Abstract
Primate face processing depends on a distributed network of interlinked face-selective areas composed of face-selective neurons. In both humans and macaques, the network is divided into a ventral stream and a dorsal stream, and the functional similarities of the areas in humans and macaques indicate they are homologous. Neural correlates for face detection, holistic processing, face space, and other key properties of human face processing have been identified at the single neuron level, and studies providing causal evidence have firmly established that face-selective brain areas are central to face processing. These mechanisms give rise to our highly accurate familiar face recognition but also to our error prone performance with unfamiliar faces. This limitation of the face system has important implications for consequential situations such as eyewitness identification and policing.
Keywords: Face Recognition, Functional Brain Organization, Neural Mechanisms of Behavior, Social Brain Function
Face recognition, from a computational point of view, is a daunting task. But for most of us detecting a face in a scene, recognizing a friend, and noticing even minute changes in facial expression, is effortless. How is that possible? The primate brain contains specialized cells and circuitry that support face recognition, and some of the computational mechanisms these circuits implement are now becoming clear. Here, we provide a comprehensive overview of primate face processing systems from single cells to circuits and their link to behavior. We highlight the surprising limitations of unfamiliar face recognition and discuss its implications for real-world situations such as eyewitness testimony.
Neural Systems for Face-Processing
Charles Gross’ discovery of cells selective for faces provided first evidence consistent with earlier ideas about single-unit coding of person identity, which Lettvin had famously referred to as grandmother cells (Gross 2002). Gross’ finding called for an explanation: how can such complex shape-selectivity arise from simpler representations in early visual cortex (Hubel 1982)? The natural answer was, and has remained, hierarchies that transform visual information through multiple levels of processing (LeCun & Bengio 1995, Riesenhuber & Poggio 1999). What the discovery of face cells did for the concept of hierarchy, the discovery of entire face areas by functional MRI (Kanwisher et al 1997, McCarthy et al 1997) did for the concept of modularity: it reinvigorated debates on local versus distributed information processing (Haxby et al 2001, Kanwisher 2000). Face-processing systems in primates, as we understand them today, instantiate multiple organizational features, including some that had been thought of as mutually exclusive: face-processing systems are modular and distributed and face-processing appears to proceed in parallel and through hierarchies.
Distributed and Modular Organization
Face cells have been found across temporal (Perrett et al 1992) and prefrontal cortex (Ó Scalaidhe et al 1997, Rolls et al 2006) of the macaque monkey brain, suggesting that faces are represented in a distributed fashion. Local clustering of face cells (Perrett et al 1984) suggested an organization of columns (Fujita et al 1992). Later neuroimaging studies revealed a large-scale organizational feature, face patches several millimeters in diameter (Pinsk et al 2009, Tsao et al 2003, Tsao et al 2008a). These patches are primarily located laterally in the lower bank of the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and medio-dorsally in the fundus (see Figure 1A). Face patches contain very high fractions of face-selective neurons (Freiwald & Tsao 2010, Issa et al 2013, Tsao et al 2006). Whatever the computational reason for local grouping of face cells, they constitute domain-specific specialized hardware, possibly implementing functional modules (Kanwisher 2010). As multiple face areas have been found across temporal and prefrontal cortex (Tsao et al 2008a, Tsao et al 2008b), it has become clear that modular organization goes hand in hand with distributed processing.
Face Networks, Parallel and Hierarchical Pathways
Distributed face areas do not operate in isolation, but are interconnected into a face-processing network through selective long-distance connections (Moeller et al 2008). Thus, facial information processing is integrated not only locally, but also across the larger distances separating face-selective areas. The spatial arrangement of face areas, relative timing of activation, and functional characteristics highlight two organizational principles within the network (Freiwald & Tsao 2010). First, face areas are organized along a posterior-anterior axis and response latencies systematically progress from early to late areas, suggesting hierarchical organization. Facial information appears to be systematically transformed from early view-specific representations into late identity-specific representations (Freiwald & Tsao 2010, Meyers et al 2015). In the most posterior area, PL, selectivity for a single feature, the eye, seems to dominate (Issa & DiCarlo 2012), at the next level, in areas MF and ML, a wide range of features are represented, and this selectivity is modulated by the embedding of features into the facial whole (Freiwald et al 2009). While face representations in MF and ML are view-selective, this dependence is reduced at the next level, AL, through construction of a mirror-symmetric invariance to head orientation, and reduced even further in AM, where representations for facial identity are robust against variation in head orientation (Dubois et al 2015, Freiwald & Tsao 2010, Meyers et al 2015). Concomitantly, tolerance for stimulus position and size is increased as well.
Orthogonal to this hierarchical principle of information processing, a second functional distinction between face areas along the ventral-dorsal axis exists. Face areas located in the fundus of the STS (dorsal) exhibit a pronounced preference for natural facial motion that face areas located laterally in the lower bank (ventral) lack (Fisher & Freiwald 2015a). These results suggest different kinds of facial information are processed through two parallel streams. However, given the complex connectivity between face areas (Moeller et al 2008) and the fact that information processed in either stream is likely informative for computations in the other, the parallelism is likely not one of functional isolation between streams.
The spatial distribution of face patches requires long-distance connections for them to interact, thus raising the question why the system is organized in this seemingly non-economical manner. The answer may be that the anatomical location of face patches is governed by another organizing principle, their belonging to multiple separate larger-scale object-processing maps. Such a scenario appears plausible given the curious co-localization of color patches directly ventral to some face areas (Lafer-Sousa & Conway 2013). Face and color processing share a foveal bias (Hasson et al 2002), and such retinotopic bias might explain their co-localization (Rajimehr et al 2014). Also abutting many face areas are body areas (Pinsk et al 2009, Popivanov et al 2012, Tsao et al 2003), whose co-localization with face areas may serve to support person perception.
Human and Non-human Primate Face-Processing Systems
Despite an estimated twenty five million years of separate evolution (Stewart & Disotell 1998), human and macaque face processing systems bear remarkable similarities, and some of these similarities might extend to the evolutionarily more distant New World monkeys (Hung et al 2015). In the human temporal and frontal lobes, multiple, spatially separate face areas have been found (for a recent review, see (Duchaine & Yovel 2015), Figure 1B). Like macaques, human ventral face areas are adjacent to body-selective areas (Peelen & Downing 2005, Schwarzlose et al 2005). The overall number of face areas is similar to that in macaques, and so is their spatial arrangement in the temporal lobe with two occipito-temporal series of areas, one in ventral temporal cortex and the other, dorsal one, in the STS (Yovel & Freiwald 2013). Functional specializations along the ventral series of face areas follow a progression similar to that in the macaque monkey, suggesting a hierarchical organization: in the occipito-temporal direction face selectivity increases (Bell et al 2009), position dependence decreases (Hemond et al 2007), mirror symmetric confusion of facial profile views emerges (Axelrod & Yovel 2012, Kietzmann et al 2012), and facial identity selectivity grows stronger (Yang, Susilo, & Duchaine, in press). Similarly, in both macaque and human face areas, the response to faces is augmented in the presence of an anatomically correctly placed body, and this augmentation grows stronger from posterior to anterior face areas (Bernstein et al 2014, Fisher & Freiwald 2015b, Song et al 2013). Furthermore selectivity for facial motion is highly pronounced in dorsal areas, but not in ventral ones (Fox et al 2009, Pitcher et al 2011a). Similar to the macaque, the ventral face areas appear to be interconnected, while connectivity of the two streams appears limited (Gschwind et al 2012) and is still the subject of active research (Yeatman et al 2014). Similarities between the systems extend beyond the temporal lobe. Face-selective neurons and areas have been found in the macaque in three major parts of prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Ó Scalaidhe et al 1997, Rolls et al 2006, Tsao et al 2008b), and later fMRI work confirmed the presence of face-selective areas in the human PFC (Axelrod & Yovel 2013, Chan & Downing 2011, Pitcher et al 2011a). Thus human and macaque face-processing systems, according to at least two sets of criteria, anatomical location and functional specialization, appear to be homologous.
Face Processing: from Cells to Behavior
Face Detection
For facial information to be extracted, the face must first be detected in a scene. Face detection has been proposed, based on psychophysical and computational work, to rely on coarse contrast relationships between the different parts of the face (Sinha 2002). Due to the three-dimensional structure of the face, e.g., with the eyes set back relative to the forehead, the forehead will be brighter than the eyes under most illumination conditions. Since change of appearance due to change of illumination is among the most difficult problems for object recognition, heuristics that are robust against this non-affine transformation are of great use. Furthermore, coarse contrast features do not require facial detail and thus should be easy to compute and relatively independent of facial identity. Humans indeed use twelve contrast-pairs (e.g. forehead brighter than left eye) to detect faces (Sinha 2002). This behavioral characteristic finds a clear correspondence at the single cell level. Half of the neurons in ML and MF, when probed with coarse-contrast face stimuli, showed sensitivity to contrast relationships between pairs of facial parts (e.g., nose and left eye) (Ohayon et al 2012) (Figure 2A). Cells exhibited highly consistent preferences for a specific polarity of each pair (e.g., nose brighter than left eye region). Importantly, the population preferences for contrast included all of the twelve pairs found to be important in human detection behavior, and the population’s polarity preferences matched that of human observers in all twelve cases.
Holistic Face Representations
A key characteristic of human face recognition is the holistic nature of upright-face processing relative to the more part-based nature of object processing and inverted face processing (Tanaka & Farah 1993). The face-inversion effect is the robust observation that humans perceive face information far less precisely in inverted than in upright faces (Yin 1969), and there is ample evidence to suggest that subjects use a more holistic strategy with upright than with inverted faces (Farah et al 1995, Young et al 1987). In the middle face patches, the response to inverted faces is, on average, delayed, more transient and overall reduced (Perrett et al 1982, Tsao et al 2006). This neural face-inversion effect may be confined to the face patches as it was found to be much weaker for face-cells located outside the face patches (Taubert et al 2015a). How might face-inversion affect the processing of facial features? Cells in ML/MF, when probed with cartoon stimuli, were tuned to more features in the upright than the inverted face (Freiwald et al 2009). Furthermore, tuning to one feature, eye-brows, was lost entirely under inversion, while tuning to the mouth was generated de novo. This pattern of results can be explained with a holistic template matching hypothesis that proposes incoming facial features in both upright and inverted faces are matched against an upright face template. The account also explains how the shape of feature tuning can be preserved upon face-inversion, as was observed for features that do not change shape and do not markedly change position when faces are inverted. Second, the part-whole effects reported in human psychophysical studies, demonstrate that features are better recognized within a face than in isolation (Tanaka & Farah 1993). ML/MF cells exhibit tuning to isolated facial features, but tuning is greatly augmented, when features are part of the entire face (Freiwald et al 2009). Third, maybe the most striking expression of the context dependence of facial features, is the so-called Thatcher illusion (Thompson 1980). Inversion of local features like the eyes or the mouth, which are easily noticed as grotesque distortions in the upright face, are hardly recognizable in the inverted face. A single unit correlate of this effect has been reported in ML (Taubert et al 2015b), where inversion of the eyes reduced the response in the upright, but not the inverted face and similar effects are reported in human electrophysiological studies (Carbon et al 2005) (Figure 2B).
Face Space
Many productive investigations of face processing have utilized the concept of face space: a multi-dimensional space in which each face occupies a unique position based on its combination of features (Leopold et al 2001, Turk & Pentland 1991, Valentine 1991). The space is thought to be organized around a center occupied by the average face, around which facial identities are located along trajectories that reflect how each face’s characteristics deviate from the average face (Figure 2C). The distance of a face along a trajectory is determined by the similarity of a face to the average face, so distinctive faces occupy the fringes of the space.
Two main models have been considered in discussions of how dimensions in face space are coded (Rhodes & Leopold 2011). One possibility is broad tuning curves spanning entire axes of face space. Such ramp-shaped tuning curves of opposite polarity can efficiently encode the position of a face feature along an axis. Second, an axis could be tiled by narrow tuning curves, in which each cell functions as a channel and coding is exemplar-based. Psychophysical results suggest that the center of face space occupies a special position such that identities located in opposite directions are related to each other even though they are perceptually dissimilar: when one adapts to one identity, subsequent perception of the “anti”-identity, but not that of an equidistant control identity, is enhanced (Leopold et al 2001). This effect is compatible with the broad tuning model, not the exemplar-based model. Moreover, these aftereffects are stronger than aftereffects found when faces matched in similarity to face/anti-face pairs are used to create a morph continuum (Rhodes & Jeffery 2006). Consistent with these psychophysical findings, broad tuning was indeed found in two macaque face areas. In MF/ML, the aforementioned feature tuning is predominantly ramp-shaped yielding a maximal response to one extreme and a minimal response to the opposite extreme (Figure 2C), and for almost all feature dimensions, opponent polarities were found across cells (Freiwald et al 2009). More anteriorly, likely in AM, broad tuning curves have also been found in response to facial identities (Leopold et al 2006). Unlike tuning curves in ML/MF, these tuning curves exhibited increased firing as a function of distance from the center of face space (Figure 2C), which provides neurophysiological evidence for the special role of the center of face space. More distinct response patterns to extreme faces in ML/MF compared to average faces might, furthermore, explain why identity has been found in some studies to be perceived more easily in caricatures than the original face (Rhodes et al 1997)
Faces as Sources of Social Information
A multitude of social information can be extracted from fine variations across faces, like gender, age, attractiveness, changing mental states like social attention and mood (Haxby et al 2000), and even inferred character attributes like trustworthiness (Todorov 2008). Many face cells have tuning properties that suggest an involvement in some of these social inferences. Some face cells are tuned to head orientation or gaze (De Souza et al 2005, Desimone et al 1984, Perrett et al 1985), and similar selectivity has been reported in the superior temporal sulcus of the human brain (Carlin et al 2011). This selectivity may support an analysis of the direction of social attention in the dorsal face pathways. Similar head-view selectivity was also found in the parallel, ventral face pathway, but there it may reflect an initial view-dependent step preceding a view-invariant face representation. Other face cells appear to be tuned to facial expressions (Gothard et al 2007, Sugase et al 1999) and yet others to physical characteristics of the face including facial features and identity (Freiwald et al 2009, Young & Yamane 1992). Mechanisms for the processing of expression and identity may overlap in common neural systems (Young & Calder, 2005, Bernstein & Yovel, 2015). Yet other studies have reported neural population codes to reflect another social quality, familiarity (Eifuku et al 2011, Young & Yamane 1992). Thus, specializations of face cells exist that are plausible neural mechanisms supporting the different social dimensions of face recognition.
Causal Links between Face-Processing Systems and Behavior
The links between particular neural processes and behavior discussed above are correlational, leaving open the question of whether these neural processes actually contribute to face recognition. Many regions in extrastriate cortex show a response to faces that is well above baseline, but as we discuss below, a number of findings suggest the response to faces is behaviorally relevant in face-selective but not in nearby areas that are not selective to faces. To determine the behavioral relevance of a neural mechanism, behavior must be assessed while that neural process is disrupted.
Acquired Prosopagnosia
The earliest evidence causally linking neural regions and face processing was provided by patients who acquired face processing deficits due to brain damage. Prosopagnosia can result from lesions extending from the ventral occipital lobe (Bouvier & Engel 2006, Dalrymple et al 2011, Rossion et al 2003) to the anterior temporal lobe (Busigny et al 2014, Dalrymple et al 2011) in the right hemisphere, while unilateral left hemisphere lesions rarely produce prosopagnosia ((Barton et al 2002, De Renzi et al 1994); see (Rossion 2014) for discussion of these cases). Thus, consistent with lateralization of face processing in human fMRI and ERP (Bentin et al 1996, Bukowski et al 2013), this pattern indicates face processing in humans is strongly right lateralized.
Functional imaging has allowed researchers to assess the status of face-selective areas in individuals with acquired prosopagnosia. Patient P.S. suffered from dense prosopagnosia following a closed-head injury, and fMRI revealed the damage overlapped with her right OFA, while leaving the right FFA intact (Rossion et al 2003, Sorger et al 2007). Findings from other prosopagnosics have provided further evidence that right hemisphere posterior face-selective areas are critical to face recognition (Dalrymple et al 2011, Fox et al 2011). However, prosopagnosia following damage to anterior temporal regions that spare posterior face-selective regions has been reported in a number of cases (Busigny et al 2014, Dalrymple et al 2011, Fox et al 2011). Thus, it appears that face-selective regions along the entire ventral pathway causally contribute to face recognition. Less is known about the areas supporting face perception for aspects of faces other than identity. R-ST1, one of the few cases with lesions to the face network restricted to the STS (Fox et al 2011), exhibited deficits with facial expression recognition consistent with the role of STS face areas in the processing of changeable facial information (Haxby et al 2000). Importantly, in identity matching tasks, R-ST1 performed normally when expression was held constant, but had pronounced deficits when expression differed in the images to be matched, suggesting STS face areas contribute to identity computations carried out in ventral stream areas when expression variation is present.
Experimental Disruption of Face-Processing
Experimental disruption during behavioral performance provides a controlled means to demonstrate a causal link between neural mechanisms and behavior, and studies using these methods in both humans and monkeys have demonstrated clear links between particular neural regions and face recognition. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) provides a non-invasive means to examine the functional role of brain regions in humans. Because of TMS’s limited ability to target regions not close to the scalp, most studies have targeted superficial face areas OFA and pSTS-FA. TMS to the right OFA impairs face shape discrimination but not object or body shape discrimination (Pitcher et al 2009, Pitcher et al 2012, Pitcher et al 2007) (Figure 3). Similarly, TMS to the extrastriate body area (EBA) selectively disrupts body discrimination while TMS over the lateral occipital complex (LO), an area implicated in object recognition, selectively disrupts object discrimination (Pitcher et al 2009). The three category-selective areas targeted in these studies all responded substantially to non-preferred categories (e.g., objects and bodies for OFA) (Schwarzlose et al 2008), so the category-selectivity of these impairments indicates (i) the presence of a response within a region does not imply behavioral relevance (see also (Dilks et al 2013, Pitcher et al 2011b), and (ii) supports a modular view of high-level visual recognition. Consistent with results from acquired prosopagnosics, TMS over both OFA and pSTS-FA has been found to disrupt expression perception, but the effective time window for pSTS-FA disruption is longer than the OFA window (Pitcher 2014), consistent with the notion of hierarchical processing.
Intracranial stimulation has a long history in human neuroscience, and recently researchers have applied it to investigate the neural basis of face processing. After mapping the category-selectivity of regions in right middle and posterior fusiform gyrus, Parvizi et al. (2012) electrically stimulated face-selective areas. These stimulations caused pronounced distortions to face percepts (Parvizi et al 2012). Consistent with a modular view, the patient reported only subtle distortions when the face regions were stimulated while he was viewing non-face objects. Evidence for functional lateralization of face-processing was provided by another study from this group which found stimulation to the right hemisphere face areas caused face-selective distortions, but comparable stimulation in the left hemisphere did not distort face percepts but led only to non-face, elementary distortions like phosphenes (Rangarajan et al 2014). Similarly, (Jonas et al 2014) stimulated right OFA, severely disrupting the ability to discriminate simultaneously presented faces (see also (Jonas et al 2012)). Thus electrical stimulation demonstrates a causal role of right hemisphere face-selective areas for face processing. Moreover, the absence of face distortions following stimulation to left hemisphere face areas in combination with the rarity of acquired prosopagnosia after unilateral left hemisphere lesions raise questions about what role, if any, left hemisphere areas make to face processing in humans.
Surprisingly few experiments disrupting face processing in the macaque have been done. Afraz et al (Afraz et al 2006) electrically stimulated clusters of face-selective neurons while macaques made face/non-face decisions on briefly presented stimuli. The macaques were more likely to report the presence of a face when face clusters were stimulated, whereas stimulation of clusters that were not face-selective did not affect behavior. Recently, Afraz et al (Afraz et al 2015) used optogenetics and muscimol in separate experiments to selectively suppress activity in face-selective clusters in the lower bank of the STS, likely in the vicinity of ML. During face gender decisions, suppression in face clusters, but not outside them, weakly disrupted performance (Figure 3A). These effects were restricted to contralaterally presented faces, likely reflecting the position of the inactivated neurons’ receptive fields. Together, the causal studies described above demonstrate that face-selective neurons and face-selective areas play critical roles in primate face processing and suggest that these areas play a much more prominent role than areas that are face-responsive but not face-selective.
Recognition of Familiar and Unfamiliar Faces
Most studies on face processing use unfamiliar faces. Unfamiliar faces are ideal stimuli to isolate the contribution of the visual system to face recognition, because they are associated with far less semantic and emotional information than familiar faces. Nevertheless, the primary goal of face recognition for social creatures is to recognize familiar individuals. Many of the basic phenomena of face recognition are common to familiar and unfamiliar faces (for review see (Johnston & Edmonds 2009)), including the face inversion effect (Yarmey 1971, Yin 1969), but see (Megreya & Burton 2006)), the composite face effect (Le Grand et al 2004, Young et al 1987) and the Thatcher illusion (Thompson 1980). Contrast negation impairs recognition of both familiar and unfamiliar faces (Kemp et al 1996), and distinctiveness enhances recognition of both familiar and unfamiliar faces (Bruce et al 1987). Also, prosopagnosic individuals almost always have difficulty recognizing both familiar and unfamiliar faces (Dalrymple et al 2011, Duchaine et al 2007, Van Belle et al 2010). Despite these similarities, the processing of familiar and unfamiliar faces differs in important ways. First, familiar faces are better recognized across different images based on their internal than external features whereas no such difference is found for unfamiliar faces (Clutterbuck & Johnston 2005, Ellis et al 1979, Young et al 1985). A second difference is that matching of unfamiliar faces across different illuminations (Johnston et al 1992), head orientations (Hill et al 1997, O’Toole et al 1998), and expressions (Bruce 1982) is prone to error whereas comparable tasks with familiar faces are effortless (Figure 4A).
How is this invariance for familiar faces generated? The original Bruce and Young (1986) model proposed that unfamiliar face representation involves a pictorial code, which is primarily image-based, whereas the representation of familiar faces relies on a structural code, which is more abstract and less susceptible to changes in view, expression and lighting. Burton, Jenkins, and colleagues (Burton et al 2005, Jenkins & Burton 2006, Jenkins & Burton 2011) suggested that this representation may be generated by averaging the various images of the same person that an observer encounters, separately for multiple views. These averaged representations will then include the essential, diagnostic features of a given identity. Consistent with this view, averaged faces made from many face exemplars of a given individual are better recognized than averaged faces made from fewer exemplars (Burton et al 2005). Thus representations of familiar faces benefit from rich perceptual exposure to the same individuals. In contrast, when we see an image of an unfamiliar face it can be difficult to disentangle superficial pictorial information from information about the stable characteristics of the face.
Familiar faces are not only rich in perceptual information but also in semantic, emotional, and social information, which can enhance face recognition. For example, participants better remember a face when asked to make a personality judgment (e.g., “Does the face look intelligent?”) than when they make a perceptual judgment (e.g., “Does the face have a large nose?”) (Curtois & Mueller 1979). Similarly, associating new faces with unique labels (i.e., each face with a different letter) increases face recognition compared to associations with similar labels (e.g. all faces are associated with the same letter) (McGugin et al 2011). These findings suggest that social, emotional, and higher-level categorizations all play a role in face recognition abilities. The contribution of conceptual information to the invariant representation that we have for familiar faces is also evident in the response of the so-called “concept cells” (Quiroga et al 2005) that demonstrate robustness to very different pictures of a known individual, even when the face is largely hidden in the image (see Figure 4B). This representation thus reflects invariance beyond purely visual information about the face, and must be based on person knowledge. Thus, combinations of visual and conceptual factors are likely to contribute to the invariant representation of familiar faces.
In summary, whereas recognition of familiar faces is robust to many different variations in the image of a given person, recognition of different images of unfamiliar people is challenging due to the limited amounts of information available.
Face Recognition in the Real World: Challenges and possible solutions
The limitations of unfamiliar face recognition have substantial effects in a number of consequential situations. Eyewitness identification is highly accurate for familiar faces, but when identification of unfamiliar faces is required, errors are frequent (Wells et al 2002) and an important factor in miscarriages of justice (Rattner 1988). In fact, eyewitness misidentification accounts for more than 70% of convictions overturned by DNA testing in the USA (www.innocenceproject.org/free-innocent/improve-the-law/fact-sheets/dna-exonerations-nationwide) (National Research Council (U.S.). Committee on Scientific Approaches to Understanding and Maximizing the Validity and Reliability of Eyewitness Identification in Law Enforcement and the Courts. et al). Another factor often limiting eyewitness identification of unfamiliar faces is the other-race effect: identification is substantially worse for faces of races a subject has had little contact with in daily life (Malpass & Kravitz 1969). Face recognition is also used at border crossings where simultaneous matching of a live person to a passport photo is required. Although simultaneous matching is easier than eyewitness identification, a recent study conducted with passport officers found these highly experienced individuals incorrectly accepted 14% of the passports that did not match their bearers (White et al 2014). Furthermore, the officers’ performance was no better than that of student participants. Notably the passport officers showed great individual differences (range 0–30% errors), and these individual differences were not associated with years of experience in their job.
In the last decade, it has become apparent that great individual differences in face processing exist (for review see (Yovel et al 2014)). In developmental prosopagnosia (DP), individuals with no history of brain damage or general impairments have difficulty recognizing the faces of family and friends (Susilo & Duchaine 2013). On the opposite end of the spectrum are super-recognizers; they rarely forget the faces of people they interact with and regularly recognize faces encountered only briefly years before (Russell et al 2012, Russell et al 2009). The majority of the population lies somewhere between these extremes, and differences among them are stable (Bowles et al 2009, Wilhelm et al 2010) and strongly heritable (Wilmer et al 2010, Zhu et al 2010). Lab-based face identity tests are not only correlated with other lab-based face recognition tests (Germine et al 2012), but also predict performance in more ecologically valid identification situations like eyewitness identification (Andersen et al 2014). Modest relationships have been found between face recognition ability and some known face processing effects such as the composite face effect (DeGutis et al 2013) and face aftereffects (Dennett et al 2012), but the neurocognitive sources of most variance in face processing abilities is unknown.
The reliable and stable individual differences across lab-based and real-life face recognition tasks suggest that when face recognition plays a role in consequential situations, individual ability should be considered. In courts, awareness of an eyewitness’ face recognition skill would allow the judge and jury to better assess the weight they should place on the eyewitness’ testimony. Selection for occupations such as passport officers and police officers would benefit from consideration of face recognition ability, and this suggestion has been successfully applied recently by the London Metropolitan Police (Davis et al 2013). In collaboration with face researchers, officers and staff with especially good face recognition have been identified. These ‘super-recognizers’ used their skills to identify hundreds of suspects from low-resolution surveillance videos taken during the large-scale riots in London in 2011, and a team of ‘super-recognizers’ have watched live video during large festivals to identify and then capture suspects wanted for previously committed crimes.
In summary, the belief that all humans are experts in face recognition has led to the assumption that face recognition can be relied on in real life settings. However although this assumption is, by and large, true for identification of familiar faces, it has detrimental effects on criminal justice, border control, and policing, because we are far from perfect when it comes to identification of unfamiliar faces (Burton & Jenkins 2011).
Conclusion
The dedicated system for face processing in the primate brain has been the focus of intensive basic and applied research in the recent decades. The findings discussed here provide an overview of attempts to bridge the gap between levels of organization from single cells to networks of areas and behavior as well as between lab-based and real-life face recognition. This work shows the promise of these approaches but also makes clear that these are only the initial steps toward a comprehensive understanding of the operations required for successful face recognition.
Contributor Information
Winrich Freiwald, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10065, USA.
Bradley Duchaine, Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover NH 03755 USA.
Galit Yovel, School of Psychological Sciences & Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel.
References
- Afraz A, Boyden ES, DiCarlo JJ. Optogenetic and pharmacological suppression of spatial clusters of face neurons reveal their causal role in face gender discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015;112:6730–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423328112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Afraz SR, Kiani R, Esteky H. Microstimulation of inferotemporal cortex influences face categorization. Nature. 2006;442:692–5. doi: 10.1038/nature04982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SM, Carlson CA, Carlson M, Gronlund SD. Individual Differences Predict Eyewitness Identification Performance. Personality and Individual Differences. 2014;60:36–40. [Google Scholar]
- Axelrod V, Yovel G. Hierarchical processing of face viewpoint in human visual cortex. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:2442–52. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4770-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Axelrod V, Yovel G. The challenge of localizing the anterior temporal face area: a possible solution. NeuroImage. 2013;81:371–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barton JJ, Press DZ, Keenan JP, O’Connor M. Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial configuration in prosopagnosia. Neurology. 2002;58:71–8. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell AH, Hadj-Bouziane F, Frihauf JB, Tootell RB, Ungerleider LG. Object representations in the temporal cortex of monkeys and humans as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of neurophysiology. 2009;101:688–700. doi: 10.1152/jn.90657.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1996;8:551–65. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein M, Oron J, Sadeh B, Yovel G. An integrated face-body representation in the fusiform gyrus but not the lateral occipital cortex. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2014;26:2469–78. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouvier SE, Engel SA. Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia. Cerebral cortex. 2006;16:183–91. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowles DC, McKone E, Dawel A, Duchaine B, Palermo R, et al. Diagnosing prosopagnosia: effects of ageing, sex, and participant-stimulus ethnic match on the Cambridge Face Memory Test and Cambridge Face Perception Test. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26:423–55. doi: 10.1080/02643290903343149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce V. Changing faces: visual and non-visual coding processes in face recognition. British journal of psychology. 1982;73:105–16. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1982.tb01795.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce V, Ellis H, Gibling F, Young A. Parallel processing of the sex and familiarity of faces. Canadian journal of psychology. 1987;41:510–20. doi: 10.1037/h0084165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bukowski H, Dricot L, Hanseeuw B, Rossion B. Cerebral lateralization of face-sensitive areas in left-handers: only the FFA does not get it right. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 2013;49:2583–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton AM, Jenkins R. Unfamiliar face perception. In: Calder A, Rhodes G, Johnson M, Haxby J, editors. Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Burton AM, Jenkins R, Hancock PJB, White D. Robust representations for face recognition: The power of averages. Cognitive Psychology. 2005;51:256–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busigny T, Van Belle G, Jemel B, Hosein A, Joubert S, Rossion B. Face-specific impairment in holistic perception following focal lesion of the right anterior temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:312–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carbon CC, Schweinberger SR, Kaufmann JM, Leder H. The Thatcher illusion seen by the brain: an event-related brain potentials study. Brain research Cognitive brain research. 2005;24:544–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlin JD, Calder AJ, Kriegeskorte N, Nili H, Rowe JB. A head view-invariant representation of gaze direction in anterior superior temporal sulcus. Curr Biol. 2011;21:1817–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan AW, Downing PE. Faces and eyes in human lateral prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2011;5:51. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clutterbuck R, Johnston RA. Demonstrating how unfamiliar faces become familiar using a face matching task. European J Cog Psychol. 2005;17:97–116. [Google Scholar]
- Curtois MR, Mueller JH. Processing multiple physical features in facial recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1979 [Google Scholar]
- Dalrymple KA, Oruc I, Duchaine B, Pancaroglu R, Fox CJ, et al. The anatomic basis of the right face-selective N170 IN acquired prosopagnosia: a combined ERP/fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2553–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis JP, Lander K, Jansari A. I never forget a face. Psychologist. 2013;26:726–29. [Google Scholar]
- De Renzi E, Perani D, Carlesimo GA, Silveri MC, Fazio F. Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined to the right hemisphere--an MRI and PET study and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:893–902. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90041-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Souza WC, Eifuku S, Tamura R, Nishijo H, Ono T. Differential characteristics of face neuron responses within the anterior superior temporal sulcus of macaques. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;94:1252–66. doi: 10.1152/jn.00949.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeGutis J, Wilmer J, Mercado RJ, Cohan S. Using regression to measure holistic face processing reveals a strong link with face recognition ability. Cognition. 2013;126:87–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennett HW, McKone E, Edwards M, Susilo T. Face aftereffects predict individual differences in face recognition ability. Psychological science. 2012;23:1279–87. doi: 10.1177/0956797612446350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Albright TD, Gross CG, Bruce C. Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1984;4:2051–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-08-02051.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilks DD, Julian JB, Paunov AM, Kanwisher N. The occipital place area is causally and selectively involved in scene perception. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2013;33:1331–6a. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4081-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois J, de Berker AO, Tsao DY. Single-unit recordings in the macaque face patch system reveal limitations of fMRI MVPA. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35:2791–802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4037-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchaine B, Yovel G. A revised neural framework for face processing. Annual Review of Vision Science. 2015;1:293–416. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchaine B, Yovel G, Nakayama K. No global processing deficit in the Navon task in 14 developmental prosopagnosics. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2007;2:104–13. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eifuku S, De Souza WC, Nakata R, Ono T, Tamura R. Neural representations of personally familiar and unfamiliar faces in the anterior inferior temporal cortex of monkeys. PloS one. 2011;6:e18913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis HD, Shepherd JW, Davies GM. Identification of familiar and unfamiliar faces from internal and external features: some implications for theories of face recognition. Perception. 1979;8:431–9. doi: 10.1068/p080431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farah MJ, Tanaka JW, Drain HM. What causes the face inversion effect? Journal of experimental psychology Human perception and performance. 1995;21:628–34. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher C, Freiwald WA. Contrasting specializations for facial motion within the macaque face-processing system. Current biology : CB. 2015a;25:261–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher C, Freiwald WA. Whole-agent selectivity within the macaque face-processing system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015b;112:14717–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512378112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox CJ, Hanif HM, Iaria G, Duchaine BC, Barton JJ. Perceptual and anatomic patterns of selective deficits in facial identity and expression processing. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:3188–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox CJ, Moon SY, Iaria G, Barton JJ. The correlates of subjective perception of identity and expression in the face network: an fMRI adaptation study. NeuroImage. 2009;44:569–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. Functional compartmentalization and viewpoint generalization within the macaque face-processing system. Science. 2010;330:845–51. doi: 10.1126/science.1194908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freiwald WA, Tsao DY, Livingstone MS. A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12:1187–96. doi: 10.1038/nn.2363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K. Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature. 1992;360:343–46. doi: 10.1038/360343a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Germine L, Nakayama K, Duchaine BC, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Wilmer JB. Is the Web as good as the lab? Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive/perceptual experiments. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2012;19:847–57. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gothard KM, Battaglia FP, Erickson CA, Spitler KM, Amaral DG. Neural responses to facial expression and face identity in the monkey amygdala. Journal of neurophysiology. 2007;97:1671–83. doi: 10.1152/jn.00714.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross CG. Genealogy of the “Grandmother Cell”. The Neuroscientist. 2002;8:512–18. doi: 10.1177/107385802237175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gschwind M, Pourtois G, Schwartz S, Van De Ville D, Vuilleumier P. White-matter connectivity between face-responsive regions in the human brain. Cerebral cortex. 2012;22:1564–76. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R. Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron. 2002;34:479–90. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00662-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haxby JV, Gobbini MI, Furey ML, Ishai A, Schouten JL, Pietrini P. Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science. 2001;293:2425–30. doi: 10.1126/science.1063736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2000;4:223–33. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemond CC, Kanwisher NG, Op de Beeck HP. A preference for contralateral stimuli in human object- and face-selective cortex. PloS one. 2007;2:e574. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill H, Schyns PG, Akamatsu S. Information and viewpoint dependence in face recognition. Cognition. 1997;62:201–22. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(96)00785-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH. Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955–78. NATURE (LONDON) 1982;299:515–24. doi: 10.1038/299515a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hung CC, Yen CC, Ciuchta JL, Papoti D, Bock NA, et al. Functional mapping of face-selective regions in the extrastriate visual cortex of the marmoset. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35:1160–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2659-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Issa EB, DiCarlo JJ. Precedence of the eye region in neural processing of faces. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:16666–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2391-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Issa EB, Papanastassiou AM, DiCarlo JJ. Large-scale, high-resolution neurophysiological maps underlying FMRI of macaque temporal lobe. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2013;33:15207–19. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1248-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins R, Burton AM. Face recognition from unconstrained images: Progress with prototypes. Proceedings of the Seventh IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition; 2006. pp. 25–30. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins R, Burton AM. Stable face representations. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2011;366:1671–83. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston A, Hill H, Carman N. Recognising faces: effects of lighting direction, inversion, and brightness reversal. Perception. 1992;21:365–75. doi: 10.1068/p210365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston RA, Edmonds AJ. Familiar and unfamiliar face recognition: a review. Memory. 2009;17:577–96. doi: 10.1080/09658210902976969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonas J, Descoins M, Koessler L, Colnat-Coulbois S, Sauvee M, et al. Focal electrical intracerebral stimulation of a face-sensitive area causes transient prosopagnosia. Neuroscience. 2012;222:281–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonas J, Rossion B, Krieg J, Koessler L, Colnat-Coulbois S, et al. Intracerebral electrical stimulation of a face-selective area in the right inferior occipital cortex impairs individual face discrimination. NeuroImage. 2014;99:487–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N. Domain specificity in face perception. Nature neuroscience. 2000;3:759–63. doi: 10.1038/77664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N. Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:11163–70. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005062107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1997;17:4302–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp R, Pike G, White P, Musselman A. Perception and recognition of normal and negative faces: the role of shape from shading and pigmentation cues. Perception. 1996;25:37–52. doi: 10.1068/p250037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kietzmann TC, Swisher JD, Konig P, Tong F. Prevalence of selectivity for mirror-symmetric views of faces in the ventral and dorsal visual pathways. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:11763–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0126-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lafer-Sousa R, Conway BR. Parallel, multi-stage processing of colors, faces and shapes in macaque inferior temporal cortex. Nature neuroscience. 2013;16:1870–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.3555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Grand R, Mondloch CJ, Maurer D, Brent HP. Impairment in holistic face processing following early visual deprivation. Psychological science. 2004;15:762–8. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00753.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeCun Y, Bengio Y. The Handbook of Brain THeory and Neural Networks. Arbib: MIT Press; 1995. Convolutional Networks for Images, Speech, and Time Series. [Google Scholar]
- Leopold DA, Bondar IV, Giese MA. Norm-based face encoding by single neurons in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature. 2006;442:572–75. doi: 10.1038/nature04951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leopold DA, O’Toole AJO, Vetter T, Blanz V. Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature neuroscience. 2001;4:89–94. doi: 10.1038/82947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malpass RS, Kravitz J. Recognition for faces of own and other race. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1969;13:330–4. doi: 10.1037/h0028434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy G, Puce A, Gore JC, Allison T. Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1997;9:605–10. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGugin RW, Tanaka JW, Lebrecht S, Tarr MJ, Gauthier I. Race-specific perceptual discrimination improvement following short individuation training with faces. Cognitive science. 2011;35:330–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01148.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Megreya AM, Burton AM. Unfamiliar faces are not faces: evidence from a matching task. Memory & cognition. 2006;34:865–76. doi: 10.3758/bf03193433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyers EM, Borzello M, Freiwald WA, Tsao D. Intelligent information loss: the coding of facial identity, head pose, and non-face information in the macaque face patch system. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35:7069–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. Patches with links: a unified system for processing faces in the macaque temporal lobe. Science. 2008;320:1355–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1157436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council (U.S.), Committee on Scientific Approaches to Understanding and Maximizing the Validity and Reliability of Eyewitness Identification in Law Enforcement and the Courts., National Research Council (U.S.). Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education., National Research Council (U.S.). Committee on Science Technology and Law., National Research Council (U.S.). Committee on Law and Justice. Identifying the culprit : assessing eyewitness identification. :xvi, 154. [Google Scholar]
- O’Toole AJ, Edelman S, Bulthoff HH. Stimulus-specific effects in face recognition over changes in viewpoint. Vision research. 1998;38:2351–63. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00042-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ó Scalaidhe SP, Wilson FAW, Goldman-Rakic PS. Areal segregation of face-processing neurons in prefrontal cortex. SCIENCE (WASHINGTON DC) 1997;278:1135–38. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5340.1135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohayon S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. What makes a cell face selective? The importance of contrast. Neuron. 2012;74:567–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Witthoft N, Rangarajan V, et al. Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:14915–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2609-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelen MV, Downing PE. Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;93:603–8. doi: 10.1152/jn.00513.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrett DI, Hietanen JK, Oram MW, Benson PJ. Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 1992;335:23–30. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrett DI, Rolls ET, Caan W. Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Experimental brain research. 1982;47:329–42. doi: 10.1007/BF00239352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrett DI, Smith PA, Potter DD, Mistlin AJ, Head AS, et al. Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 1985;223:293–317. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1985.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrett DI, Smith PAJ, Potter DD, Mistlin AJ, Head AS, et al. Neurones responsive to faces in the temporal cortex: studies of functional organization, sensitivity to identity and relation to perception. Human neurobiology. 1984;3:197–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinsk MA, Arcaro M, Weiner KS, Kalkus JF, Inati SJ, et al. Neural representations of faces and body parts in macaque and human cortex: a comparative FMRI study. Journal of neurophysiology. 2009;101:2581–600. doi: 10.1152/jn.91198.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D. Facial expression recognition takes longer in the posterior superior temporal sulcus than in the occipital face area. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2014;34:9173–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5038-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D, Charles L, Devlin JT, Walsh V, Duchaine B. Triple dissociation of faces, bodies, and objects in extrastriate cortex. Current biology : CB. 2009;19:319–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D, Dilks DD, Saxe RR, Triantafyllou C, Kanwisher N. Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions. NeuroImage. 2011a;56:2356–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D, Goldhaber T, Duchaine B, Walsh V, Kanwisher N. Two critical and functionally distinct stages of face and body perception. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:15877–85. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2624-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D, Walsh V, Duchaine B. The role of the occipital face area in the cortical face perception network. Experimental brain research. 2011b;209:481–93. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2579-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher D, Walsh V, Yovel G, Duchaine B. TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing. Current biology : CB. 2007;17:1568–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popivanov ID, Jastorff J, Vanduffel W, Vogels R. Stimulus representations in body-selective regions of the macaque cortex assessed with event-related fMRI. NeuroImage. 2012;63:723–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature. 2005;435:1102–07. doi: 10.1038/nature03687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajimehr R, Bilenko NY, Vanduffel W, Tootell RB. Retinotopy versus face selectivity in macaque visual cortex. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2014;26:2691–700. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rangarajan V, Hermes D, Foster BL, Weiner KS, Jacques C, et al. Electrical stimulation of the left and right human fusiform gyrus causes different effects in conscious face perception. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2014;34:12828–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0527-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rattner A. Convicted but innocent: Wrongful conviction and the criminal justice system. Law and Human Behavior. 1988;12:283–93. [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G, Byatt G, Tremewan T, Kennedy A. Facial distinctiveness and the power of caricatures. Perception. 1997;26:207–23. doi: 10.1068/p260207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G, Jeffery L. Adaptive norm-based coding of facial identity. Vision research. 2006;46:2977–87. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G, Leopold DA. Adaptive Norm-Based Coding of Facial Identity. In: Calder Rhodes, Johnson Haxby., editors. The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. Oxford: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nature neuroscience. 1999;2:1019–25. doi: 10.1038/14819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning AS, Inoue K. Face-selective and auditory neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Experimental brain research. 2006;170:74–87. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0191-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossion B. Understanding face perception by means of prosopagnosia and neuroimaging. Frontiers in bioscience. 2014;6:258–307. doi: 10.2741/E706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossion B, Caldara R, Seghier M, Schuller AM, Lazeyras F, Mayer E. A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2003;126:2381–95. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell R, Chatterjee G, Nakayama K. Developmental prosopagnosia and super-recognition: no special role for surface reflectance processing. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:334–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell R, Duchaine B, Nakayama K. Super-recognizers: people with extraordinary face recognition ability. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2009;16:252–7. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzlose RF, Baker CI, Kanwisher N. Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2005;25:11055–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzlose RF, Swisher JD, Dang S, Kanwisher N. The distribution of category and location information across object-selective regions in human visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:4447–52. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800431105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha P. Qualitative representations for recognition. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2002. pp. 249–62. [Google Scholar]
- Song Y, Luo YL, Li X, Xu M, Liu J. Representation of contextually related multiple objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2013;25:1261–9. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorger B, Goebel R, Schiltz C, Rossion B. Understanding the functional neuroanatomy of acquired prosopagnosia. NeuroImage. 2007;35:836–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart CB, Disotell TR. Primate evolution - in and out of Africa. Current biology : CB. 1998;8:R582–8. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(07)00367-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugase Y, Yamane S, Ueno S, Kawano K. Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature. 1999;400:869–73. doi: 10.1038/23703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susilo T, Duchaine B. Advances in developmental prosopagnosia research. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2013;23:423–9. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka JW, Farah MJ. Parts and wholes in face recognition. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology A, Human experimental psychology. 1993;46:225–45. doi: 10.1080/14640749308401045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taubert J, Van Belle G, Vanduffel W, Rossion B, Vogels R. The effect of face inversion for neurons inside and outside fMRI-defined face-selective cortical regions. Journal of neurophysiology. 2015a;113:1644–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.00700.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taubert J, Van Belle G, Vanduffel W, Rossion B, Vogels R. Neural Correlate of the Thatcher Face Illusion in a Monkey Face-Selective Patch. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015b;35:9872–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0446-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson P. Margaret Thatcher: a new illusion. Perception. 1980;9:483–4. doi: 10.1068/p090483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todorov A. Evaluating faces on trustworthiness: an extension of systems for recognition of emotions signaling approach/avoidance behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:208–24. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Knutsen TA, Mandeville JB, Tootell RB. Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex. Nature neuroscience. 2003;6:989–95. doi: 10.1038/nn1111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Tootell RB, Livingstone MS. A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science. 2006;311:670–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1119983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Moeller S, Freiwald WA. Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008a;105:19514–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809662105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Schweers N, Moeller S, Freiwald WA. Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nature neuroscience. 2008b;11:877–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.2158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turk M, Pentland A. Eigenfaces for recognition. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1991;3:71–86. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentine T. A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1991;43A:161–204. doi: 10.1080/14640749108400966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Belle G, De Graef P, Verfaillie K, Busigny T, Rossion B. Whole not hole: expert face recognition requires holistic perception. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2620–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells GL, Olson E, Charman S. Eyewitness identification confidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:151–54. [Google Scholar]
- White D, Kemp RI, Jenkins R, Matheson M, Burton AM. Passport officers’ errors in face matching. PloS one. 2014;9:e103510. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm O, Herzmann G, Kunina O, Danthiir V, Schacht A, Sommer W. Individual differences in perceiving and recognizing faces-One element of social cognition. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2010;99:530–48. doi: 10.1037/a0019972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilmer JB, Germine L, Chabris CF, Cahatterjee G, Williams M, et al. Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107:5238–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913053107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarmey AD. Recognition memory for familiar “public” faces: Effecs of orientation and delay. Psychonomic Science. 1971;24:286–88. [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Weiner KS, Pestilli F, Rokem A, Mezer A, Wandell BA. The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111:E5214–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin R. Looking at upside down faces. J Exp Psychol. 1969;81:141–45. [Google Scholar]
- Young AW, Hay DC, McWeeny KH, Flude BM, Ellis AW. Matching familiar and unfamiliar faces on internal and external features. Perception. 1985;14:737–46. doi: 10.1068/p140737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young AW, Hellawell D, Hay DC. Configurational information in face perception. Perception. 1987;16:747–59. doi: 10.1068/p160747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young MP, Yamane S. Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. Science. 1992;29:1327–31. doi: 10.1126/science.1598577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yovel G, Freiwald WA. Face recognition systems in monkey and human: are they the same thing? F1000prime reports. 2013;5:10. doi: 10.12703/P5-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yovel G, Wilmer JB, Duchaine B. What can individual differences reveal about face processing? Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:562. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu Q, Song Y, Hu S, Li X, Tian M, et al. Heritability of the specific cognitive ability of face perception. Current biology : CB. 2010;20:137–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]