An official website of the United States government
Here's how you know
Official websites use .gov
A
.gov website belongs to an official
government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS
A lock (
) or https:// means you've safely
connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive
information only on official, secure websites.
As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsement of, or agreement with,
the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more:
PMC Disclaimer
|
PMC Copyright Notice
Points of View (POV) addresses issues faced by many people within the life science education community.Cell Biology Education (CBE) has launched the POV Feature to present two or more opinions published in tandem on a common topic. We consider POVs to be “Op-Ed” pieces designed to stimulate thought and dialog on significant educational issues. Each author had the opportunity to revise or add to his/her POV after reading drafts of the other's POV.
In this issue, we ask the question, “What is the appropriate role of the lecture format for teaching?” Many teaching methods are used around the world, but the “tried and true” lecture format is surely the best known and probably the most common. To what extent should lectures be used and how would we know if they are effective or merely convenient? The POVs in this issue are grounded in different class settings and teaching philosophies, and yet they share a subset of common principles that are evident on careful reading. Harvey Lodish, Professor of Biology at Massachusetts Institute of Technology, is well known for coauthoring a cell biology textbook and is the current President of the American Society for Cell Biology. Dan Klionsky is Professor of Life Sciences at the University of Michigan; in 2003 he received the National Science Foundation Director's Award for Distinguished Teaching Scholars in recognition for his high-quality research and teaching. Both authors are deeply concerned about the quality of instruction in their own classrooms as well as nationally, but they differ in many of their teaching methods. Readers are encouraged to compare the authors' perspectives and share their thoughts and reactions using the online discussion forum hosted by CBE at http://www.cellbioed.org/discussion/public/main.cfm.
How to decide on the format for an undergraduate course in cell biology—a “standard” combination of lectures and recitations sections, or something else? The answer depends on many factors, including the numbers, abilities, and course backgrounds of the students and, perhaps most importantly, the purpose of the course. Thus, to explain why we feel that our junior-senior level cell biology course, taught with a combination of lectures, teaching assistant (TA)-led recitation sections, and extensive problem sets, works extraordinarily well for the vast majority of Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) students and accomplishes its intended purposes, we need to describe several aspects of the MIT undergraduate curriculum and also what we expect the students to learn in the course.
An introductory course in biology, also taught in the lecture/recitation section/problem set mode, is required of all MIT students and is usually taken in the freshman year. Students interested in biology or bioengineering will also take organic chemistry in the freshman year, and both biochemistry and genetics (also in the lecture/recitation/problem set format) in the second year. Generally in the sophomore year, these students will also take the introductory laboratory course in biology, which teaches basic techniques in genetics (e.g., mutant hunts and mapping), biochemistry (e.g., enzyme assays), cell biology and microscopy, and some developmental biology.
Our course has prerequisites of both biochemistry and genetics, and most of the students have taken the introductory laboratory course. Thus we are able to teach at a fairly high level. One of us (HL) has had over 25 years of experience teaching this course, generally sharing the responsibility with another instructor.
Our goals are several-fold. First, we want to teach the basic facts and concepts of broad areas of modern cell biology, ranging from membrane transport and vesicle trafficking to control of the cell cycle and the functions of cytoskeletal and extracellular matrix proteins. We want the students to understand the experimental underpinnings of these key concepts. Perhaps most importantly, we want to teach the students how to apply the experimental techniques they have been exposed to in previous courses to real research problems. We want them to be able to understand experiments that utilize a broad range of research techniques ranging from recombinant expression of proteins to cellular imaging to protein–protein interactions. We want our students to understand why yeasts are so useful for studies such as on the cell cycle and vesicle transport, and why many results obtained with this organism are immediately relevant to mammals. Our students also come to appreciate the importance of genetic studies in flies and worms in fundamental aspects of cell biology such as cell–cell signaling. Finally, we want our students to be able to integrate genetic, biochemical, and morphological approaches in designing experiments to test a particular hypothesis, and we want them to be able to interpret experimental results that use multiple methodologies and experimental organisms.
Indeed, these are ambitious goals but we feel that our lecture/recitation session/problem set format works very well for the ∼180 students in this one-semester course each year. RKR vouches for this based on his experience as a TA in the course and previously as an undergraduate at the University of California, Davis, where he had several classes in this format.
The lectures are a key part of the course but clearly lectures alone are not enough. The lectures are tightly integrated with the other two parts of the course.
During much of each 90-minute lecture the instructor mainly tries to discuss the “big picture” and motivates the students to learn particular topics. One way to do this is to tell the students not only what is most important about a particular topic, but explain to them why it is important. Another way to motivate students to learn topics like cyclin-dependent kinases or sodium-coupled transport proteins is to relate studies on isolated proteins and cells in culture to important functions of cells in the organism and, more broadly, to human physiology. We present many examples, especially from human medicine, that have proven useful in exciting the student's interest. For instance, alterations in cyclin-dependent kinases (Cdks) in certain cancers inform the lectures on cell-cycle control, and the fact that certain psychiatric medicines are inhibitors of sodium-coupled transport proteins inform discussions of membrane transport.
The lectures also outline the principal types of experimental techniques for studying particular problems, (e.g., patch clamping for measuring ion flow through single-channel proteins or green fluorescent protein fusions to study protein trafficking in living cells). Special effort is paid to the integration of biochemical and genetic studies, (e.g., in the analysis of the cell cycle and apoptosis). However, the details of these techniques are generally reserved for the problem sets and the recitation sections.
There are many ways we keep a large class of students in an auditorium engaged and attentive during our 90-min lectures. For example, the Microsoft PowerPoint slides used in the lecture are on the web before the class. Many students download then beforehand, enabling them to write notes on the printouts during the lectures.
Even in a relatively large lecture one can have discussions that involve several students as well as the instructor and, occasionally, a TA. One way to do this is for the instructor to pause several times during the lecture and repeatedly encourage students to ask questions. Another way to elicit questions is to reiterate the point that, if any student feels confused about a point, many others in the class undoubtedly are also confused but are too reluctant to speak up. Another method is to “bait” the class telling them that, because they ask fewer questions than last year's group, they must be less talented.
During these pauses the lecturer roams through the lecture theater with a portable microphone and insists that the student volunteers use it to ask questions so that everyone can hear the discussion. Often these questions lead into tangential but interesting side discussions. Occasionally the lecturer will call on several students to improve on the original answer or to suggest alternatives (e.g., Do you think that Sara gave the correct answer? Can anyone tell me why it might be wrong? Before I tell you the correct answer shall we vote on whether John or Sara is correct?).
It is important for students to hear from the lecturer that not all questions have answers. There is much in cell biology that is not known; often the most illuminating part of a lecture is when the students learn why a particular question is so difficult to answer experimentally. Spontaneity and knowledge of the current literature on the part of the lecturer is helpful; sometimes in response to a student question one can bring up results obtained with a different experimental system and use the opportunity to explain, as an example, why a genetic rather than a subcellular fractionation experiment led to the answer.
Another way to encourage participation, even among the students in a large class, is for the lecturer to ask the class how one might design an experiment to test a particular hypothesis. Again, student volunteers use the portable microphone to speak, and the lecturer gives hints and asks others to suggest improvement until several students have spoken and the class has come to agree on a particular strategy. These class discussions generally last five to 10 minutes; they do help increase the students' interest in the material and also help them think about experimental design and interpretation.
The problem sets—eight sets per semester each with one-half dozen difficult and generally multipart questions—are an essential part of the course. These questions are newly generated each year by the TAs and ask the student to solve a numerical problem (e.g., use the Nernst equation to calculate membrane potential changes), interpret a set of experimental data, or design a set of experiments to test a certain hypothesis. These are not graded and are meant to illustrate the types of questions on the four open-book exams. All problem sets and exams from the past five years of the course are posted on the class web site, generally in versions with and without the answer key. In this way students have ample problems with which to practice their mastery of the topic.
One disadvantage of this format is that the lecturer gets to know relatively few of the students in the course. Given the large number of the students and the need to preserve the sanity of both the lecturers and the TAs, this is a necessary trade-off. However, this is one reason why we have recitation sections, as they allow smaller groups of students (about 30 students per section) to get more personal attention from a TA.
The recitation sections are meant to be a forum for students to learn how to apply the techniques they learn in lecture and to go over the problem sets. Whereas the lectures expand on the history and theories of cell biology, the sections focus on developing the skills that students need to apply their knowledge to experimental approaches to cell biology. The TAs are second- and fourth-year students in our Ph.D. program. They meet regularly as a group and work together with the course lecturer to plan the recitation sessions and the problem sets.
During the hour-long sessions, the TAs go over many common cell biology techniques and experimental designs, which help to teach the students how to solve multiple types of problems in cell biology. They also lead students through practice problems similar to the assigned homework problems, and often present short lectures that emphasize how techniques are used to experimentally approach specific types of problems.
Through problem sets and practice problems covered in sections students progress in their ability to solve problems and design experiments as the semester goes along. The TAs expect the students to use these sections to practice their developing skills and to ask questions in order to clarify any questions they may have about the material. If the students come prepared, they should be able to think, analyze, and solve the problems that the TAs pose to them every week.
One difficulty with the recitation sections is that the students are usually not as prepared as we would like. Ideally, after each lecture the students will have read the accompanying chapter in their text book and started to work on some of the problems in the assigned problem sets. Then, the students would come prepared to section ready to ask questions to help assuage any deficiencies they may have in their learning. Ideally, we would like the students to ask specific questions about what is confusing them and how they can use the techniques they've learned in class to solve the assigned problems. Instead, many students put off reading the textbook and practicing their problem-solving skills and expect the TAs to cover everything they need to know to do well on the exams during the recitation section. The students expect the TAs to cover the important lecture material, highlight the techniques they will need to know, and show them how to apply it to solve problems all within the single hour per week that the TAs meet with them! Obviously, this is an unattainable goal!
Nonetheless, most of the students do well on the exams, which require a fairly high level of understanding of broad areas of cell biology. The exam questions usually ask the student for interpretations of experiments or ask the student to design a genetic or biochemical experiment to test a specific hypothesis. Many students struggle at the beginning of the course because they never had to deal with problems in experimental design and interpretation in earlier courses. However, we find that at the end, a large majority of the students will have mastered the broad concepts of cell biology and also understand how research in this field is carried out and why this research is important. The lectures, the problem sets, and the recitation sections all are important parts of the course.
1Life Sciences Institute and Departments of Molecular Cellular and Developmental Biology and Biological Chemistry University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109
1Life Sciences Institute and Departments of Molecular Cellular and Developmental Biology and Biological Chemistry University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109
Our knowledge of biology has been changing dramatically, yet most of us continue to teach biology the same way we have for the past century. In fact, teaching styles have not changed substantially since before the invention of the printing press. A current wave of reform in undergraduate science education has prompted many instructors to move away from a passive learning format in which the instructor attempts to convey knowledge to students through lectures and note taking. They are replacing this approach with ones that involve active learning. In this article, I describe my own conversion from teaching with a lecture format to the development of an active-learning approach (Klionsky, 1998, 2001; Klionsky and Tomashek, 1999). Along the way, I was introduced to several important concepts such as the difference between “teaching” and “learning,” the need to set learning goals and to utilize methods of assessment, and the value of becoming aware of the pedagogic research and adapting it to courses taught even at research-oriented universities.
RECOGNIZING THE PROBLEM
I have taught introductory biology for the past 15 years. For the first 6 years, I used a standard lecture format, and on the basis of student evaluations, the course was a success; I routinely received excellent scores for both the course and for me as an instructor. The course was “working,” so what made me think I needed to change anything? Two recurring experiences made me wonder about the effectiveness of my approach. First, colleagues who taught upper division courses would occasionally stop me in the hall and make comments such as: “Didn't you cover transcription and translation in your course? I have students from your class and they don't even seem to know the difference.” Certainly we had covered those topics. And those comments were not unique to my course. Why was it that even the top students were not retaining a clear understanding of this material? The second revelation came from office hours. A student would come to see me expressing a total lack of understanding of a topic. I would explain the material to them and they would nod along. At the end of my explanation they would thank me and indicate that everything was now clear; however, if I asked them a simple question to verify their understanding, they were unable to answer, even though moments ago they had professed an understanding of what I had explained. What was going on? It occurred to me that what was happening in office hours was a microcosm of what was happening in my lecture course. I knew the material, and I could explain it in a clear and organized manner so that the students understood what I was talking about, but that did not mean my students were learning it. So what was the problem?
The problem had two origins: our students and us. This combination, which essentially amounts to an unstated collusion between instructor and learner, creates a pernicious and self-reinforcing cycle. As faculty members, we need to realize the difference between teaching and learning. Just because I was a good teacher did not mean that my students were learning. The research in learning and teaching makes it clear that lecturing promotes passive learning, and passive learning is not the most effective way to learn (Figure 1). In fact, “passive learning is an oxymoron” (Leamson, 2000). There is increasing, and I would say undeniable, evidence that students learn better when they take an active role in the learning process (e.g., Cooper and Robinson, 2000; Davis, 1993; Lord, 1994, 2001; McKeachie, 1999; National Research Council, 2000; and references therein). Lecturing has its place (Bonwell, 1996); for example, it can be entertaining when carried out by a skilled practitioner. But lecturing is not a good approach when it is used as the sole or primary means of getting students to learn. The pedagogical literature has many examples of this point, so there is no need, and insufficient space, to reiterate it throughout this article — lecturing by itself is relatively ineffective as a learning tool.
We as faculty members are at fault because we are using an outdated methodology. Before the development of the printing press, the only way to transmit information was by word of mouth. With printed materials, and now electronic resources, other means of information delivery might be more effective. But there is plenty of blame to go around, because our accomplices in this educational deceit are the students. As one of my students once wrote, “Education is the only business where the consumer is satisfied with less product.” In general, students do not prepare for class and they do not keep up. They have found it unnecessary for success in the course and they are pragmatists with many demands on their time. But these study habits have huge negative consequences for the course. Because my students did not read before class, I had no choice but to lecture to them; otherwise, they would not know what I was talking about. By not reading the book or studying their notes after class, they made it impossible for me to build on concepts presented in previous lectures; when I said, “As you remember from last time … ,” they had no idea what we covered last time, and I was perfectly aware of that. In fact, there is often a tacit understanding between the students and the instructor: Don't expect too much of me and I won't expect too much of you.
I wanted to break out of this cycle that forced me to lecture, an ineffective method of teaching. But how should I teach and what approach should I use? My first insight came from a presentation in which I was introduced to the concept of constructivism. Constructivism states knowledge cannot be transferred from one person to another; knowledge must be created by each person in order for him or her to learn it. In addition, pedagogical research suggests that one of the best ways for students to learn is by teaching other students (McKeachie et al., 1986). Finally, as opposed to lecturing, active learning is an effective way to facilitate the acquisition of knowledge. I wanted to develop an approach that utilized these three concepts: 1) students must construct their own knowledge; 2) learning often works best as a collaborative effort; 3) active engagement in the learning process is critical. “Students learn best when they take an active role: when they discuss what they are reading, practice what they are learning, and apply concepts and ideas” (Davis, 1993).
However, I needed a method that recognized certain constraints. First, at a research university, the time I can devote to teaching is limited: If I devote excessive time to improving my teaching at the expense of my research, my career will pay the price. I wanted a method that was simple and that would fit within the time demands of an active researcher. It must be inexpensive because funds for improving teaching are limited. Finally, it must not depend on technological fixes, because the ability to implement technological changes might be limited. I wanted to develop a simple, low-cost, low-tech approach.
DEVELOPMENT OF AN ACTIVE LEARNING APPROACH
Given these conceptual goals and practical constraints, what method did I develop? Students could learn the basic material as well on their own as they could if I read it to them, which is essentially what I would be doing if I lectured. How could I get students to read the material prior to class? If I tried to reason with them and explained that lecturing was not an effective way to promote learning, they would read and keep up — for about the first week, until they got math homework and a chemistry quiz, etc., and then they would stop studying for my class until the midterm.
My solution was to give my students good notes. These notes are concise but complete; they are what I would say if I were lecturing to them. But the amount of reading is quite reduced compared with a standard textbook, perhaps an average of three pages per class hour; a standard text is available as an option for students who are willing to devote additional time to course preparation. Accordingly, I could demand that the students do this minimal amount of reading from my notes prior to class. I also included guideline questions to emphasize key points. For example, the reading for cellular energetics will include the following types of guideline questions: “What is the purpose of fermentation (under what conditions does it happen)?” and “With regard to energy production, what is the main product of the TCA cycle?” To ensure that the students did the reading, I administered a reading quiz. This quiz was factual in nature and was meant to test whether they had read the notes. For example, a typical reading quiz would have the following types of questions: “What is the purpose of glycolysis?” and “What is the role of oxygen in the electron transport chain?” The reading quizzes were administered at the start of every class. This quiz constitutes approximately half of the course grade, so the students need to take it seriously (Figure 2).
If I made no other change aside from instituting the reading quiz, I would have made major progress. The reading quiz forces the students to keep up, an unusual experience for most of them. Students have told me during the course how good it felt to be following along with the class. These students ask whether this approach — keeping up — would work for their other classes! When the students read ahead, they come to class with a certain sense of ownership of the material. They are no longer passive blank slates waiting to accept my words of wisdom. Most importantly, because the students have done the reading, I do not need to lecture. Typically, I will lecture for a few minutes, but the majority of class time is spent on problem solving.
We often teach as though the particular facts we are covering are critical when in reality there are few specifics that most students retain from their undergraduate education (Figure 3). Fortunately, retaining factoids is not as important as the learning concepts represented by factual examples or, more generally, learning how to think and solve problems. I want my introductory students to be scientifically literate, but I also want them to be able to think about problems that they have never encountered. Lecturing and note taking does little to develop such higher order thinking skills (Sorcinelli, 1991; Svinicki, 1999). In contrast, I break my class up into groups and put a problem up on the overhead projector. For the discussion of energetics, a problem might include the following: “Prokaryotes do not have a mitochondria like eukaryotes and therefore cannot use cellular respiration as a method of attaining energy. Is this true or false? Explain.” or “The TCA cycle does not use oxygen directly. Can the TCA cycle proceed in the absence of oxygen?” and “When an uncoupling agent such as CCCP is added to a flask of respiring tissue culture cells, oxygen consumption continues but little ATP is made. Why?”
I ask the class to discuss the problem within their groups and later ask for a group's answer. We hear each group's answer and discuss both the correct and incorrect answers — I want my students to see the thinking process that goes into solving the problem, not just to see which answer is right. I de-emphasize memorization. Certainly the students need to memorize some facts, but that is where the typical course ends. I emphasize using facts to solve problems. Unless we show our students how to gather, process, and apply information, they will not have learned very much that is useful beyond the course exams. To ensure participation in the problem solving, I administer a second quiz that is concept oriented and that constitutes the other half of the course grade. The concept quiz includes questions such as: “Describe what happens to your muscle cells if you exercise strenuously and run out of oxygen.” and “What is the effect of an uncoupler on ATP production through oxidative phosphorylation?” I administer this quiz at the start of the following class session so the students have some time to seek help if they are having difficulty. A typical day then consists of a concept quiz for the previous day of class, a reading quiz on that day's material, a short lecture and predominantly a problem-solving session.
EVALUATION OF THE ACTIVE LEARNING APPROACH
Does this method work? I have attempted to determine whether I have actually improved the course. First, I carried out an “objective” evaluation. To do this, I compared student performance on primarily fact-based, multiple-choice questions to evaluate the effectiveness of the active-learning format. Students who learned through the problem-solving format were asked to answer identical questions to those administered to students who were taught through a lecture format. Although this evaluation was undeniably biased in various ways, the results suggested that students learned information more effectively through the problem-solving format (Klionsky, 1998, 2001).
As a second method of evaluation, I compared these methods subjectively. In my experience, most faculty members devote little thought to the development of their own pedagogical approach (Klionsky, 2003). Although most people reading this article could easily think of various resources if they were trying to devise a protocol for an experiment in the lab, probably few could name even a single reference (aside from this journal) to guide them in pedagogical development. But such resources certainly exist. For example in 1986, a study was initiated that evaluated 50 years of research on teaching and learning (Chickering and Gamson, 1987). This study led to the proposal of “seven principles for good practice in undergraduate education.” The seven principles are only a guideline for instructors, but they have set a foundation for curricular revision across the nation. Below, I briefly compare the lecture and active learning formats with regard to the seven principles.
Good practice in undergraduate education:
Encourages contact between students and faculty. With the problem-solving approach, the instructor's role is primarily to facilitate group interactions while students discuss problems. The students have an opportunity to ask questions more freely and see the instructor in a different light. As a result, they become more willing to interact with the instructor. With this approach, the instructor sets the tone for an interactive environment rather than a one-way transfer of information from teacher to student as typically occurs during a lecture. Also, it is interesting to note that there is a high correlation between student–teacher interaction and teacher effectiveness, but a very low frequency of this behavior in natural sciences (Murray and Renaud, 1995).
Develops reciprocity and cooperation among students. Having students help each other learn is the primary feature of the group problem-solving format. In contrast, the lecture format typically focuses on individual competition: memorizing facts and getting high scores on the exams. For collaborative learning to be most effective, it is important to avoid grading on a curve; this eliminates a major aspect of competition (Kohn, 1992; Malacinski and Zell, 1995) so there is no disincentive to helping classmates learn.
Encourages active learning. The lecture/note-taking format is the epitome of passive learning. The approach that I describe encourages two types of active learning that might appeal to different types of students. First, students are responsible for reading the assigned material prior to each class session. They are encouraged to review their notes on a routine basis to prepare for the concept quiz following each topic. Reading and reviewing notes is a type of active learning that can be performed individually. Second, students actively solve problems during class time and in this case, active learning is cooperative.
Gives prompt feedback. Midterms are typically returned at least 1 week after they are administered, by which point the class is discussing a different topic. Final exams are returned after the end of the course, if at all. Generally, neither exam serves as a teaching tool, but rather as a means of summative evaluation. Research suggests that students remember information from a classroom presentation better when it is followed by a test (Menges and Mathis, 1988). Short and frequent quizzes can be graded quickly and returned to the students by the next class session. Quizzes quickly alert both the student and instructor to potential problems in comprehension. In contrast to typical midterms and final exams, quizzes provide a formative means of evaluation, and various studies suggest that this type of feedback is central to learning (Sorcinelli, 1991; Theall, 1999).
Furthermore, I will look at the quizzes as soon as they are collected. I can then make real-time course adjustments. Imagine a class in which you teach based on what the students have actually learned, compared with the standard lecture format in which your goal is to cover the information you have already chosen regardless of whether the students have learned any of it. The in-class problem solving provides an additional avenue for routine evaluation; students can get immediate feedback from their peers when working collaboratively. I provide additional feedback by asking for group responses and going over answers to each problem with the entire class after the discussions by the individual groups. This is in stark contrast to the lecture format, in which students usually do not receive routine feedback about their comprehension during the course of the lecture.
Emphasizes time on task. Not surprisingly, the more time that students devote to mastering a subject, the more they learn (Sorcinelli, 1991). Most students do not study for introductory biology on a routine basis. They allocate their time to meet other more pressing demands. They actively study only when an exam is imminent. The use of daily quizzes forces students to devote routine time to the course.
Communicates high expectations. Students generally appreciate high expectations as long as they are reasonable and if they are supported. The primary expectation of the lecture format is that students attend class and take notes. As a result, students expect to focus on memorization, with a tacit understanding that cramming for the exams is accepted. This “learn and burn” mentality emphasizes getting a good grade, not on learning or retaining the material. With the active learning strategy, students bear the major responsibility for learning. The frequent quizzes provide a set of routine and obtainable goals. When students perform well on these quizzes, they are motivated to continue making progress.
Respects diverse talents and ways of learning. People learn in different ways and active learning gives them the opportunity to work alone and in groups. Students are better able to retrieve information from long-term memory when it fits within the realm of their personal experiences (Svinicki, 1999); that is, the more relevant the material is to their own lives, the greater the likelihood that they will become engaged in the learning process and be able to remember it (Leamson, 2000). In addition, research suggests that the group approach benefits women and minorities (Anderson and Adams, 1992; Marchesani and Adams, 1992).
DIFFICULTIES (REAL OR IMAGINED) IN IMPLEMENTING ACTIVE LEARNING
The biggest variable in the learning process is what the students (learners) bring with them, not what the instructor provides. For this reason, it is important to use teaching strategies that motivate students and show them ways to actively learn long after they have left a particular course. In considering the above information, one question becomes obvious: If it is so clear that active learning is a better approach, why isn't everyone using it?
Faculty members do not know that there are alternatives. We teach the way we were taught, which was usually through a lecture and note-taking format. Also, few faculty members (particularly at research-oriented universities) read any pedagogical literature. Finally, many people are hesitant to give up control of their class; there is a comforting feeling being the “expert” who has all the answers (Figure 4). Along these lines, there is a concern that the students will not participate in the active learning approach. I found that the students picked up my own conviction in the value of this approach.
As (research) faculty members, we are too busy. Many people think they do not have the time to make major changes in the way they teach; that is why I wanted to develop an approach that was not time intensive. It is also worth noting that the course material does not have to change completely or even extensively when changing to an active learning format. For example, I cover the same topics but my learning goals shifted from memorization of facts to developing scientific literacy and problem-solving skills.
Concern over depth of coverage versus breadth. Many instructors think that if time is spent solving problems in class, they cannot cover as much material. In fact, I cover as much with the active learning approach as I used to when I lectured; I just make the students take responsibility for learning part of the information on their own. In addition, I am not convinced that it is important to cover “everything” if the students are not learning anything (Figure 5).
We need to understand the difference between teaching and learning (National Research Council, 2000). Both students and teachers share responsibility for this confusion. Professors try to spoon-feed students, and the students keep asking for more; they don't want to take responsibility for learning. Also, the students want their tuition's worth, which they see as a lecture by an expert. We are evaluated on the basis of our performance as judged by students who don't necessarily have a long-term viewpoint. We should be evaluated on the basis of whether our alumni learned (National Research Council, 2003).
Large classes pose logistical problems. Some people think lecturing is the only possible way to handle a large class; however, I have used this active learning approach with a class of 240. Furthermore, when I taught in 50-min lectures, I had to cover a set amount of material and did not have a lot of time for interacting with the class. I always felt rushed as I tried to fit everything in during each class session. Now the students have already done the reading and covered all of the “essential” information. Whether I lecture for 5 or 10 min, and whether I cover 10 or 20 problems is not critical, so it is a much more relaxing experience.
There is a fixation on technology. Research suggests that technology for the purpose of education is often misused (Theall, 1999). Technology should not drive pedagogy and is only effective when there is a justifiable need for it. Many people equate technology with innovative teaching. For example, there is a common view that a multimedia approach such as the use of PowerPoint solves some perceived problems with the lecture format, although in my experience, few people use this software effectively for teaching (Lanius, 2004; McDonald, 2004; Voss, 2004). Furthermore, some instructors make their slides available ahead of time on the Web, allowing students to bring printouts to class to assist in taking notes. Although this sounds reasonable, I have found this approach produced unexpected problems. Several years ago, when I still used lectures as my primary means of teaching, I handed out my notes at the start of class then proceeded to lecture. Surprisingly, many students in the class complained and asked that I not hand out the notes beforehand. The problem was that some students would sit in class, "follow along" to the extent that they occasionally looked at the board to see where I was in my lecture, and talked with their friends; they were not paying attention to me because they “already had the notes” (McDonald, 2004), and they were interfering with the students who were interested in hearing the lecture. At any rate, the attitude that a change in presentation style will improve a course is somewhat laughable in light of the next point.
Most courses do not have explicit goals, so attempts to assess whether the course is meeting those goals are necessarily absent. Imagine conducting an experiment without evaluating the results. As research scientists, who would ever do that? Yet that is precisely the way we conduct our teaching (Klionsky, 2003). Do we objectively evaluate whether our students are learning? If we implement changes, do we have any way of determining whether they actually improved the ability of students to learn? The lack of goals and assessment (National Research Council, 2003) have led to the common acceptance of the lecture format because after all, “the current methods have “worked' for decades.”
Support and recognition for innovative teaching are lacking from colleagues, department chairs, or deans (National Research Council, 2003). Part of the academic culture at research universities is that serious scientists don't spend any more time than necessary on teaching.
Finally, I have to conclude with a simple question: do I advocate the elimination of all lecturing? The answer is, certainly not. I continue to lecture in my own course, although it is a minor part of the course. There is no single best way to facilitate learning, and lectures remain an effective way to deliver some types of information. But if you rely primarily on lecturing, I would have to ask another question: Do you know this is the most effective way to get your own students to learn? If you have not experimented with active learning, you cannot answer this question. As a scientist, why would you make an assumption that lecturing is meeting your learning goals without conducting the obvious experiment? We can't expect students to learn how to think scientifically when we don't apply the scientific method to our pedagogy (Klionsky, 2003). There is a trend away from teaching to learning, from a focus on facts and memorization to concepts and problem solving, from the use of passive to active learning formats, and from atmospheres that favor individual competition to ones that promote collaborative learning. These trends reflect ongoing changes in the way we do science, which is becoming more collaborative and interdisciplinary, in which we often work in teams and the focus is on conceptual problems and an effort to solve complex questions. At least with regard to introductory biology, the method I have described is much more effective than a lecture-style course at meeting these goals.
Acknowledgments
I thank Lisa Klionsky and Mary Lee Ledbetter and A. Malcolm Campbell for helpful comments on the manuscript. D.J.K. is supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation as part of the Director's Award for Distinguished Teaching Scholars.
REFERENCES
Anderson J.A., Adams M. Acknowledging the learning styles of diverse student populations: implications for instructional design. In: Border L.L.B., Chism N.V.N., editors. In: Teaching for Diversity: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 49. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1992. pp. 19–33. [Google Scholar]
Bonwell C.C. Enhancing the lecture: revitalizing a traditional format. In: Sutherland T., Bonwell C.C., editors. In: Using Active Learning in College Classes: A Range of Options for Faculty. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1996. pp. 31–44. [Google Scholar]
Chickering A.W., Gamson Z.F. Seven principles for good practice in undergraduate education. AAHE Bull. 1987;39:3–7. [Google Scholar]
Cooper J.L., Robinson P. Getting started: informal small-group strategies in large classes. In: MacGregor J., Cooper J.L., Smith K.A., Robinson P., editors. In: Strategies for Energizing Classes: From Small Groups to Learning Communities: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 81. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2000. pp. 17–24. [Google Scholar]
Davis B.G. Tools for Teaching. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1993. [Google Scholar]
Klionsky D.J. Application of a cooperative learning approach to introductory biology. J. Coll. Sci. Teach. 1998;27:334–338. [Google Scholar]
Klionsky D.J. Constructing knowledge in the lecture hall. J. Coll. Sci. Teach. 2001;31:246–251. [Google Scholar]
Klionsky D.J. Why the scientific method matters: a cautionary tale. Teach. Prof. 2003;17:4. [Google Scholar]
Kohn A. No Contest:the Case Against Competition. New York: Houghton Mifflin Company; 1992. [Google Scholar]
Lanius C. PowerPoint, not your grandmother's presentations, but is it evil? Cell Biol. Educ. 2004;3:158–160. [Google Scholar]
Leamson R. Learning as biological brain change. Change. 2000;32:34–40. [Google Scholar]
Lord T.R. Using constructivism to enhance student learning in college biology. J. Coll. Sci. Teach. 1994;23:346–348. [Google Scholar]
Lord T.R. 101 Reasons for using cooperative learning in biology teaching. Am. Biol. Teach. 2001;63:30–38. [Google Scholar]
Malacinski G.M., Zell P.W. Learning molecular biology means more than memorizing the formula for tryptophan. J. Coll. Sci. Teach. 1995;25:198–202. [Google Scholar]
Marchesani L.S., Adams M. Dynamics of diversity in the teaching-learning process: a faculty development model for analysis and action. In: Adams M., editor. In: Promoting Diversity in College Classrooms: Innovative Responses for the Curriculum, Faculty, and Institutions: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 52. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1992. pp. 9–19. [Google Scholar]
McDonald K. Examining PowerPointlessness. Cell Biol. Educ. 2004;3:160–161. [Google Scholar]
McKeachie W.J. Teaching Tips: Strategies, Research, and Theory for College and University Teachers. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1999. [Google Scholar]
McKeachie W.J., Pintrich P.R., Lin Y., Smith D. Teaching and Learning in the College Classroom: A Review of the Research Literature. Ann Arbor: National Center for Research to Improve Postsecondary Teaching and Learning, University of Michigan; 1986. [Google Scholar]
Menges R.J., Mathis B.C. Key Resources on Teaching, Learning, Curriculum, and Faculty Development: A Guide to the Higher Education Literature. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1988. [Google Scholar]
Murray H.G., Renaud R.D. Disciplinary differences in classroom teaching behaviors. In: Marincovich M., Hativa N., editors. In: Disciplinary Differences in Teaching and Learning: Implications for Practice: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 64. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1995. pp. 31–39. [Google Scholar]
National Research Council. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience and School: Expanded Edition. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
National Research Council. Evaluating and Improving Undergraduate Teaching in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
Sorcinelli M.D. Research findings on the seven principles. In: Chickering A.W., Gamson Z.F., editors. In: Applying the Seven Principles for Good Practice in Undergraduate Education: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 47. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1991. pp. 13–25. [Google Scholar]
Svinicki M.D. New directions in learning and motivation. In: Svinicki M.D., editor. In: Teaching and Learning on the Edge of the Millenium: Building on What We Have Learned: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 80. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1999. pp. 5–27. [Google Scholar]
Theall M. New directions for theory and research on teaching: a review of the past twenty years. In: Svinicki M.D., editor. In: Teaching and Learning on the Edge of the Millenium: Building on What We Have Learned: New Directions for Teaching and Learning. vol. 80. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1999. pp. 29–52. [Google Scholar]
Voss D. PowerPoint in the classroom, is it really necessary? Cell Biol. Educ. 2004;3:155–156. doi: 10.1187/cbe.04-06-0045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]