Abstract
For the first time, chimpanzees have been observed using tools to clean the corpse of a deceased group member. A female chimpanzee sat down at the dead body of a young male, selected a firm stem of grass, and started to intently remove debris from his teeth. This report contributes novel behaviour to the chimpanzee’s ethogram, and highlights how crucial information for reconstructing the evolutionary origins of human mortuary practices may be missed by refraining from developing adequate observation techniques to capture non-human animals’ death responses.
Results
Social tool use is virtually absent in the animal kingdom. Apart from an early report by McGrew & Tutin1,2, all accounts of non-human animal tool use are in the context of individual activities (e.g. refs 3, 4, 5). Here, we report a new case of social tool use in chimpanzees in a very unusual context: grass tool use for dental corpse cleaning.
Noel, a wild-born female chimpanzee living at the Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage Trust in Zambia, attended the dead body of Thomas, a nine-year-old male whom she adopted (sensu6) when his mother died four years earlier. At the time of the reported observations, Noel was 33 years old. She was brought to Chimfunshi when she was 14 years old, after spending most of her life at a private home in former Zaïre. Autopsy revealed that Thomas had most likely died from a combination of a viral and bacterial lung infection (for more details, see ref. 7). The majority of the group visited Thomas’ body at least once7, but when most chimpanzees were lured away with highly attractive food, Noel remained at his body and cleaned his teeth with a grass tool. Nina, her adolescent daughter, stayed at her side and observed the cleaning efforts of her mother. In more detail, this unique event consisted of the following actions.
Noel approached Thomas’ body, sat down close to his head, turned her upper body sideways to select a hard piece of grass, put the grass in her mouth, and opened Thomas’ mouth with both of her hands. Then she wrapped her fingers around Thomas’ chin and jaw, and used her thumbs to explore his teeth. After 3 sec, she took the grass out of her mouth with her right hand, while maintaining focused grip on Thomas’ mouth with her left hand, and started to meticulously poke the grass in the same dental area as where her thumbs had been. During this time, Noel kept her face within 10 cm of Thomas’ face, never deflecting her gaze from his mouth. After 26 sec of teeth cleaning with the grass, Noel brought the grass to her mouth and inspected the grass orally, seemingly tasting debris removed from Thomas’ mouth. Soon thereafter, Noel engaged in cleaning Thomas’ teeth with the same piece of grass a second time (at least for 5 sec, until the caretakers requested for the recording to end – see Methods), tasting the debris once more (see Figs 1 and 2, and Supplementary Videos 1 and 2).
Discussion
We consider the cleaning of a corpse’s teeth by Noel noteworthy for several reasons. First and foremost, to date, this behaviour has never been reported in chimpanzees or any other non-human animal species. The records of social tool use in animals are limited to McGrew & Tutin’s1,2 descriptions of dental cleaning in live chimpanzees. McGrew & Tutin reported how chimpanzees cleaned and even extracted their own and each other’s teeth – an unprecedented behavioural phenomenon in non-human animals that the authors related to the chimpanzees’ experience with object manipulation and their intimate social relationships2. The current report extends these observations to dental cleaning of a dead conspecific and corroborates McGrew & Tutin’s perspective on the preconditions of this unusual social behaviour in the form of Noel’s intimate relationship with Thomas. Furthermore, the records of death responses in non-human animals to date do not contain any elements of tool use8. In fact, whereas the limited documentations of chimpanzees’ responses in death-related situations range from passive behaviours like resting, to active behaviours like dragging and slapping the dead body9, gentle corpse handling in form of tool-assisted dental cleaning (see Supplementary Videos 1 and 2) has never been reported and warrants scientific interest both from an intraspecific and comparative point of view8. In particular, the reported corpse handling in chimpanzees may suggest that chimpanzees have evolved in complex social organizations conducive to the emergence of post-mortem affiliation10. In other words, chimpanzees may form long-lasting social bonds (e.g. see refs 11 and 12) that continue to influence their behaviour once the bonding partner has died. Like humans, chimpanzees may not treat deceased conspecifics carelessly, but instead handle corpses in a socially meaningful way – i.e. as social beings instead of inanimate objects – especially when this group member is a close associate, as in the reported case13. An alternative, non-mutually exclusive explanation for the reported behaviour may be that it emerged from a motivation to learn about death, perhaps fuelled by a curiosity about the unique circumstances14. Second, in over 8000 hours of observing the chimpanzees at Chimfunshi, including Noel, we have not observed a single bout of a chimpanzee engaging in tool-assisted teeth cleaning. Notwithstanding the reports by McGrew & Tutin1,2, this indicates that Noel engaged in a rather unique and meaningful social activity, all the more given that she forfeited abundant high quality food by doing so (i.e. the caretakers tried to lure the chimpanzees away from the body so it could be removed from the enclosure). The fact that Noel preferred to stay close to Thomas over traveling to the high-quality food being offered by the caretakers furthermore casts doubt on the interpretation that Noel cleaned Thomas’ teeth merely to obtain food.
Anecdotal evidence needs to be treated cautiously15. However, given the availability of high-quality video footage, we present a valid case of unique non-human animal behaviour which could shed light on the evolution of behaviours that are believed to be typically human. Death responses represent core features of human civilization, with great diversity in mortuary rites found across cultures16. In general, for animals critically depending on group living17, like humans and chimpanzees, responding to death may be a means to reorganize the social unit, especially when so-called “brokers” die: individuals who play an important role in maintaining group cohesion by connecting sub-groups18,19. Unfortunately, accounts of dying animals and the responses of their dependents and other group members to their (impending) death are virtually absent from scientific records due to lack of close observation and lack of interest in publishing case studies. The current report on dental corpse cleaning in chimpanzees warrants focused efforts in developing more sophisticated observation techniques to critically capture non-human animals’ (social) behaviour in death-related situations and contributes new behaviour to consider when investigating the evolutionary origins of human death responses.
Methods
The responses of group members to finding the dead body of Thomas were recorded with two video cameras from the moment that Thomas’ body was discovered until the caretakers requested for the recording to end so that they could make an attempt to climb the fence and retrieve the body from the enclosure. The recording comprised 20 min of uninterrupted filming and was previously used to investigate the group’s responses to the dead body in terms of proximity and individual differences based on association history with the deceased7. In the current report, we focused on one peculiar behaviour during the group’s response to Thomas’ dead body – tool use for corpse cleaning – in order to significantly expand the chimpanzee’s ethogram20, and highlight how we may miss out on crucial information for reconstructing the evolutionary origins of human mortuary practices by refraining from developing adequate observation techniques to capture non-human animals’ death responses21. We inferred from independent interviews with local caregivers (>4 years working with the same chimpanzees) and personal observations by the lead author during a 4-mon period in 2007 that Noel had adopted Thomas by measures of close association, co-feeding (especially during competitive feeding sessions, i.e. the distribution of small portions of high quality food) and co-traveling to nesting sites. All methods were carried out in accordance with the Pan African Sanctuary Alliance guidelines and Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage Trust regulations. The research was non-invasive and complied with the ethical guidelines of the Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage Trust.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: van Leeuwen, E. J. C. et al. Tool use for corpse cleaning in chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 7, 44091; doi: 10.1038/srep44091 (2017).
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank the Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage Trust for their ongoing support of our research. We thank Nick Wood of the technical team of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen for helping us with the video files.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author Contributions E.J.C.v.L. coded the videos. E.J.C.v.L. wrote the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
References
- McGrew W. C. & Tutin C. E. G. Chimpanzee dentistry. Journal of American Dental Association 85, 1198–1204 (1972). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrew W. C. & Tutin C. E. G. Chimpanzee Tool Use in Dental Grooming. Nature 241, 477–478 (1973). [Google Scholar]
- Levy D. J., Scot Duncan R. & Levins C. F. Animal behaviour: Use of dung as a tool by burrowing owls. Nature 431, 39 (2004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiten A., Horner V. & de Waal F. B. M. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437, 737–740 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutz C. et al. Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature 537, 403–407 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boesch C., Bolé C., Eckhardt N. & Boesch H. Altruism in forest chimpanzees: the case of adoption. PLoS One 5, e8901 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Leeuwen E. J. C., Mulenga I. C., Bodamer M. D. & Cronin K. A. Chimpanzees’ Responses to the Dead Body of a 9-Year-Old Group Member. American Journal of Primatology 78, 914–922 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson J. R. Comparative thanatology. Current Biology 26, 553–556 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart F. A., Piel A. K. & O’Malley R. Responses of chimpanzees to a dead community member at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 74, 1–7 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCorkle W. W. Ritualizing the disposal of the deceased: From corpse to concept. New York, NY, Peter Lang Publishing (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Langergraber K., Mitani J. & Vigilant L. Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 71, 840–851 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitani J. C. Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal Behaviour 77, 633–640 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- White C. J., Marin M. & Fessler D. M. T. Not just dead meat: An evolutionary account of corpse treatment in mortuary rituals. Journal of Cognition and Culture 17, 146–168 (2017).
- Cronin K. A., van Leeuwen E. J. C., Mulenga I. C. & Bodamer M. D. Behavioral response of a chimpanzee mother toward her dead infant. American Journal of Primatology 73, 415–421 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarringhaus L. A., McGrew W. C. & Marchant L. F. Misuse of anecdotes in primatology: lessons from citation analysis. American Journal of Primatology 65, 283–288 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkes C. M., Laungani P. & Young W. (eds) Death and Bereavement Across Cultures: Second edition. London: Routledge (2015). [Google Scholar]
- van Schaik C. P. Why Are Diurnal Primates Living in Groups? Behaviour 87, 122–144 (1983). [Google Scholar]
- Lusseau D. & Newman M. E. Identifying the role that animals play in their social networks. Proc R Soc Lond B 271, 477–481(2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Rohr et al. Impartial third-party interventions in captive chimpanzees: A reflection of community concern. Public Library of Science ONE 7, p. e32494 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishida T., Zamma K., Matsusaka T., Inaba A. & McGrew W. C. Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild. An Audio-Visual Encyclopedia. Springer (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Pettitt P. The paleolithic origins of burial. London & New York, Routledge (2011). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.