Abstract
Hemodynamic-based brain imaging techniques are typically incapable of monitoring brain activity with both high spatial and high temporal resolutions. In this study, we have used intrinsic signal optical imaging (ISOI), a relatively high spatial resolution imaging technique, to examine the temporal resolution of the hemodynamic signal. We imaged V1 responses in anesthetized monkey to a moving light spot. Movies of cortical responses clearly revealed a focus of hemodynamic response traveling across the cortical surface. Importantly, at different locations along the cortical trajectory, response timecourses maintained a similar tri-phasic shape and shifted sequentially across cortex with a predictable delay. We calculated the time between distinguishable timecourses and found that the temporal resolution of the signal at which two events can be reliably distinguished is about 80 milliseconds. These results suggest that hemodynamic-based imaging is suitable for detecting ongoing cortical events at high spatial resolution and with temporal resolution relevant for behavioral studies.
Keywords: temporal resolution, intrinsic signal optical imaging, visual cortex, monkey, V1
Introduction
Monitoring cortical events with both high spatial and high temporal resolution is crucially important for studying brain function. Most recording and imaging techniques, however, cannot meet both requirements simultaneously. The intrinsic signal optical imaging (ISOI) technique, which measures cortical reflectance change due to hemodynamic response (Grinvald et al. 1986), has high spatial resolution (on the order of 100 μm). Its temporal resolution, however, is believed to be very slow, taking 1-2 seconds to peak, followed by a long (up to 10 seconds) return to baseline. Because of this, ISOI is not considered useful for studying fast cortical events.
Our premise in this study is that extended signal timecourses and long delays to peak do not necessarily imply low temporal precision. In fact, we argue that if used properly, significant temporal information can be inferred from the response. Simply put, if two events occur at two different cortical locations with a small onset-time difference, and if the induced response timecourses are similar at the two sites, then the two responses should reach their peak with a similarly small time difference. This would happen regardless of a long delay to peak. Thus the temporal relationship between the two events, even if it is quite small, is faithfully relayed, thereby resulting in a high temporal resolution of event onsets. The limits of detecting such differences can also be determined. Recent fMRI studies have used this rationale to demonstrate that cortical responses can be tracked with high temporal precision using hemodynamic signals. Thus, event-related fMRI has been used not only for brain mapping, but also for tracing the sequence of cortical activation across brain regions during task performance (see review Formisano & Goebel 2003). However, because the fMRI scanning rate is normally low and the spatial resolution of fMRI signal is on the order of 1 mm, both pose serious limitations on its application for high-spatial, high-temporal resolution brain imaging. In contrast, ISOI has a much higher spatial resolution, with almost unlimited scanning rate, making this approach potentially more suitable for such goals.
Similarly, precise temporal resolution can also be achieved in another hemodynamic-based optical technique, fNIRS (Ferrari & Quaresima 2012; Scholkmann et al. 2014; Gratton & Fabiani 2001). fNIRS is a non-invasive optical measurement of brain activity. It uses longer wavelength (650 - 1000 nm) for better penetration. Due to the influence of skull and scalp, the signal noise ratio and spatial resolution of fNIRS (in the order of 1cm) is poorer than ISOI. However, the temporal aspects of the origin of these two signals are similar and thus the rational of the precise temporal measurement discussed above still holds. The most important advantage of fNIRS is that it is non-invasive, and thus can be used in human subjects.
Reliability of signal timecourse across different cortical locations is important for this approach. Similar to the signal sources in fMRI and fNIRS, the signal measured in ISOI mainly reflects the overall changes in blood due to neural activation. There are three main sources (blood volume, blood oxygenation, light scattering) that contribute to the overall light reflectance change in ISOI. The amplitude and temporal dynamics of this signal depends on the wavelength of the illumination light and specific stimulus parameters. A large set of studies have shown that, for red light illumination (e.g. 605nm or 630nm) the intrinsic signal has a stereotypical response timecourse characterized by an early phase (the initial dip) followed by a large rebound (late positive signal, known as the BOLD in fMRI). Such signals have been observed in both anesthetized (e.g. Grinvald et al. 1990, Chen-Bee et al. 2007) and awake preparations (e.g. Vanzetta et al. 2004, Sirotin et al. 2009; Tanigawa et al 2010).
ISOI signal can also be broken down into global and local response components. The global signal is stimulus-non-specific; the local signal is feature-specific and is confined to functionally specific cortical columns. Many studies have characterized the global signal in response to a point stimulus, including its tri-phasic nature, its point-spread function and the possible underlying hemodynamic components (Chen-Bee et. al. 2007, Sirotin et al. 2009). Temporal features for the local (mapping) signal, however, are less well-understood. In this study we imaged cortical response to moving spot stimuli presented to one eye. We analyzed eye-specific activation (mapping signal) in a 10-second long period after stimulus onset. We found the presence of functionally specific ocular-dominance (OD) signals in all three phases of the hemodynamic responses. This signal appears independent of the global signal, as it does not change its sign with the global signal and lasts longer than the global signal. The timecourses of cortical activation are also similar at different locations. By calculating the timecourse shift along the cortical activation trace, we estimate the temporal resolution limit of ISOI is about 80ms.
Materials and Methods
All surgical and experimental procedures conformed to the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (Institute of Neuroscience, Chinese Academy of Sciences; Vanderbilt University, USA).
Intrinsic Optical Imaging
Two Macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta) were used in this study. Monkeys were anesthetized (thiopental sodium, 1–2 mg/kg/hr intravenously [i.v.] and/or isoflurane, 0.2%–1.5%), paralyzed with vecuronium bromide (0.05 mg/kg/hr i.v.), and artificially ventilated. Anesthetic depth was assessed continuously via implanted wire electroencephalographic electrodes, end-tidal CO2, oximetry, and heart rate, and by regular testing for response to toe pinch while monitoring heart rate changes. A craniotomy and durotomy were performed to expose visual areas V1 and V2 (near the lunate sulcus at an eccentricity of 1-6 degrees from the fovea). Eyes were dilated (atropine sulfate) and fit with contact lenses of appropriate curvature to focus on a computer screen 57 or 76 cm from the eyes. Risley prisms were placed over each eye and eyes were aligned using a rapid retinotopic imaging method (Lu et al., 2009). Alignment was checked before and after each imaging block. The brain was stabilized with agar and images were obtained through a cover glass.
Images of reflectance change (intrinsic hemodynamic signals) corresponding to local cortical activity were acquired with 632 nm illumination (for details, see Roe and Ts’o, 1995; Lu and Roe, 2007). Signal-to-noise ratio was enhanced by trial averaging (usually 20-50 trials per stimulus condition). Frame size was 504 × 504 pixels and represented 20 mm × 20 mm of cortical area. Stimuli were presented in blocks. Each block contained four to nine stimulus conditions presented in a randomly interleaved fashion.
Visual stimulus
Visual stimuli were created using VSG 2/5 or ViSaGe (Cambridge Research Systems Ltd., Rochester, UK) and presented on a CRT monitor (SONY GDM F500R or CPD-G520). The stimulus screen was gamma corrected. Full screen drifting squarewave gratings were used to obtain basic functional maps including those for ocular dominance, orientation, and color. Spatial and temporal frequencies were optimized for these maps (see Lu and Roe, 2007).
Retinotopic Grid stimulus
Stationary bar stimuli containing orthogonal squarewave gratings were used for mapping the retinotopy of the imaged V1. Full screen stationary squarewave gratings (SF=1 cycle/deg, duty cycle 0.1) were presented monocularly at two different locations (phase shifted 90 degrees) for 3.5 seconds each after a 0.5-second delay. Subtraction of these two phase-shifted gratings resulted in a retinotopic map of horizontal or vertical grids (See Figure 1E & F).
Moving spot stimulus
A white spot (luminance 60 cd/m2) was displayed on a black background traveling vertically downward for 4.5 degrees parallel to the vertical meridian. Two traveling speeds (1.5 and 4.5 deg/sec) were tested. Spot size was 0.23 degrees and was viewed monocularly. Optical signal was collected for 10 seconds at 8 Hz, resulting in a total of 80 frames. During the imaging, the first 0.5 seconds of imaging was collected with no stimulus on the screen for background activity. A spot appeared at time 0.5 second and started to move immediately. The spot disappeared at time 3.5 second (for 1.5 deg/sec speed) or 1.5 second (for 4.5 deg/sec speed), with a traveling duration of 3 or 1 seconds, respectively. Each condition was imaged 20 times and results shown are the average of these 20 trials.
Data Analysis
To visualize the cortical response to moving spot, we constructed “single-frame maps”. For each frame, the gray value of each pixel was calculated using the following function: dR/R = (Fx - F0)/F0, in which dR/R represents percent change, F0 is the average raw reflectance value of the first four frames (taken before stimulus onset and thus representative of the baseline activity), and Fx is the raw reflectance values of the frame under study. Single-frame maps obtained in this way represent the percent intrinsic signal change relative to the initial (pre-stimulus) baseline. Multiple repeated trials were averaged to improve signal-noise ratio.
Cortical functional maps (e.g. Figure 1D, E, F) were calculated with a standard subtraction method (i.e. difference map, Lu & Roe 2007). Unless otherwise specified, difference maps were clipped at two standard deviations on each side of the median pixel values for display.
The response timecourse is a measurement of how the signal changes over time. Reflectance change values (dR/R) were first calculated for each pixel and for each frame. Then pixels within a region of interest (e.g. color regions in Figure 2B) were averaged and plotted against time (e.g. Figure 2C). No additional filtering was used. To calculate the peak time of each timecourse, a cosine function was fitted to the initial dip and positive rebound and the peak time was calculated from the fitting function.
Results
Two macaque monkeys (two hemispheres) were imaged in this study. For each hemisphere, we first obtained basic functional maps of the cortical regions under investigation. These maps include ocular dominance (e.g. Figure 1D), orientation, color selectivity, motion direction etc. Some functional maps of one case have been previously published (Lu et al. 2010 Figure 1).
Mapping Retinotopy in V1
Figure 1 illustrates our mapping procedure. For each imaging field of view (approximate location on brain shown in Figure 1B, imaged cortical region shown in Figure 1C), we first demarcate the extent of V1 by identifying the V1/V2 border by ocular dominance mapping (Figure 1D); this also demarcates the representation of the vertical meridian. Each mapping session began by projecting the optic disk locations onto the stimulus screen to obtain an estimate of the foveal location on the screen (star in Figure 1A). This foveal estimate then guided the placement of initial spot stimuli used for mapping. We then determined the approximate extent of visual field in the exposed cortical region by using a rapid spot imaging method (Lu et al., 2009). This was determined separately for each eye.
Having more precisely determined the representation within the field of view, we were then able to precisely place grid lines for detailed retinotopic mapping. We imaged cortical response to stationary horizontal and vertical grids through each eye presented in the lower contralateral visual field (Figure 1A, c.f. Tootell et al. 1988 Figure 7, Blasdel and Campbell 2001, Shmuel et al 2005): Each line in the grid is 0.1 degree wide and spaced at 1 degree (i.e. 1 cycle/deg gratings). Cortical responses to two phase shifted grid stimuli are subtracted to produce the images shown in Figure 1E &F. Figure 1E is a V1 representation of horizontal grids presented to left eye (right eye is covered). For this left hemisphere field of view, foveal representation is toward the left (more lateral). Consistent with this, cortical magnification is larger towards the left than towards the right of the image (larger spacing). Similarly, Figure 1E, obtained in response to a vertical grid, reveals larger spacing between lines on the left than on the right side of the image. In both Figures 1E & F, there are approximately 5-6 activation lines in the imaging area, indicating a representation extent of at least 4-5 degrees of visual space. [Note also the patchiness evident within the lines of activation; these are horizontal (1E) and vertical (1F) orientation domains, also revealed with full field orientation maps.]
A summary map of the overall representation is illustrated in Figure 1C (red lines: horizontal grid, green lines: vertical grid) overlain on the surface blood vessel map. An extrapolated map on the monkey brain is shown in Figure 1B, and the extent of the visual field within the chamber is shown in Figure 1A (gray area, estimated from above analysis). In combination with results from spot imaging (not shown), these retinotopic maps provided accurate visual spatial representation in V1.
Tri-phasic response to a moving spot
We illustrate an example of the spatiotemporal progression of cortical response to a moving spot (Figure 2). We imaged cortical responses to a spot moving vertically downward in visual space parallel and close to the vertical meridian. Thus, the activation is expected to move from left to right in the field of view parallel to the V1/V2 border (foveal representation is to left out of field of view). The stimulus was a white spot (0.23 × 0.23 degree square) moving on a black background at a fixed speed of 1.5 degree/second and was viewed monocularly through the left eye. Cortical activation was imaged at 8Hz frame rate for 10 seconds. A total of 20 repeats (trials) were averaged. See Movie in supplemental materials (Video S1) as well as on web at http://www.psy.vanderbilt.edu/faculty/roeaw/spot/ss2_movie-25101.gif. Figure 2A contains representative frames with time stamps shown at top left corners. Each frame is a dR/R frame representing changes from pre-stimulus baseline (see Method). For the first 5 images (top row), the actual locations of the spot stimulus are estimated (via retinotopic mapping procedure described in Figure 1) and illustrated on the images as small white circles. The spot appeared at 0.5 second and disappeared at 3.5 second, and thus the white circle is not plotted in the last 5 frames.
Tri-phasic responses are evident in these maps. In the first phase, during what would correspond to the initial dip period (0-2 second post stimulus onset), a darkening appears. Note the spot first appeared outside the field of view. The spatial distance between this darkening region and the actual stimulus location (white circle) represents the temporal delay between the hemodynamic signal and stimulus onset (about 1-2 second). Importantly, within this darkening region are evident ocular dominance (left eye) columns (e.g. red arrow in the 2.3-second frame).
In the second phase (approx. 3-6 sec), we observe a large patch of whitening which extends across a large mediolateral spatial extent. This whitening is the positive rebound of the hemodynamic signal presumably associated with the positive BOLD. Note that this whitening signal is closely associated with the large vasculature. During this phase, ocular dominance columns, although partially obscured by the large positive signal, are still present.
In the third phase (approx. 7-10 sec), we observe an undershoot which lingers long after the stimulus disappears. In this stimulus condition, the stimulus disappears at 3.5 second. The last frame in Figure 2A (time 10 sec) which still contains the undershoot is imaged 6.5 second after the stimulus offset. Ocular dominance columns remain evident during this third phase.
By observing the entire 10-second cortical activation sequence, we note that at any given time, different regions along the trace contain different phases of this spatiotemporal activation pattern. Due to temporal smearing, cortical activation is elongated along the spot trajectory. These activation maps reveal important points. First, if one observes the darkening signal, there is clearly ocular dominance structure, indicating the negative signal is spatially specific. However, if one observes the whitening signal, it is, in contrast, highly vascular, with less obvious ocular dominance structure and appears less spatially specific. Thus, the darkening and whitening components of the signal appear to have different spatial specificities. Second, it is evident that the functionally specific signal is long-lasting. Visually, left-eye-specific domains (black vertical stripes) are most obvious in the initial dip and late undershoot periods (e.g. at 1.6 and 7.5 seconds) and less clear, but still present, during the period of positive rebound.
To analyze the timecourse of the response in detail, we selected an oval region located on the path of the traveling spot. Figure 2B shows the ocular dominance map and the oval region selected. The size of this region is close to the size of the V1 response to a stationary spot of 0.25 deg in size (see Lu et. al. 2009). To improve the signal/noise ratio, pixels belonging to the left eye columns (blue) and the right eye columns (red) are averaged to generate two timecourses. The time-dependent changes of pixel values (dR/R) are plotted in Figure 2C. The overall response (global signal) from both stimulated eye domains (blue curve) and non-stimulated-eye domains (red curve) shows a clear tri-phasic pattern: a negative initial dip (0.5-2.5sec), a large positive rebound (2.5-6.5sec), and a late negative undershoot (6.5-10sec).
Although only one eye is stimulated, the differences between these two curves are small, indicating the majority of the signal is stimulus non-specific. This is likely caused both by the fact that the majority of V1 neurons are binocular and by the spatial non-specificity of the underlying hemodynamic signal. Both of these two factors contribute to the common part of the two curves (so called global signal). Interestingly, the blue curve (representing the stimulated eye) is always below the red curve (non-stimulated eye), indicating that the eye-specific signal is always negative. The differential curve in the bottom panel in Figure 2C also clearly shows that, throughout this 10 second imaging period, the left eye activation is consistently more negative than the right-eye activation, and this relationship remains even after the overall reflectance has returned to the baseline. Thus, importantly, the domain-specific signal is always negative (Fig 2C bottom, cf. Moon et al 2007).
Temporal resolution
To study the temporal resolution of cortical activation, we compared the spatiotemporal profile of spots moving at two different speeds. Figure 3 illustrates movie frames of a faster moving spot (4.5 deg/sec). The 5 frames in the top row in Figure 3A represent events occurring within the initial 2 seconds. Cortical response to the spot is strong even though the exposure of each cortical location to the stimulus is quite brief (~0.2 second). Compared with the spot traveling at lower speed (Fig 2), both the initial dip (e.g. at 1.8 second) and the positive rebound (e.g. at 2.8 second) show a more elongated response pattern along the path. This greater elongation is due to the fact that the spot traveled a longer distance in each frame (about 0.56 degrees each frame for the speed of 4.5 degree/sec, compared with 0.19 degrees each frame for 1.5 degree/second). Although different in speeds, the temporal delay between initial dip and positive rebound is similar for the fast and slow moving spots (~2 seconds). In the fast moving spot condition, this temporal separation should result in less interference (spatial overlap) between the initial dip signal and the positive rebound (schematically illustrated as black and gray, respectively, in Figure 3B). Such interference between initial dip and rebound will shift the peak locations both spatially and temporally and lead to inaccuracy of spatial and temporal judgments. The average timecourses of the fast moving spot are shown in Figure 3C (averaged from 20 trials) using the same plotting convention as in Figure 2C. Due to shorter activation time, cortical responses to the faster moving stimulus are smaller in amplitude and last for a briefer period of time. However, the same tri-phasic response timecourses are still evident. Since images obtained from faster spot contain less spatial interference between different temporal components, response from the faster spot may provide more accurate temporal resolution estimates.
To assess temporal resolution, we selected a 30 × 400 pixel rectangular region over the trajectory of the moving dot (yellow outline in Figure 4A). Two sets of representative timecourses, averaged from the pixels under the green lines in Figure 4A, are shown for both fast (Figure 4B) and slow (Figure 4C) dot conditions. (Note that, unlike Figure 2C and 3C, pixels from both eye domains were averaged.) It is clear that normalized timecourses from different cortical regions show similar tri-phasic pattern, and are shifted along the time axis. The temporal activation profile at each location is constant across cortex and serves as a fundamental template for hemodynamic cortical response.
For a pair of lines that are a large distance apart (e.g. any pair of lines in Figure 4A), it is clear that their temporal timecourses are separable (Figure 4C). We then calculated how close two timecourses need to be to make them statistically inseparable. This minimum detectable shift then reflects the temporal resolution for sequential events occurring across cortex surface. By selecting any two lines within the trajectory rectangle, we obtained two sets of pixels, each has a trial-averaged timecourses. We then calculated the rebound peak locations for each of these timecourses. The separability of these two groups of peak times was evaluated with a t-test (p<0.05, one-tailed). For each temporal separation, we evaluated many (~200) such pairs along the trajectory. The probability of separating two pairs is increased when two locations are further apart. Figure 4D&E shows the percentage (Y axis) of the pairs having the same distance (X axis) that are statistically separable. For fast moving dots (Figure 4D), over 95% of pairs are separable at 80ms time difference. For slow moving dots (Figure 4E), this value is larger (110ms), probably due to interference between different phases of the initial dip and rebound signals.
Discussion
In this study we demonstrate that the temporal structure of intrinsic optical responses to a moving spot is very similar at different time points (and cortical locations) in V1. This permits the extraction of stimulus timing information by comparing temporal delays. The hemodynamic-based imaging technique, therefore, can be used in certain studies where high (~100 ms) temporal resolution is needed. We also demonstrate that domain-specific signals in intrinsic optical responses are persistently negative (Figures 2C and 3C).
Temporal precision of the intrinsic signal
Imaging cortical response to a moving object has been successfully achieved with voltage sensitive dye methods (Jancke et al. 2004). In their study, cortical waves induced by a moving spot were observed in cat visual cortex. Imaging cortical waves based on intrinsic signals, however, has not yet been demonstrated. Although the intrinsic optical signal has a long delay and takes a long time to return to baseline, its temporal structure is rather fixed for similar stimuli at different locations. This phenomenon has been shown in previous studies using stationary stimuli (Grinvald et al. 1990, Chen-Bee et al. 2007). It means that useful timing information can be extracted from the onset latencies and peak latencies. In this study, we showed that movies of cortical response faithfully represent a fast moving object in the visual space. Furthermore, the traveling speed of cortical response can be calculated with high precision. For spots moving at relatively low speeds (1.5 deg/sec), it is very likely that the first two phases of response (which are opposite in sign) mix to a certain degree (see Figure 2A at 2.3 sec). Our interpretation is that the late coming positive rebound drives the overall signal to more positive values, making the apparent peak time of the initial dip peak earlier than it otherwise would be. It has been shown that when use shorter wavelength (e.g. 530nm) as illumination light in ISOI imaging, the responses are always negative (Sirotin et al. 2009), and interactions between the first and second phases of response can be avoided. We predict that a better temporal precision can be achieved when a shorter wavelength is used.
The temporal precision measured in the present study is from an anesthetized preparation. In awake conditions, the signal strength will increase while noise also increases (Roe 2007), how they affect the temporal precision measurement is not clear. However, under anesthetized condition, visual cortex is mainly driven by bottom-up (stimulus-driven) activity. In awake conditions, there might be contamination from top-down signals (e.g. anticipation, see Sirotin & Das 2009.) which add additional variables to the signals and the temporal measurement maybe less accurate. The temporal dynamics of these top-down related variables are themselves interesting topics and can be studied with ISOI (see discussion below).
Potential applications and significance
The advantage of the intrinsic signal over voltage sensitive dye signal is that it involves no staining with dyes. Given its potential for tracking fast changing events, ISOI can be used not only to image response to a moving object, but also for other types of cortical dynamics. The temporal resolution achieved in this study (80ms) can be further improved by including more pixels into comparison samples (the green lines in Figure 4A). Furthermore, all hemodynamic signal based imaging, including fMRI and fNIRS, can use the same methodology to achieve high temporal resolution. With enough repeats and pixels (or voxels), we predict that even higher resolution should be achievable, or less repeats are required. For example, by averaging many eye-specific pixels in V1, a recent study in anesthetized monkey V1 has demonstrated that on-going dynamics of eye-specific signals can be measured in single-trial ISOI imaging (Xu et al. 2016).
However, some precautions must be taken when applying this method. Importantly, to reliably calculate temporal offsets between two cortical locations, the shapes of their signal timecourses must be the same. That is, the assumption that the same hemodynamic response is obtained at different cortical locations must hold. In the present study, we used the same stimuli (moving dot) in a relatively homogenous cortical area (V1), thus stereotypical response timecourses were obtained for the delay calculation. When different types of stimuli are used, or the spatial separation is large (or between two different areas), the differences between their timecourse shapes must be considered. Alternatively, one can study the factors that modulates the temporal shape of the timecourse, for example, by measuring the activity in the same cortical regions during different behavioral tasks.
After these limitations are considered, ISOI/fMRI/fNIRS can be appropriate for studying perceptual events such as filling-in, tracking patterns of cortical motor response, and behavioral and cognitive studies such as attentional modulation (e.g. attend vs. ignore). Potential for use in studying functional connections based on the difference in feed forward and feedback timing is also on the horizon (Ahmed et al. 2008).
Tri-phasic response
Previous studies have shown multiple phases in intrinsic optical imaging. Chen-Bee et al. (2007) studied rat barrel cortex activation in response to a one-second whisker stimulation with 635 nm illumination. They found three phases in the signals: an initial dip, then a positive overshoot, followed by a negative undershoot. In awake fixating monkeys, Sirotin et al. (2009) demonstrate tri-phasic response in V1 under 605 and 630 nm illumination, but not for 530 nm. Here we also found three such phases in the moving spot activation. The similarity between these two studies indicates the common cortical hemodynamic activity in response to sensory stimulation. Consistent with previous findings (e.g. Figure 1D in Vanzetta et al. 2004), we show that domain-specific signal (the ocular dominance signal) is constantly negative and lasts through the three phases (initial dip – rebound – undershoot) of the global signal. This also means that the subtraction map does not change its sign even though the sign of the original signal (dR/R) reversed twice during the tri-phasic period. Furthermore, the lingering domain-specific signal in the late undershoot is interesting, as it remains long after the stimulus has disappeared. Although it is possible that this late response is due to an associated “afterimage” (due to the high contrast and small size of the stimulus), studies of rat barrel cortex suggest that such long lasting signal may not be related to neuronal activity (Chen-Bee et al. 2007). By decomposing the underlying hemodynamic components of the intrinsic signal using different wavelengths of illumination, Sirotin et al. (2009) suggests that this late darkening is dominated by slowly decaying of blood volume (i.e. total hemoglobin), rather than a late increase in deoxyhemoglobin. However, how such a signal remains so local (domain-specific) for so long is still not clear.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Intrinsic signal optical imaging (ISOI) movies demonstrate tracking of fast cortical events.
Temporal resolution of reliably distinguishing two events with ISOI is about 80ms.
ISOI signal at each location of moving spot trajectory has a tri-phasic timecourse.
Unlike the global signal, the domain-specific signal is negative in all 3 phases.
Acknowledgements
We thank Lisa Chu for surgical assistance and Jie Lu for imaging processing assistance. This work was supported by NIH grant EY11744 to AWR; National Natural Science Foundation of China (31371111, 31530029, 31625012) to HDL; National Natural Science Foundation of China (31471052), Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2015QN81007), Zhejiang Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China (LR15C090001) to GC; and Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, and Vanderbilt University Center for Integrative & Cognitive Neuroscience.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ahmed B, Hanazawa A, Undeman C, Eriksson D, Valentiniene S, Roland PE. Cortical dynamics subserving visual apparent motion. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2796–2810. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel G, Campbell D. Funcrtional retinotopy of monkey visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8286–301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08286.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen-Bee CH, Agoncillo T, Xiong Y, Frostig RD. The triphasic intrinsic signal: implications for functional imaging. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4572–4586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0326-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari M, Quaresima V. A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage. 2012;63(2):921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Formisano E, Goebel R. Tracking cognitive processes with functional MRI mental chronometry. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:174–81. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00044-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton G, Fabiani M. Shedding light on brain function: the event-related optical signal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(8):357–363. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01701-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinvald A, Lieke E, Frostig RD, Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Functional architecture of cortex revealed by optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Nature. 1986;324:361–364. doi: 10.1038/324361a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frostig RD, Lieke EE, Ts'o DY, Grinvald A. Cortical functional architecture and local coupling between neuronal activity and the microcirculation revealed by in vivo high-resolution optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1990;87:6082–6086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.16.6082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jancke D, Chavane F, Naaman S, Grinvald A. Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex. Nature. 2004;428:423–426. doi: 10.1038/nature02396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalatsky VA, Stryker MP. New paradigm for optical imaging: temporally encoded maps of intrinsic signal. Neuron. 2003;38:529–545. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00286-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu HD, Roe AW. Optical imaging of contrast response in Macaque monkey V1 & V2. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:2675–2695. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu HD, Chen G, Ts'o DY, Roe AW. A rapid topographic mapping and eye alignment method using optical imaging in Macaque visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2009;44:636–646. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu HD, Roe AW. Functional organization of color domains in V1 and V2 of Macaque monkey revealed by optical imaging. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:516–533. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu HD, Chen G, Tanigawa H, Roe AW. A Motion Direction Map in Macaque V2. Neuron. 2010;68:1002–1013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moon C-H, Fukuda M, Park S-H, Kim S-G. Neural interpretation of blood oxygenation level-dependent fMRI maps at submillimeter columnar resolution. J Neurosci. 2007;27:6892–6902. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0445-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tootell RB, Switkes E, Silverman MS, Hamilton SL. Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. II. Retinotopic organization. J Neurosci. 1988;8:1531–1568. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-05-01531.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roe AW. Long-term optical imaging of intrinsic signals in anesthetized and awake monkeys. Applied Optics. 2007;46(10):1872–1880. doi: 10.1364/ao.46.001872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roe AW, Ts'o DY. Visual topography in primate V2: multiple representation across functional stripes. J Neurosci. 1995;15:3689–3715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-03689.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholkmann F, Kleiser S, Metz AJ, Zimmermann R, Mata Pavia J, Wolf U, Wolf M. A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. Neuroimage. 2014;85(1):6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shmuel A, Korman M, Sterkin A, Harel M, Ullman S, Malach R, Grinvald A. Retinotopic axis specificity and selective clustering of feedback projections from V2 to V1 in the owl monkey. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2117–2131. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4137-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirotin YB, Das A. Anticipatory haemodynamic signals in sensory cortex not predicted by local neuronal activity. Nature. 2009;457(7228):475–479. doi: 10.1038/nature07664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirotin YB, Hillman EM, Bordier C, Das A. Spatiotemporal precision and hemodynamic mechanism of optical point spreads in alert primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:18390–18395. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905509106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanigawa H, Lu HD, Roe AW. Functional organization for color and orientation in macaque V4. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:1542–1548. doi: 10.1038/nn.2676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanzetta I, Slovin H, Omer DB, Grinvald A. Columnar resolution of blood volume and oximetry functional maps in the behaving monkey; implications for FMRI. Neuron. 2004;42:843–854. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu H, Han C, Chen M, Li P, Zhu S, Fang Y, Hu J, Ma H, Lu HD. Rivalry-like neural activity in primary visual cortex in anesthetized monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:3231–3242. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3660-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.