Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Fertil Steril. 2017 Jan 16;107(3):714–722. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2016.12.011

Preconception stress and the secondary sex ratio in a population-based preconception cohort

Jisuk Bae a,b,*, Courtney D Lynch c, Sungduk Kim b, Rajeshwari Sundaram b, Katherine J Sapra b, Germaine M Buck Louis b
PMCID: PMC5337443  NIHMSID: NIHMS838317  PMID: 28104242

Abstract

Objective

To examine the association between preconception parental stress and the secondary sex ratio (SSR), defined as the ratio of males to females at birth.

Design

A population-based preconception cohort.

Setting

Couples were interviewed separately at baseline to obtain information on perceived stress (Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale) and lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety and/or mood disorders. Female partners were also trained to collect basal saliva samples for the measurement of salivary stress markers, alpha-amylase and cortisol.

Patients/Animals

235 couples who were enrolled prior to conception in Michigan and Texas between 2005 and 2009 and who had a singleton birth during the follow-up period.

Intervention(s)

None.

Main Outcome Measure(s)

Birth outcome data including infant sex were collected upon delivery. Modified Poisson regression models were used to estimate the relative risks (RRs) of a male birth for each stress marker.

Results

After adjusting for potential confounders, we observed a 76 percent increase in the risk of fathering a male infant (RR, 1.76; 95% CI, 1.17–2.65) in men diagnosed with anxiety disorders compared to those who were not diagnosed. When lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders was modeled jointly for the couple, the association was slightly strengthened (RR, 2.03; 95% CI, 1.46–2.84).

Conclusion

This prospective cohort study suggests that paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders may be associated with an increase in the SSR, resulting in an excess of male births.

Keywords: alpha-amylases, anxiety disorders, hydrocortisone, sex ratio, stress

INTRODUCTION

Human sex ratios from birth through the lifespan have shown their modulation at the population level depending upon a variety of factors (1). The primary sex ratio (PSR) or the ratio of males to females at conception is relatively unknown due to the difficulty in measuring conceptions (2). Although it is conventionally recognized that the PSR is male-biased (1), a recent comprehensive study reported that the PSR is unbiased (3). On the other hand, the secondary sex ratio (SSR) or the ratio of males to females at birth is expected to range from 1.05 to 1.07 in the United States and worldwide, indicative of a slight excess of male births (4, 5). The SSR has been suggested as a possible indicator of population health and fertility despite the controversy over its utility, given that it is easy to measure, frequently recorded, and hardly subject to recall bias (69). Recent declines in the SSR observed in several developed countries have raised concerns about biological, psychological, and social factors disturbing sex selection and sex-selective survival in humans (7, 9).

Stress, which comprises multiple domains such as biological and psychological stress that induce physiological and behavioral responses, has been extensively evaluated in relation to human reproduction and development (10, 11). For instance, given the complex neuro-hormonal response exerted by stress, maternal stress is hypothesized to be responsible for various adverse reproductive outcomes, including preterm birth, low birth weight, and small for gestational age (12). It has also been suggested that the SSR, as a fertility endpoint, may be associated with maternal stress from various sources, such as economic contraction (13), stressful life events (14, 15), weather extremes (16), natural disasters (1719), human-made disasters including wars (20), political upheaval (21), and terrorist attacks (22, 23), and psychiatric disorders including anxiety and depression (24, 25).

The stress response in humans is mainly regulated by the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM) system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (26). The SAM system is the primary mechanism in control of the fight-or-flight response and the secretion of catecholamines, such as norepinephrine and epinephrine, by the adrenal medulla. Meanwhile, the HPA axis is responsible for the release of glucocorticoids, such as cortisol, which is modulated by the secretion of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in hypothalamus and pituitary gland, respectively. These pathways are believed to interact with the reproductive system, wherein the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis plays an important role in the regulation of normal reproductive function (27). The physiologic role of these neuroendocrine systems in stress response is evident by several readily measurable biomarkers, such as salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol (28, 29), which have been investigated in search for possible predictors of adverse reproductive endpoints in population-based studies (30, 31). To our knowledge, only one study evaluated maternal salivary stress markers, such as alpha-amylase and cortisol, in relation to the SSR. In a population-based preconception cohort study of 130 singleton births, the adjusted odds ratio (OR) for a male birth was decreased for women in the highest quartile of preconception salivary cortisol levels in comparison with women in the lowest quartile (adjusted OR, 0.26; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.09–0.74) (32).

Of note, less attention has been paid to the impact of paternal stress in comparison with that of maternal stress on human reproduction and development despite the couple-dependent nature of human conception. While prior research on paternal stress and the SSR is lacking, several hypotheses have been proposed to explain the role of both maternal and paternal stress in offspring sex determination (8, 3339). As theorized previously, maternal and paternal stress may affect the SSR in diverse ways, eliciting opposite effects on human sex selection. Namely, maternal stress is related to an increased testosterone secretion from the adrenal glands, whereas paternal stress is related to a decreased testosterone production by the testes (40, 41). According to one of the prevailing hypotheses on the SSR focusing on parental hormone levels around the time of conception (8, 3739), stressed women tend to produce sons, whereas stressed men tend to produce daughters. Furthermore, maternal stress results in high circulating glucose levels, which may be related to the development of male blastocysts relative to female blastocysts, possibly due to sex differences in the rate of glucose uptake (33, 34). However, as pregnancy continues, persistent maternal stress, especially during early pregnancy, may reduce or reverse an excess of male births, because it may be related to selective male losses relative to female losses (35, 36).

On the basis of the existing hypotheses on the SSR, the present study aimed to evaluate the impact of both maternal and paternal stress on the SSR in a population-based preconception cohort. Specifically, multiple domains of stress, which comprised both biological (i.e., salivary stress markers) and psychological (i.e., perceived stress) stress markers, were investigated in the present study in light of possible divergent human reactivity to various stressors.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study population

The Longitudinal Investigation of Fertility and the Environment (LIFE) Study is a prospective cohort study, in which 501 couples discontinuing contraception and attempting pregnancy were recruited from 16 counties in Michigan and Texas between 2005 and 2009, as described previously in detail (42). Couples were followed until pregnant or up to 12 months of trying to conceive and through delivery for those becoming pregnant. The eligibility criteria for participation included the following: (a) couples in a committed relationship; (b) females aged 18–40 years and males aged ≥ 18 years; (c) female partner’s self-reported menstrual cycle length of 21–42 days; (d) no use of injectable contraceptives during the past year; (e) no sterilization procedures or physician-diagnosed infertility; and (f) couples able to communicate in English or Spanish. Of the 501 couples, 237 couples (47.3%) had a live birth during the follow-up period, two of whom had twins. Our study cohort comprised 235 couples with a singleton birth.

Data collection

Baseline and follow-up data collection

Research assistants visited the couple’s home and interviewed each partner of the couple separately using standardized baseline questionnaires, allowing for ascertaining baseline characteristics of the couple, such as socio-demographic (i.e., age, sex, race/ethnicity, annual income, education level, and research site) and lifestyle factors (i.e., perceived stress) and medical (i.e., self-reported physician-diagnosed anxiety and/or mood disorders) and reproductive histories (i.e., maternal parity and number of pregnancies fathered).

Upon the baseline visit, the female partner underwent a urine pregnancy test to ensure the absence of a pre-existing pregnancy. Blood was collected and used to quantify serum cotinine (ng/mL), a metabolite of nicotine, using liquid chromatography-isotope dilution tandem mass spectrometry (43). Couples who had a live birth during the follow-up period were asked to return standardized birth announcements to ascertain information on date of birth, infant sex, birth size, and delivery mode. This study was performed in adherence with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at all collaborating institutions. All study participants provided written informed consent before any data collection.

Assessment of stress markers

Using the baseline questionnaires, perceived stress of each partner of the couple was assessed by the four-item version of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4) (44). The PSS-4 score was calculated by reversing the scores on two positive items and then summing across all four items, and ranged from 0 to 16 with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived stress. Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety and mood disorders was determined by the answer to the following question: Have you ever been told by a doctor that you have any of the following health conditions? Physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders included agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and other anxiety disorders. Physician-diagnosed mood disorders included major depression, bipolar disorder, and other mood disorders.

In addition, the female partner was asked to provide a basal saliva sample using the Salivette® collection device at two time points: (a) the morning following enrollment; and (b) the morning following the first observed menses after enrollment. The female partner was instructed to collect a basal or first-morning saliva sample immediately upon awakening before starting any daily activities, such as eating, drinking, smoking or tooth brushing. The samples were shipped overnight to a laboratory (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA, USA), and stored at −20°C until analysis. Salivary stress markers were quantified using established laboratory protocols inclusive of ongoing quality assurance and quality control procedures: salivary alpha-amylase (U/mL) was measured using a commercially available kinetic reaction assay (45); and salivary cortisol (μg/dL) was measured using a highly sensitive enzyme immunoassay (46).

Statistical analysis

In the descriptive phase of analysis, various statistical methods were utilized to examine the completeness of data and the distributions of variables. The distributions were summarized as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or geometric mean (GM) and accompanying 95% CI for continuous variables and frequency and percentage for categorical variables. Given the lack of statistically significant differences in salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol levels between the first and second samples, the average values of these salivary stress markers were used for analysis. Differences in baseline characteristics and stress markers by partner or infant sex were assessed using the nonparametric Wilcoxon test for continuous variables and Fisher’s exact test for categorical variables.

In the analytic phase, modified Poisson regression models with a robust error variance (47) were used to estimate the relative risk (RR) and 95% CI of a male birth for each stress marker. Separate models were first run for each stress marker. Subsequently, select maternal and paternal stress markers (i.e., PSS-4, lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders, and lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders) were modeled jointly for the couple to examine each partner’s stress while controlling for the other partner’s stress. Maternal salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol were modeled both as a continuous variable (log-transformed values) and a categorical variable (tertiles). Along with the unadjusted model, we evaluated two different adjusted models while controlling for a priori confounders: (a) age (years, continuous) and serum cotinine (ng/mL, continuous); and (b) further adjusting for annual income (< $70,000/≥ $70,000) and maternal parity (nulliparous/parous; for maternal stress markers only), based upon our review of the literature (5, 4858). Consistent with the exploratory design of the present study, significance was set at p-value < 0.05 without adjusting for multiple comparisons. All statistical analyses were performed by the SAS Version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

RESULTS

As reflected in Table 1, non-Hispanic white and college-educated couples comprised the majority of the study cohort. The mean age (± SD) of the study cohort was 29.8 (± 3.7) years for female partners and 31.5 (± 4.6) years for male partners. Approximately half of the female partners (46.8%) were nulliparous and 42.5% of the male partners had not previously fathered a pregnancy. Among the 235 live births, the SSR was 0.97 (95% CI, 0.75–1.26). A slightly higher age among men who fathered boys (mean ± SD, 32.2 ± 5.1) than among those who fathered girls (mean ± SD, 30.8 ± 3.9) was noted (p-value, 0.03). However, no significant differences were observed for the distributions of parity, race/ethnicity, annual income, education level, and research site by infant sex (Table 1).

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of the study participants by infant sex, 2005–2009

Characteristic Male (n=116)
n (%)
Female (n=119)
n (%)
Maternal characteristic

 Age (years, mean ± SD) 30.0 ± 4.0 29.5 ± 3.4
 Serum cotinine (ng/mL, GM [95% CI]) 0.03 (0.02–0.04) 0.04 (0.02–0.08)
 Parity
  Nulliparous 58 (50.4) 51 (43.2)
  Parous 57 (49.6) 67 (56.8)
 Annual income ($)
  < 70,000 27 (23.9) 32 (27.6)
  ≥ 70,000 86 (76.1) 84 (72.4)
 Education
  ≤ High school graduate/GED 5 (4.4) 4 (3.4)
  Some college/technical school 13 (11.4) 14 (11.8)
  College graduate or higher 96 (84.2) 101 (84.9)
 Race/ethnicity
  Non-Hispanic white 92 (80.7) 102 (85.7)
  Non-Hispanic black 2 (1.8) 1 (0.8)
  Hispanic 13 (11.4) 7 (5.9)
  Other 7 (6.1) 9 (7.6)

Paternal characteristic

 Age (years, mean ± SD) 32.2 ± 5.1* 30.8 ± 3.9*
 Serum cotinine (ng/mL, GM [95% CI]) 0.08 (0.04–0.15) 0.12 (0.06–0.23)
 Number of pregnancies fathered
  0 46 (43.0) 48 (42.1)
  ≥ 1 61 (57.0) 66 (57.9)
 Annual income ($)
  < 70,000 25 (21.9) 36 (30.5)
  ≥ 70,000 89 (78.1) 82 (69.5)
 Education
  ≤ High school graduate/GED 3 (2.6) 4 (3.4)
  Some college/technical school 34 (29.6) 23 (19.5)
  College graduate or higher 78 (67.8) 91 (77.1)
 Race/ethnicity
  Non-Hispanic white 91 (79.1) 105 (88.2)
  Non-Hispanic black 3 (2.6) 2 (1.7)
  Hispanic 13 (11.3) 8 (6.7)
  Other 8 (7.0) 4 (3.4)

Couple characteristic

 Research site
  Michigan 21 (18.1) 26 (21.9)
  Texas 95 (81.9) 93 (78.2)

CI, confidence interval; GED, General Educational Development; GM, geometric mean; SD, standard deviation.

*

Bolded values indicate p-value < 0.05.

The distributions of maternal and paternal stress markers by infant sex are presented in Table 2. The mean PSS-4 score was significantly higher among female partners (mean ± SD, 3.4 ± 2.3) than among male partners (mean ± SD, 3.0 ± 2.3) (p-value, 0.03). More female partners (7.2%) had lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders than did male partners (2.1%) (p-value, 0.01). However, none of the stress markers examined was significantly different by infant sex (Table 2).

Table 2.

Distributions of maternal and paternal stress markers by infant sex, 2005–2009

Stress marker Male (n=116)
n (%)
Female (n=119)
n (%)
Maternal stress marker

 PSS-4 (mean ± SD) 3.4 ± 2.3 3.4 ± 2.4
 Salivary α-amylase (U/mL, GM [95% CI]) 15.1 (12.3, 18.6) 14.8 (12.3, 17.8)
 Salivary cortisol (μg/dL, GM [95% CI]) 0.40 (0.36, 0.45) 0.36 (0.33, 0.40)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders
  No 111 (95.7) 107 (89.9)
  Yes 5 (4.3) 12 (10.1)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders
  No 107 (92.2) 111 (93.3)
  Yes 9 (7.8) 8 (6.7)

Paternal stress marker

 PSS-4 (mean ± SD) 2.9 ± 2.4 3.0 ± 2.3
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders
  No 111 (95.7) 116 (97.5)
  Yes 5 (4.3) 3 (2.5)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders
  No 114 (98.3) 115 (97.5)
  Yes 2 (1.7) 3 (2.5)

CI, confidence interval; GM, geometric mean; PSS-4, the four-item version of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale; SD, standard deviation.

Note: None of the stress markers differed significantly by infant sex (all p-values > 0.05). Physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders included agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Physician-diagnosed mood disorders included major depression, bipolar disorder, and other mood disorders.

Table 3 presents the RRs of a male birth by maternal and paternal stress markers when modeled separately. Neither paternal PSS-4 nor maternal PSS-4 was significantly associated with the SSR. No significant associations were observed for maternal salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol, when analyzed continuously and categorically. However, after adjustment for age, serum cotinine, and annual income, we observed a 76 percent increase in the risk of fathering a male infant (RR, 1.76; 95% CI, 1.17–2.65) in men diagnosed with anxiety disorders (two with panic disorder; two with generalized anxiety disorder; one with agoraphobia; and three with other anxiety disorders) compared to those who were not diagnosed (Table 3). When lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders was modeled jointly for the couple, the association was slightly strengthened (RR, 2.03; 95% CI, 1.46–2.84) (Table 4).

Table 3.

Stress markers and the relative risks of a male birth by partner, 2005–2009 (n=235)

Stress marker Unadjusted
RR (95% CI)
Adjusteda
RR (95% CI)
Adjustedb
RR (95% CI)
Maternal stress marker

 PSS-4 1.00 (0.95–1.06) 1.01 (0.95–1.06) 1.00 (0.95–1.06)
 Salivary α-amylase (U/mL)
  Log-transformed 1.01 (0.88–1.17) 1.01 (0.87–1.16) 1.00 (0.86–1.17)
  1st tertile 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  2nd tertile 1.06 (0.74–1.52) 1.06 (0.74–1.51) 1.07 (0.74–1.55)
  3rd tertile 1.13 (0.80–1.59) 1.11 (0.79–1.57) 1.10 (0.77–1.58)
 Salivary cortisol (μg/dL)
  Log-transformed 1.09 (0.98–1.21) 1.08 (0.97–1.21) 1.08 (0.97–1.21)
  1st tertile 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  2nd tertile 1.22 (0.86–1.74) 1.21 (0.85–1.72) 1.19 (0.83–1.71)
  3rd tertile 1.24 (0.87–1.77) 1.22 (0.86–1.75) 1.22 (0.85–1.75)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders
  No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  Yes 0.58 (0.27–1.22) 0.65 (0.31–1.38) 0.67 (0.32–1.39)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders
  No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  Yes 1.08 (0.68–1.72) 1.11 (0.69–1.77) 1.04 (0.62–1.75)

Paternal stress marker

 PSS-4 1.00 (0.94–1.05) 1.00 (0.94–1.06) 0.98 (0.92–1.05)
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders
  No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  Yes 1.28 (0.74–2.22) 1.32 (0.84–2.07) 1.76 (1.17–2.65)*
 Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders
  No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
  Yes 0.80 (0.27–2.37) 0.72 (0.27–1.94) 0.52 (0.10–2.86)

CI, confidence interval; PSS-4, the four-item version of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale; RR, relative risk.

Note: Modified Poisson regression models were used to estimate the relative risks of a male birth (Zou, 2004). All point and interval estimates were rounded to two decimal places. Physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders included agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Physician-diagnosed mood disorders included major depression, bipolar disorder, and other mood disorders.

a

Adjusted for age (continuous) and serum cotinine (continuous).

b

Adjusted for age (continuous), serum cotinine (continuous), annual income (< $70,000/= $70,000), and maternal parity (nulliparous/parous; for maternal stress markers only).

*

Bolded values indicate p-value < 0.05.

Table 4.

Couples’ stress markers and the relative risks of a male birth, 2005–2009 (n=235)

Stress marker Unadjusted
RR (95% CI)
Adjusteda
RR (95% CI)
Adjustedb
RR (95% CI)
PSS-4 (maternal) 1.00 (0.95–1.06) 1.00 (0.94–1.06) 0.97 (0.91–1.03)
PSS-4 (paternal) 1.00 (0.94–1.05) 1.00 (0.95–1.06) 0.99 (0.93–1.05)
Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders (maternal)
 No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
 Yes 0.57 (0.27–1.22) 0.65 (0.30–1.43) 0.65 (0.30–1.39)
Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders (paternal)
 No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
 Yes 1.31 (0.78–2.19) 1.35 (0.87–2.10) 2.03 (1.46–2.84)*
Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders (maternal)
 No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
 Yes 1.10 (0.68–1.76) 1.17 (0.69–1.98) 1.04 (0.55–1.96)
Lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders (paternal)
 No 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent) 1.00 (referent)
 Yes 0.78 (0.26–2.31) 0.67 (0.23–1.91) 0.51 (0.08–3.06)

CI, confidence interval; PSS-4, the four-item version of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale; RR, relative risk.

Note: Modified Poisson regression models were used to estimate the relative risks of a male birth (Zou, 2004). All point and interval estimates were rounded to two decimal places. In combination with other covariates, both maternal and paternal stress markers were included simultaneously in the model. Physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders included agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Physician-diagnosed mood disorders included major depression, bipolar disorder, and other mood disorders.

a

Adjusted for both partners’ age (continuous) and serum cotinine (continuous).

b

Adjusted for both partners’ age (continuous), serum cotinine (continuous), and annual income (< $70,000/= $70,000), and maternal parity (nulliparous/parous).

*

Bolded values indicate p-value < 0.05.

DISCUSSION

In our analysis of multiple domains of preconception stress markers and the SSR in a population-based prospective cohort, we identified evidence suggesting that paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders may influence sex allocation in offspring, resulting in an excess of male births. However, the significant association between paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders and the SSR was observed depending upon model specification or statistical methods used, possibly reflecting an uncertain association between this stress marker and the SSR. Given that the observed significant association was sensitive to the adjustment for annual income, we examined the effects of other sociodemographic factors, which may be related to this important covariate, such as health insurance and employment status. We found that health insurance (for female partners, p-value < 0.0001; for male partners, p-value = 0.03) and employment status (for female partners, p-value = 0.0009; for male partners, p-value = 0.03) were significantly associated with annual income. We also undertook sensitivity analyses where we further adjusted for these variables in the multivariate-adjusted model. We observed that results on stress markers including paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders and the SSR were similar (data not shown), although we cannot rule out the possibility of residual confounding. Besides, our findings on paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders conflict with the influential hormonal hypothesis proposed by James (8, 3739), which theorizes that a decreased testosterone production by the testes caused by paternal stress around the time of conception is associated with an excess of female births. In light of the lack of prior research findings, the association between paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders and the SSR observed in the present study needs to be corroborated through further investigation. Of note, although not significant, paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders was found to be associated with an excess of female births (adjusted RR, 0.52; 95% CI, 0.10–2.86; Table 3). Differences in the steroid metabolome have been reported between men with anxiety disorders and men with mood disorders, providing a possible explanation for the varying effects of these psychiatric disorders on the SSR (59).

In contrast to a previous population-based prospective cohort study assessing maternal preconception salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol in relation to the SSR (32), the present study did not show any significant associations between maternal preconception salivary stress markers and the SSR. Although not significant, we found that the adjusted RR of a male birth was increased for women in the third tertile of preconception salivary cortisol levels in comparison with women in the first tertile (adjusted RR, 1.22; 95% CI, 0.85–1.75; Table 3). This finding conflicts with an inverse association between maternal preconception salivary cortisol levels and the odds of a male birth (4th versus 1st quartile, adjusted OR, 0.26; 95% CI, 0.09–0.74) observed in a cohort of 130 singleton births from the Oxford Conception Study (OCS) (32). According to the hormonal hypothesis, an increased testosterone secretion from the adrenal glands caused by maternal stress around the time of conception is associated with an excess of male births (8, 3739). Furthermore, it has been proposed that high circulating glucose levels caused by maternal stress favor the development of male blastocysts rather than female blastocysts due to sex differences in the rate of glucose uptake (33, 34). However, persistent maternal stress during early pregnancy, which is recognized as a predictor for spontaneous abortion that disproportionately affects male conceptuses, may compensate for the male-biased PSR, or even result in an excess of female births (35, 36). Given that salivary cortisol is a marker for chronic stress, which represents the HPA axis activity, rather than a marker for acute stress (26), the inverse association between maternal preconception salivary cortisol levels and the odds of a male birth observed in the OCS cohort may not be considered unexpected. It is worth noting that the median salivary cortisol concentration (μg/dL) for the OCS cohort was 0.41 (interquartile range, 0.29–0.51), which was similar to that for the present study cohort (0.39; interquartile range, 0.28–0.51). However, the basal saliva samples of the two cohorts were collected at different time points (i.e., day 6 of the first observed cycle for the OCS cohort; the first day following enrollment and day 1 of the first observed cycle for the present cohort). Given the equivocal findings regarding longitudinal variations in basal cortisol secretion throughout the menstrual cycle (6062), it is challenging to know how best to model stress in the absence of validation studies. We averaged the salivary stress markers in light of few differences between the first and second measurements. Future research may identify other options for considering multiple measurements. Also of note, maternal preconception salivary cortisol levels were not found to be significantly correlated with maternal preconception salivary alpha-amylase levels or other stress markers obtained from the baseline questionnaires among female partners (correlation coefficient range, −0.04–0.10), reflecting the importance of assessing markers for multiple domains of stress (e.g., biological and psychological stress markers) and multiple pathways of stress response (e.g., the SAM system and the HPA axis).

Some previous studies have reported that psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and depression are associated with the SSR (24, 25). In a time-series analysis of Swedish data for the 276 months beginning January 1974, dispensing of anxiolytics and antidepressants, as a marker for population stress, was observed to be associated with the SSR (24). Specifically, an increase of one defined daily dose of anxiolytics or antidepressants per 1000 Swedish women predicted (a) an increase of about seven male conceptions one month prior to gestation; and (b) a loss of about five male embryos in the first month of gestation at the time of the increase. Although not statistically significant, an increase of one defined daily dose of anxiolytics or antidepressants per 1000 Swedish men predicted a decrease of about four male conceptions one month prior to gestation (24). These findings based on monthly data appear to be congruent with several existing hypotheses on the effects of parental stress on the SSR, as described above (8, 3339). As we have data on couples’ prescription medication use including psychotropic drugs, we undertook sensitivity analyses to evaluate any potential impact of prescription medication use on the SSR. However, no significant association between prescription medication use, whether it is any prescription drug use or psychotropic drug use, and the SSR was noted, possibly due to a relatively small sample size. In addition, our findings on stress markers and the SSR did not change significantly, after further adjustment for prescription medication use in the multivariate-adjusted model (data not shown). As such, we cannot rule out potential confounding by indication nor can we assume that couples complied with prescription medications. A retrospective case-control study conducted in California found that infants born to African American women diagnosed with anxiety disorders exhibited a significantly lower SSR compared to those born to African American women with other psychiatric disorders (the odds for a male birth, 0.89; 95% CI, 0.79–0.99; p-value, 0.04) or to African American women without mental health diagnoses (the odds for a male birth, 0.88; 95% CI, 0.78–1.00; p-value, 0.04) (25). However, no significant associations between anxiety disorders and a decreased SSR were observed for non-Hispanic white, Hispanic, and Asian women (25). Likewise, the present study did not show any significant associations between maternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders and the SSR. Literature argues that high reactivity to stress manifests clinically as anxiety-related symptoms (63). Individuals with anxiety disorders may exert greater autonomic and neuroendocrine responses to stressors in comparison with unaffected counterparts (63, 64). As yet, little is known about the biological mechanisms that underlie the effects of anxiety disorders on the SSR, but these mechanisms deserve more research attention.

Along with anxiety disorders, maternal stress from various sources has been reported to be associated with the SSR. In contrast to our findings on perceived stress as measured by PSS-4, previous studies have reported that stress life events (14) and psychological distress as measured by the General Health Questionnaire (15) were significantly associated with a female-biased SSR. In addition, various population stressors such as economic contraction (13), natural disasters (1719), and human-made disasters (2023) have been reported to be associated with a decrease in the SSR. With the accumulated evidence on maternal stress and the reversal of the SSR, some of these studies have examined whether the decline in the odds of a male birth resulted from an excess of male fetal loss or reduced male conceptions, suggestive of the fetal death sex ratio as a sentinel indicator of population stress reactivity (22, 23). Furthermore, subsequent research has examined whether stressful times make male fetuses less fit or male fetuses need greater fitness to avoid spontaneous abortion during stressful times, supporting that the latter mechanism is responsible for reducing the SSR (36). Still, controversy remains over underlying biological mechanisms that link stressors to spontaneous abortion. For instance, preconception salivary cortisol levels were not found to be associated with an increased risk of hCG-confirmed pregnancy loss in the OCS cohort, despite the significant association between this salivary stress marker and the SSR observed in this cohort (32). This finding indicates that the decline in the SSR may have resulted from alterations in the PSR or sex-selective losses of preimplantation embryos rather than postimplantation losses (32). Other potential mechanisms by which preconception stress alters the SSR or the PSR include reduced coitus (8, 24), sperm abnormalities (17, 65), and perturbations in the female reproductive tract (66). Of note, when we evaluated the correlation between stress markers and frequency of sexual intercourse, paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed mood disorders (correlation coefficient, −0.13; p-value = 0.04), but not anxiety disorders, was significantly correlated with frequency of sexual intercourse.

The present study is strengthened by its population-based prospective cohort design, both partners’ preconception measurements of multiple domains of stress, and the use of a couple-based analytic approach for the assessment of a couple-dependent outcome. However, our study is limited by its relatively small sample size for the detection of variability in the SSR, which should be taken into account when interpreting our results. For instance, we observed a relatively low SSR of 0.97 in comparison with the SSR observed in the U.S. general population, approximately ranging from 1.05 to 1.07 (5). This may be due to our small sample size, resulting in the estimate of the SSR with a wide 95% CI (0.75–1.26). In light of our sampling of couples planning pregnancies, our findings may not be generalizable to the general population or among couples with unplanned pregnancy. Our findings await corroboration before a more complete interpretation of the findings is possible in light of our important study limitations and uncertain external validity, if couples planning pregnancy have unique mood or stress profiles relative to unplanners. Still, the majority of births in the United States are reported planned (67). Some unique but unmeasured characteristics of our study cohort may be an explanation for a slightly higher age among men who fathered boys than among those who fathered girls noted in our study, which is contrast to existing literature (51, 54). Additionally, selection bias is a consideration, if couples with higher or lower stress levels disproportionately participated in the study. Compared with the U.S. general population, our study participants may be less likely to be stressed, coupled with the lower prevalence of psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and mood disorders (68). Given that we performed multiple statistical tests to assess the effects of multiple stress markers on the SSR, chance may be an explanation for our results. However, when we adjusted for multiple comparisons in our analysis, we found that our findings on paternal history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders remained significant (p-value > 0.05/8 [the number of stress markers examined]). Due to our inability to measure the PSR for all conceptions and the fetal death sex ratio, our findings only speak to the SSR. In addition, the lack of data on hormone levels of each partner of the couple prevents us from proving underlying mechanisms with regard to hormone reactivity to stressors. Due to fiscal and logistical concerns, we were unable to obtain repeated saliva samples across the full cycles of pregnancy attempt, preventing us from addressing possible changes in salivary stress markers over time in our analysis. The incompleteness of self-reported measures is a consideration, if couples did not feel comfortable disclosing their medical history. Given the uncertainty as to factors affecting the SSR, we cannot eliminate residual confounding or model misspecification in the interpretation of our results.

In summary, this population-based prospective cohort study suggests that paternal lifetime history of physician-diagnosed anxiety disorders may be associated with an increase in the SSR, resulting in an excess of male births. However, given the relatively small sample size, our findings require cautious interpretation and await future corroboration. Furthermore, the pathways through which paternal stress influences the SSR remain to be established. Previous research on paternal stress suggests that fathers may transmit neurobiological, metabolic, and behavioral phenotypes induced by stress to their offspring through inherited epigenetic variation (69). Meanwhile, the varying effects of maternal stress on the SSR underscore the need for a more comprehensive study which includes multiple stress markers across different reproductive stages such as preconception and early pregnancy and assesses changes in stress levels across the spectrum of gestation (34). These research efforts would provide a more complete investigation regarding the impact of parental stress on sex selection and sex-selective survival in humans.

Acknowledgments

Funding: This study was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (contracts #N01-HD-3–3355, N01-HD-3–3356, N01-HD-3–3358). JB was supported by the Korea-US Visiting Scientist Training Award (VSTA) of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (award #VFTB057303).

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest: None.

Contributors: GMBL supervised cohort recruitment and data collection. All authors designed the study. SK and RS analyzed the data. All authors interpreted the data. JB drafted the manuscript. CDL, KJS and GMBL provided critical revisions to the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Jongbloet PH. Over-ripeness ovopathy: a challenging hypothesis for sex ratio modulation. Hum Reprod. 2004;19:769–74. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deh136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Buck Louis GM, Platt RW. Reproductive and Perinatal Epidemiology. 1. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Orzack SH, Stubblefield JW, Akmaev VR, Colls P, Munné S, Scholl T, et al. The human sex ratio from conception to birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112:E2102–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416546112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Grech V. Secular trends and latitude gradients in sex ratio at birth in Asia during the past 60 years. Pediatr Int. 2013;55:219–22. doi: 10.1111/ped.12063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mathews TJ, Hamilton BE. Trend analysis of the sex ratio at birth in the United States. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2005;53:1–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Davis DL, Gottlieb MB, Stampnitzky JR. Reduced ratio of male to female births in several industrial countries: a sentinel health indicator? JAMA. 1998;279:1018–23. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.13.1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Davis DL, Webster P, Stainthorpe H, Chilton J, Jones L, Doi R. Declines in sex ratio at birth and fetal deaths in Japan, and in U.S. whites but not African Americans. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:941–6. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.James WH. Evidence that mammalian sex ratios at birth are partially controlled by parental hormone levels around the time of conception. J Endocrinol. 2008;198:3–15. doi: 10.1677/JOE-07-0446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McDonald E, Watterson A, Tyler AN, McArthur J, Scott EM. Multi-factorial influences on sex ratio: a spatio-temporal investigation of endocrine disruptor pollution and neighborhood stress. Int J Occup Environ Health. 2014;20:235–46. doi: 10.1179/2049396714Y.0000000073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V Early Stress, Translational Research and Prevention Science Network: Fetal and Neonatal Experience on Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:245–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Vrekoussis T, Kalantaridou SN, Mastorakos G, Zoumakis E, Makrigiannakis A, Syrrou M, et al. The role of stress in female reproduction and pregnancy: an update. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1205:69–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Witt WP, Litzelman K, Cheng ER, Wakeel F, Barker ES. Measuring stress before and during pregnancy: a review of population-based studies of obstetric outcome. Matern Child Health J. 2014;18:52–63. doi: 10.1007/s10995-013-1233-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Catalano RA, Bruckner T. Economic antecedents of the Swedish sex ratio. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:537–43. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hansen D, Moller H, Olsen J. Severe periconceptional life events and the sex ratio in offspring: follow-up study based on five national registers. BMJ. 1999;319:548–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7209.548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Obel C, Henriksen TB, Secher NJ, Eskenazi B, Hedegaard M. Psychological distress during early gestation and offspring sex ratio. Hum Reprod. 2007;22:3009–12. doi: 10.1093/humrep/dem274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Helle S, Helama S, Jokela J. Temperature-related birth sex ratio bias in historical Sami: warm years bring more sons. Biol Lett. 2008;4:60–2. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fukuda M, Fukuda K, Shimizu T, Møller H. Decline in sex ratio at birth after Kobe earthquake. Hum Reprod. 1998;13:2321–2. doi: 10.1093/humrep/13.8.2321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Saadat M. Decline in sex ratio at birth after Bam (Kerman Province, Southern Iran) earthquake. J Biosoc Sci. 2008;40:935–7. doi: 10.1017/s0021932008002745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Torche F, Kleinhaus K. Prenatal stress, gestational age and secondary sex ratio: the sex specific effects of exposure to a natural disaster in early pregnancy. Hum Reprod. 2012;27:558–67. doi: 10.1093/humrep/der390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zorn B, Sucur V, Stare J, Meden-Vrtovec H. Decline in sex ratio at birth after 10-day war in Slovenia: brief communication. Hum Reprod. 2002;17:3173–7. doi: 10.1093/humrep/17.12.3173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kemkes A. Secondary sex ratio variation during stressful times: the impact of the French revolutionary wars on a German parish (1787–1802) Am J Hum Biol. 2006;18:806–21. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Catalano R, Bruckner T, Gould J, Eskenazi B, Anderson E. Sex ratios in California following the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001. Hum Reprod. 2005;20:1221–7. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deh763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Catalano R, Bruckner T, Marks AR, Eskenazi B. Exogenous shocks to the human sex ratio: the case of September 11, 2001 in New York City. Hum Reprod. 2006;21:3127–31. doi: 10.1093/humrep/del283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Catalano R, Bruckner T, Hartig T, Ong M. Population stress and the Swedish sex ratio. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2005;19:413–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2005.00677.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Subbaraman MS, Goodman-Mellor SJ, Anderson ES, Lewinn KZ, Saxton KB, Shumway M, et al. An exploration of secondary sex ratios among women diagnosed with anxiety disorders. Hum Reprod. 2010;25:2084–91. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deq166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Weerth C, Buitelaar JK. Physiological stress reactivity in human pregnancy--a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:295–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Toufexis D, Rivarola MA, Lara H, Viau V. Stress and the reproductive axis. J Neuroendocrinol. 2014;26:573–86. doi: 10.1111/jne.12179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hansen AM, Garde AH, Persson R. Sources of biological and methodological variation in salivary cortisol and their impact on measurement among healthy adults: a review. Scand J Clin Lab Invest. 2008;68:448–58. doi: 10.1080/00365510701819127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nater UM, Rohleder N. Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:486–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Louis GM, Lum KJ, Sundaram R, Chen Z, Kim S, Lynch CD, et al. Stress reduces conception probabilities across the fertile window: evidence in support of relaxation. Fertil Steril. 2011;95:2184–9. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2010.06.078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lynch CD, Sundaram R, Maisog JM, Sweeney AM, Buck Louis GM. Preconception stress increases the risk of infertility: results from a couple-based prospective cohort study--the LIFE study. Hum Reprod. 2014;29:1067–75. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deu032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Chason RJ, McLain AC, Sundaram R, Chen Z, Segars JH, Pyper C, et al. Preconception stress and the secondary sex ratio: a prospective cohort study. Fertil Steril. 2012;98:937–41. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2012.06.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cameron EZ. Facultative adjustment of mammalian sex ratios in support of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: evidence for a mechanism. Proc Biol Sci. 2004;271:1723–8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cameron EZ, Lemons PR, Bateman PW, Bennett NC. Experimental alteration of litter sex ratios in a mammal. Proc Biol Sci. 2008;275:323–7. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Catalano RA, Saxton K, Bruckner T, Goldman S, Anderson E. A sex-specific test of selection in utero. J Theor Biol. 2009;257:475–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Catalano RA, Currier RJ, Steinsaltz D. Hormonal evidence of selection in utero revisited. Am J Hum Biol. 2015;27:426–31. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.James WH. Hypotheses on the stability and variation of human sex ratios at birth. J Theor Biol. 2012;310:183–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.James WH. Evolution and the variation of mammalian sex ratios at birth: reflections on Trivers and Willard (1973) J Theor Biol. 2013;334:141–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.James WH. Hypothesis: high levels of maternal adrenal androgens are a major cause of miscarriage and other forms of reproductive suboptimality. J Theor Biol. 2015;364:316–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2014.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Baischer W, Koinig G, Hartmann B, Huber J, Langer G. Hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in depressed premenopausal women: elevated blood testosterone concentrations compared to normal controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1995;20:553–9. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)00081-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lennartsson AK, Kushnir MM, Bergquist J, Billig H, Jonsdottir IH. Sex steroid levels temporarily increase in response to acute psychosocial stress in healthy men and women. Int J Psychophysiol. 2012;84:246–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Buck Louis GM, Schisterman EF, Sweeney AM, Wilcosky TC, Gore-Langton RE, Lynch CD, et al. Designing prospective cohort studies for assessing reproductive and developmental toxicity during sensitive windows of human reproduction and development -- the LIFE Study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2011;25:413–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2011.01205.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bernert JT, Jr, Turner WE, Pirkle JL, Sosnoff CS, Akins JR, Waldrep MK, et al. Development and validation of sensitive method for determination of serum cotinine in smokers and nonsmokers by liquid chromatography/atmospheric pressure ionization tandem mass spectrometry. Clin Chem. 1997;43:2281–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, el-Sheikh M, Gordis EB, Stroud LR. Salivary alpha-amylase in biobehavioral research: recent developments and applications. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1098:122–44. doi: 10.1196/annals.1384.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Raff H, Homar PJ, Skoner DP. New enzyme immunoassay for salivary cortisol. Clin Chem. 2003;49:203–4. doi: 10.1373/49.1.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Zou G. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159:702–6. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Adil MM, Khan UA. Offspring sex ratio in smokers. J Pak Med Assoc. 2004;54:442–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Biggar RJ, Wohlfahrt J, Westergaard T, Melbye M. Sex ratios, family size, and birth order. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;150:957–62. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a010104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Beratis NG, Asimacopoulou A, Varvarigou A. Association of secondary sex ratio with smoking and parity. Fertil Steril. 2008;89:662–7. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2007.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Chahnazarian A. Determinants of the sex ratio at birth: review of recent literature. Soc Biol. 1988;35:214–35. doi: 10.1080/19485565.1988.9988703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Fukuda M, Fukuda K, Shimizu T, Andersen CY, Byskov AG. Parental periconceptional smoking and male: female ratio of newborn infants. Lancet. 2002;359:1407–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08362-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ibrahim MM, Khalil AA, Khan UA. Offspring sex ratios among male tobacco smokers in Khartoum, Sudan. J Pak Med Assoc. 2012;62:1045–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Jacobsen R, Møller H, Mouritsen A. Natural variation in the human sex ratio. Hum Reprod. 1999;14:3120–5. doi: 10.1093/humrep/14.12.3120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Juntunen KS, Kvist AP, Kauppila AJ. A shift from a male to a female majority in newborns with the increasing age of grand grand multiparous women. Hum Reprod. 1997;12:2321–3. doi: 10.1093/humrep/12.10.2321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kolk M, Schnettler Socioeconomic status and sex ratios at birth in Sweden: No evidence for a Trivers-Willard effect for a wide range of status indicators. Am J Hum Biol. 2016;28:67–73. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Koshy G, Delpisheh A, Brabin L, Attia E, Brabin BJ. Parental smoking and increased likelihood of female births. Ann Hum Biol. 2010;37:789–800. doi: 10.3109/03014461003742803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ruckstuhl KE, Colijn GP, Amiot V, Vinish E. Mother’s occupation and sex ratio at birth. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:269. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dušková M, Hill M, Bičíková M, Šrámková M, Řípová D, Mohr P, et al. The steroid metabolome in men with mood and anxiety disorders. Physiol Res. 2015;64:S275–82. doi: 10.33549/physiolres.933067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kajantie E, Phillips DI. The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:151–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lee YH, Kim YY, Chang JY, Kho HS. Changes in oral mucosal MUC1 expression and salivary hormones throughout the menstrual cycle. Oral Dis. 2015;21:962–8. doi: 10.1111/odi.12367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Nepomnaschy PA, Altman RM, Watterson R, Co C, McConnell DS, England BG. Is cortisol excretion independent of menstrual cycle day? A longitudinal evaluation of first morning urinary specimens. PLoS One. 2011;6:e18242. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dieleman GC, Huizink AC, Tulen JH, Utens EM, Tiemeier H. Stress reactivity predicts symptom improvement in children with anxiety disorders. J Affect Disord. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.022. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Bakker MJ, Tijssen MA, van der Meer JN, Koelman JH, Boer F. Increased whole-body auditory startle reflex and autonomic reactivity in children with anxiety disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:314–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Bae J, Kim S, Chen Z, Eisenberg ML, Buck Louis GM. Human semen quality and the secondary sex ratio. Asian J Androl. doi: 10.4103/1008-682X.173445. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Grant VJ, Irwin RJ. A simple model for adaptive variation in the sex ratios of mammalian offspring. J Theor Biol. 2009;258:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Mumford SL, Sapra KJ, King RB, Louis JF, Buck Louis GM. Pregnancy intentions-a complex construct and call for new measures. Fertil Steril. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2016.07.1067. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Braun K, Champagne FA. Paternal influences on offspring development: behavioural and epigenetic pathways. J Neuroendocrinol. 2014;26:697–706. doi: 10.1111/jne.12174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES