthy male volunteers with mean age 23.31±2.68 (Mean ±SD) and were matched with the smoking group for age, sex, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. This control group primarily consisted of university students, as well as technicians, secretaries, research assistants, and receptionists. The Institutional Review Board, King Khalid University Hospital and College of Medicine, King Saud University, approved the study.

Methods

Blood sampling

Venous blood samples were collected from all participants and were stored at −70°C until assayed. Blood samples were analyzed for brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and apolipoprotein E (APOE) levels. Plasma BDNF and APOE concentrations were measured by competitive enzyme immunoassay using Human BDNF and APOE ELISA kits, following manufacturer’s instructions (Elabsciences Biotechnology Co., Ltd., China). Pre-coated antibody specific to BDNF and APOE reacted with samples and standards. Following blocking and incubation, BDNF- and APOE-specific antibodies were used. Both BDNF and APOE peptide and targeted peptide in samples interacted competitively with the BDNF and APOE antibodies.

Cognitive function

Neuropsychological testing was performed using CANTAB (Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery) research suite software (version 6. 0.37, Cambridge Cognition). The entire battery required 25–30 min to complete. The participants were made to sit comfortably on a chair and were asked to respond to test items by pressing the response button with the index finger of the dominant hand.

Attention-switching task (AST)

AST measured the participant’s ability to switch attention between the direction of an arrow and its location on the screen and to ignore task-irrelevant information in the face of interfering or distracting events. This test was designed to measure top-down cognitive control processes involving the prefrontal cortex. This test is a sensitive measure of frontal lobe and ‘executive’ dysfunction. The test displayed an arrow, which can appear on either side of the screen (right or left) and can point in either direction (to the right or to the left). Each trial displayed a cue at the top of the screen that indicates to the participant whether they have to press the right or left button according to the “side on which the arrow appeared” or the “direction in which the arrow was pointing”. Some trials displayed congruent stimuli (e.g., arrow on the right side of the screen pointing to the right) whereas other trials displayed incongruent stimuli, which required a higher cognitive demand (e.g., arrow on the right side of the screen pointing to the left).

Pattern recognition memory (PRM)

This is a test of visual pattern recognition memory in a 2-choice forced discrimination paradigm. A sequence of visual patterns was presented in the center of the screen. These patterns were designed so that they cannot easily be given verbal labels. In the recognition phase, the participants were required to choose between a pattern they had already seen and a novel pattern.

Statistical analysis

AST test outcome measures include response latencies and error scores, which reflected the participant’s attention-switching ability and the interference of congruent and incongruent task-irrelevant information. The PRM task allows measurement of the numbers of correct patterns selected, and statistical analysis measuring the probability of an error after a correct or incorrect response. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS software (version 22.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Comparison of quantitative data between the smoking group and the control group was performed using t tests for normally distributed data and the Mann-Whitney test for skewed data. Two-tailed statistics were used and statistical significance was set at P<0.05. Pearson correlations were used as needed.

Results

Table 1 summarizes the anthropometric variables including age, BDNF, and APOE. The control group consisted of 30 participants with mean age 23.31±2.68 (Mean ±SD) years and the smoking group consisted of 22 participants with mean age 24.4±5.30 (Mean ±SD) years.

Table 1.

Anthropometric data of smoking groups and control group.

Parameters Control group (n=30) Smoking group (n=22) P value
Age 21.8±1.73 22.9±3.75 0.190
BDNF 610±401 397±328 0.103
APOE 635±370 718±417 0.525

Vales are presented in Mean ±SD; NS – non-significant.

Table 2 summarizes the comparison of the cognitive function test parameters of smokers and their matched control group. There was a decline in the cognitive function parameters, including AST-Latency; 411±58.3 in the control group vs. 591±128.3 in smokers (p=0.001); AST-Congruent in control group was 393±51.3 vs. smokers 565±128.3 (p=0.001); AST-Incongruent in control 430±70 vs. smokers 619±137.8 (p=0.001) (Figure 1); PRM control 91.8±7.9 vs. smokers 88.35±7.4 (p=0.101).

Table 2.

Comparison of cognitive functions between smoking group compared to their matched control group.

Parameters Control group Smoking group p-value
AST-Latency 411±58.3 591±128.3 0.001
AST-Congruent 393±51.3 565±128.9 0.001
AST-Incongruent 430±70 619±137.8 0.001
PRM 91.8±7.9 88.5±7.4 0.101

Vales are presented in Mean ±SEM.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Comparison of cognitive functions between smokers compared to their matched control group for attention-switching task (AST) latency, AST-congruent, and AST-incongruent conditions.

Discussion

In the present study, we determined the effects of smoking on cognitive functions among young Saudis. We found that smoking impaired the cognitive performance compared to their matched control group. The impairment was statistically significant for attention, reaction time, and memory tasks. These domains of cognition were selectively tested because their use is supported by the literature and use of only selected domains decreases the testing time, thereby reducing participant exhaustion and resultant cofounding effects. However, we did not observe a significant difference in the PRM paradigm, perhaps due to preservation of memory functions in smokers. Confounders such as sleep, vision problems, drinking coffee, or smoking half an hour before testing were studied. The finding of associations between smoking and relative cognitive problems is concordant with several previous studies in young people [1921] but extends these findings to include dissociable domains tapped by well-validated translational cognitive tests. This study is the first of its kind in the Middle-East that investigated the effect of smoking on cognitive function.

Functional outcomes in smokers can be improved by smoking cessation [22]. Cognitive effects of smoking tobacco and drinking alcohol have been examined in other research, which found a cognitive decline but normal BDNF level. However, smoking severity is associated with low BDNF level [23]. Smoking is decreases cognition in patients with Parkinson disease, even after quitting [24]. Smokers who were matched for education, age, income, and sex had significant impairment in cognition, spatial working memory, sustained attention, and executive planning and had inappropriate behavior involving risk-taking as compared to non-smokers [15].

The limitations of our study are small its sample size and cross-sectional design. Large-scale prospective studies with more detailed assessments are required to determine the true links between smoking and cognitive impairment. Smoking is continuously spreading among youth worldwide and has been growing in popularity in the Middle-East.

Conclusions

Smoking impairs cognitive functions, expressed in our data by the significant increase in latency, as well as congruent and incongruent condition, of smokers compared to their matched controls in AST, which may be due to early frontal lobe and ‘executive’ dysfunction. Evidence of the harm caused by smoking justifies effective policies such as factual and visible health warnings on all tobacco products and advertising. Investment in research and policy initiatives to understand and control cigarette smoking needs to become a public health priority.

Footnotes

Source of support: Work on this study was supported by grants from the Deanship of Scientific Research grant (RGP-216) from King Saud University, Saudi Arabia

Competing interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  • 1.Al-Arifi MN. Prevalence of smoking and attitudes towards smoking cessation among community pharmacists, Saudi Arabia. J Pharm Technol. 2004;20:329–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Abolfotouh MA, Abdel Aziz M, Alakija W, et al. Smoking habits of King Saud University students in Abha, Saudi Arabia. Ann Saudi Med. 1998;18:212–16. doi: 10.5144/0256-4947.1998.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Siddiqui S, Ogbeide DO, Al Khalifa I. Smoking in a Saudi community: Prevalence, influencing factors, and risk perception. Fam Med. 2001;33:367–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Benowitz NL. Nicotine Addiction. New Engl J Med. 2010;362(24):2295–303. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0809890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.McKillop J, Amlung MT, Wier LM, et al. The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging study of delay discounting of monetary and cigarette rewards in smokers. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202(1):20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tuesta LM, Fowler CD, Kenny PJ. Recent advances in understanding nicotinic receptor signaling mechanisms that regulate drug self-administration behavior. Biochem Pharmacol. 2011;82:984–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2011.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Saß H, Wittchen H-U, Zaudig M, Houben I. Diagnostische Kriterien des Diagnostischen und statistischen Manuals psychischer Störungen DSM-IV. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1998. [in German] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mansvelder HD, van Aerde KI, Couey JJ, et al. Nicotinic modulation of neuronal networks: From receptors to cognition. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;184(3–4):292–305. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0070-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Perry EK, Smith CJ, Perry RH, et al. Regional distribution of muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic receptor binding activities in the human brain. J Chem Neuroanat. 1989;2(4):189–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rubboli F, Court JA, Sala C, et al. Distribution of neuronal nicotinic receptor subunits in human brain. Neurochem Int. 1994;25(1):69–71. doi: 10.1016/0197-0186(94)90055-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Paterson D, Nordberg A. Neuronal nicotinic receptors in the human brain. Prog Neurobiol. 2000;61(1):75–111. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(99)00045-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(3):255–61. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Robbins TW, Arnsten AF. The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: Monoaminergic modulation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:267–87. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chamberlain SR, Robbins TW, Winder-Rhodes S, et al. Translational approaches to frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using a computerized neuropsychological battery. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):1192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Schreiber LRN, Grant JE. Association between tobacco smoking and cognitive functioning in young adults. Am J Addict. 2012;21(Suppl 1):S14–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00290.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Amadio P, Baldassarre D, Sandrini L, et al. Effect of cigarette smoke on monocyte procoagulant activity: Focus on platelet-derived brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Platelets. 2016 doi: 10.1080/09537104.2016.1203403. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rice SJ, Liu X, Miller B, et al. Proteomic profiling of human plasma identifies apolipoprotein E as being associated with smoking and a marker for squamous metaplasia of the lung. Proteomics. 2015;15(18):3267–77. doi: 10.1002/pmic.201500029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Durazzo TC, Mattsson N, Weiner MW. Interaction of cigarette smoking history with APOE genotype and age on amyloid level, glucose metabolism, and neurocognition in cognitively normal elders. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(2):204–11. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Trauth JA, McCook EC, Seidler FJ, Slotkin TA. Modeling adolescent nicotine exposure: Effects on cholinergic systems in rat brain regions. Brain Res. 2000;873(1):18–25. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02465-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jacobsen LK, Krystal JH, Mencl WE, et al. Effects of smoking and smoking abstinence on cognition in adolescent tobacco smokers. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(1):56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R. Neurocognitive consequences of cigarette smoking in young adults – a comparison with pre-drug performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2006;28(4):517–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Glass JM, Adams KM, Nigg JT, et al. Smoking is associated with neurocognitive deficits in alcoholism. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;82:119–26. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zhang XY, Tan Y, Chen D, et al. Effects of cigarette smoking and alcohol use on neurocognition and BDNF levels in a Chinese population. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233(3):435–45. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4124-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Doiron M, Dupré N, Langlois M, et al. Smoking history is associated to cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Aging Ment Health. 2015 doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1090393. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Medical Science Monitor Basic Research are provided here courtesy of International Scientific Information, Inc.

RESOURCES