ABSTRACT
Infection avoidance behaviors are the first line of defense against pathogenic encounters. Behavioral plasticity in response to internal or external cues of infection can therefore generate potentially significant heterogeneity in infection. We tested whether Drosophila melanogaster exhibits infection avoidance behavior, and whether this behavior is modified by prior exposure to Drosophila C Virus (DCV) and by the risk of DCV encounter. We examined 2 measures of infection avoidance: (1) the motivation to seek out food sources in the presence of an infection risk and (2) the preference to land on a clean food source over a potentially infectious source. While we found no evidence for preference of clean food sources over potentially infectious ones, previously exposed female flies showed lower motivation to pick a food source when presented with a risk of encountering DCV. We discuss the relevance of behavioral plasticity during foraging for host fitness and pathogen spread.
Keywords: avoidance behavior, Drosophila, DCV, foraging, Infection
Introduction
Hosts vary considerably in their ability to acquire and transmit infection,1-3 and much of this variation is caused by differences in the contact rate between susceptible individuals and sources of infection.4,5 For example, viruses of Drosophila fruit flies are not only widely distributed, they also show very broad host range.6 Given the high viral prevalence of pathogens in natural environments, mounting a timely and efficient immune response to all possible pathogenic challenges would be physiologically costly and ultimately ineffective. Hosts capable of reducing the probability of contacting parasites, infected conspecifics or infectious environments can therefore not only prevent the deleterious effects of infection, but also circumvent the undesirable energetic costs of immune responses, including immunopathology.4,7 Avoiding infection is therefore the first line of non-immunological defense against infection,8 and is known to occur across a broad range of host taxa.7,9
Like most traits, infection avoidance behaviors are likely to vary according to the context of infection, and pathogens are major drivers of this context.4,7,9-11 Pathogens may alter host responses in 2 ways. First, by altering the immunophysiology of the host during infection, pathogens can modify host behavior.12,13 Common behavioral changes in infected individuals include increased sleep and lethargy, or reduced feeding, mating, parental care or foraging (reviewed in12,13). In addition to internal, physiological cues of infection, external cues that indicate the magnitude of infection risk are also known to influence host behavioral responses.4,7 Understanding variation in infection avoidance behaviors therefore provides an important functional link between the neurological, behavioral and immunological processes that together govern the spread of disease.12
Insects are ideal systems to investigate the interplay between infection and behavior.12,14 The fruit fly Drosophila is especially amenable to these studies, as it is one of the best developed model systems for host-pathogen interactions15 and behavioral ecology and genetics.16,17 One of the most studied pathogenic interactions in Drosophila is the host response to systemic and enteric infection with Drosophila C Virus (DCV).18,19 DCV is a horizontally transmitted +ssRNA virus that naturally infects the fly gut,19-21 causing intestinal obstruction, severe metabolic dysfunction and eventually death.22,23 As a consequence of its pathology, female flies infected with DCV are also known to exhibit behavioral modifications, such as reduced locomotion and increased sleep.24 The Drosophila-DCV interaction therefore offers a powerful system to investigate the ecological consequences that may arise from the physiological and behavioral effects of enteric viral infections.
In the present study we used a combination of controlled experimental infections and foraging choice assays to test whether adult D. melanogaster are able to avoid potentially infectious environments, and if avoidance behavior is modified in response to virus exposure history and to different risks of acquiring DCV infection. We find evidence for avoidance behaviors in the form of reduced motivation to seek out and land on provided food sources according to the risk of infection. These effects were clearest in female flies previously exposed to DCV, indicating potentially important sexual dimorphism in infection avoidance.
Results
Viral exposure prior to the behavioral assay was achieved by placing flies in a DCV contaminated environment for 3 days, allowing flies to acquire DCV infection orally. DCV acquired through the oral route using this protocol continued to replicate within the fly, increasing by 10–100 fold by day 13 following oral exposure (F4,19 = 8.78, p = 0.0003; Fig. 1A) and in both male and female flies resulted in up to 20% mortality within this period (Fig. 1B).
To measure infection avoidance, we took 2 approaches. First, we hypothesized that the motivation to seek out food sources would be lower in environments where the risk of infection is higher.7 We therefore compared the proportion of flies that chose to seek out and land on any of the provided food sources in the “no risk” and “high-risk” cages. Only a fraction of flies chose either of the food sources provided, and this proportion increased over time for flies in all treatment groups (χ21 = 11.00, p = 0.001; Fig. 2A). The rate at which motivation increased differed between sexes (‘Time × Sex' interaction, χ21= 12.47, p=0.0004), and on average female flies showed greater motivation to forage than males (χ21 = 5.01, p = 0.025), with 67% of female and 36% of male flies making a choice to land on any of the provided substrates during the observation period.
Across the entire observation period, the motivation to seek out and land on any of the provided food sources differed between sexes, and depended both on their previous exposure and on their current risk of infection (Fig 2B; ‘Sex’ × ‘risk of infection’ × ‘Previous exposure’ interaction, χ21 = 21.82, p < 0.0001). The proportion of males choosing any food substrate did not vary with previous exposure to DCV in either high-risk (χ21 = 2.21, p=0.137) or no-risk environments (χ21 = 0.09, p = 0.764; Fig. 2).
In female flies however, previous exposure and current infection risk affected the motivation to land on the provided food sources. When there was no risk of infection (Fig. 2B, light gray bars) the motivation to seek out a food source was greater in females that were previously exposed to DCV than in otherwise healthy, non-exposed females (χ21 = 104.11, p < 0.001). Among females that were previously exposed to infection, we found that the presence of a risk of acquiring infection resulted in lower foraging effort - with just over 50% of flies choosing a food source - compared to females in cages where there was no risk of acquiring infection, where over 80% of flies made the choice to land on a food source (Fig. 2B; χ21 = 168.48, p < 0.001).
We also asked whether flies that chose to feed showed any evidence of avoiding potentially infectious food sources. For this analysis we focused on the “high risk” cages and recorded the proportion of flies choosing the clean food source over the infectious food source in each replicate cage. Once flies had made the choice to land on one of the provided food sources, the choice between a clean and a potentially infectious food source was not affected by previous exposure to DCV (‘previous exposure’, χ21 = 0.513, p = 0.47) in either male or female flies (‘sex’, χ21 = 0.595, p = 0.44) (Fig. 3).
Discussion
The ability to detect and discriminate between clean and potentially infectious environments is vital to avoid the adverse consequences of infection. In this study we found that the motivation of female Drosophila melanogaster to seek out food sources is modified by its previous exposure to a viral pathogen and by the risk of encountering infection during foraging. Behavioral plasticity due to infection is widely reported among animals,9,25 and can be classified into (i) parasitic manipulation that enhances parasite transmission9 (ii) sickness behaviors that benefit the host by conserving energetic resources during infection,13 or (iii) side-effects of pathogenicity that do not benefit the host or the parasite.25
Female flies infected orally with DCV are known to experience increased lethargy and sleep,24 so these effects could also explain the reduced food seeking activity we detected in female flies that had been previously exposed to DCV. Another potential explanation for reduced motivation to find a food source in previously exposed flies is infection-induced anorexia,26 a commonly described sickness behavior.13 However, it is unlikely that lower motivation is simply a symptom of a “sick” fly, because in our experiment it varied according to the risk of infection, and even reached 80% in exposed flies when foraging in a ‘no risk’ environment (Fig. 2). This suggests that flies are actively avoiding contact with the potentially contagious food source by lowering their foraging effort.
The higher motivation of some female flies to seek out a food source when the risk of infection was absent (Fig. 2) suggests flies were able to identify external cues of infection risk. Identifying infection cues is a general prerequisite of avoidance behaviors and occurs across a wide range of different taxa. For example, lobsters are known to detect and avoid virus-infected conspecifics27; fruit flies and nematodes are capable of avoiding pathogenic bacteria28,29; gypsy moth larvae are able to detect and avoid virus-contaminated foliage14; sheep have been found to prefer to graze in parasite-poor patches30; and it is has been argued that the disgust response in humans has evolved because it decreases contact with potential infection.31 It is unclear how flies are able to identify food sources contaminated with a viral pathogen. In Drosophila and C. elegans avoidance of pathogenic bacteria is enabled by evolutionary conserved olfactory and chemosensory pathways,28,29 while avoidance of parasitic wasps appears to be mainly enabled by the visual sensory system.32 While avoiding virus infected conspecifics is probably driven by visual cues of infection,27 it remains unclear how virus-contaminated environments may trigger a lower motivation to feed in Drosophila.
The fact that only female flies demonstrated avoidance is an indication that any potentially adaptive effects of avoiding infection may be related to oviposition, which coincides with feeding. For flies previously exposed to DCV, avoiding infection would not confer substantial direct benefits given the physiological and behavioral costs of this infection,22-24 but would however reduce the exposure of future offspring to infection. While flies previously exposed to DCV do not appear to immune primed following an initial viral exposure,33 our results point to a sort of behavioral priming, where females previously exposed to infection avoid foraging in potentially infectious environments. Future work should therefore focus on testing how oviposition decisions are affected by female infection status and by external cues of infection. Oviposition decisions are likely to be critical for organismal fitness, especially if the ability of larvae to void infectious environments is reduced32
In summary, using a combination of experimental infections and behavioral assays, we find evidence that Drosophila can avoid infectious environments by showing reduced motivation to seek out a food source, which was most pronounced when flies were faced with an increased risk of encountering an infectious food source. However, these effects were only present in female flies, indicating potentially important sexual dimorphism in infection avoidance. Understanding how avoidance behaviors may vary is therefore important for our understanding of how disease will spread in natural populations,4 and more broadly how pathogens might evolve in response to variation in host infection avoidance strategies.34,35
Materials and methods
Fly and virus stocks
All flies used were from a long-term laboratory stock of Wolbachia-free Drosophila melanogaster Oregon R (OreR) line, maintained on Lewis medium in standard conditions: 25°C, with a 12:12 h light:dark cycle. Fly stocks were routinely kept on a 14-day cycle with non-overlapping generations under low larval densities. The DCV culture used in this experiment was grown in Schneider Drosophila Line 2 (DL2) as described in.24 Ten-fold serial dilutions of this culture (diluted in Ringers buffer solution) were aliquoted and frozen at −80°C for long-term storage before use.
Virus exposure
Flies used in the foraging choice assays were obtained by preparing 10 vials of Lewis medium and yeast containing 10 mated females. Flies were allowed to lay eggs for 48 hours resulting in progeny reared in similar larval densities. To test the effect of previous exposure to virus on avoidance behavior during foraging, we exposed the progeny to DCV via the oral route of infection 2 to 3 d after eclosion. Oral DCV infection causes a small but significant reduction in fly survival19 and also experience changes in fecundity and fecal shedding (Vale, unpublished data), activity and sleep.24 Single-sex groups of 20 flies were placed in vials containing agar previously sprayed with DCV (“exposed” to 50 µl of 108 viral copies/ml) or the equivalent volume of Ringers buffer solution as a control (“not exposed”). This procedure produced 10 replicate vials of either healthy or virus-exposed male or female flies (Fig. 4). The viral dose used here was lower than previously reported methods,19 so we first tested this dose was sufficient to result in viable DCV infections by measuring changes in virus titres and fly survival in separate experiments (Fig. 1). Fly survival was monitored on 9 replicate groups of 12 OreR flies per vial for 11 d following oral exposure. To measure changes in DCV titer, 2five, 2–3 day-old female flies were individually housed in vials previously sprayed with DCV as described above for 3 d. Five flies were collected 1, 3, 6, 9 or 13 d after exposure and total RNA was extracted from flies homogenized in Tri Reagent (Ambion), reverse-transcribed with M-MLV reverse transcriptase (Promega) and random hexamer primers, and then diluted 1:10 with nuclease free water. qRT-PCR was performed on an Applied Biosystems StepOnePlus system using Fast SYBR Green Master Mix (Applied Biosystems). We measured the relative fold change in DCV RNA relative to rp49, an internal Drosophila control gene, calculated as 2−ΔΔCt as described in.36
Foraging choice assays
Following 3 d of virus exposure, we set up independent foraging choice assays in cages - cylindrical transparent plastic containers (12 cm in diameter × 15 cm in height) containing 2 equally spaced plastic vials of standard Lewis fly medium supplemented with dry yeast (Fig. 4). For each combination of “DCV exposed” and “not exposed” male or female flies, we set up 2 sets of cages to simulate different risks of infection: a “no risk” environment, with 2 clean vials (sprayed with sterile Ringers solution), and a “high-risk” environment where one of the vials was sprayed with DCV, as described above. Six replicates of 20-fly groups were allocated to the “high-risk” chambers and 4 replicates to the “no risk” chambers, resulting in a total of 40 independent foraging choice cages (Fig. 4). Flies were transferred without anesthesia with the aid of an aspirator directly from vials into a neutrally placed hole in the lid of each chamber. The number of flies that settled on each vial was recorded every 30 minutes for 5 hours. Care was taken to randomize the position of the cages so that the orientation of the light did not influence the choice of the flies in any systematic way.
Statistical analysis
In both analyses of ‘motivation to feed’ and ‘infection avoidance’, data on the proportion of flies choosing each food source within each replicate cage were analyzed with a generalized linear model assuming binomial error and logit link function, and included fly ‘sex’, ‘previous exposure’ and ‘infection risk’ as fixed effects. ‘Replicate cage’ was included as a random effect, nested within treatments. We also analyzed the average motivation to feed and infection avoidance across all time points, in a model that included “time” as a random effect. Treatment specific contrasts were used to test the significance of pairwise comparisons. Analyses were carried out using JMP 12.37
Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest
No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.
Acknowledgments
We thank H. Cowan and H. Borthwick for preparing fly medium, K. Monteith and S. Lewis for assistance with the qPCR, the members of the Vale, Obbard and Ross labs for helpful suggestions.
Funding
This work was supported by a strategic award from the Wellcome Trust for the Center for Immunity, Infection and Evolution (grant reference no. 095831), and by a Society in Science – Branco Weiss fellowship, both awarded to P. Vale. P.Vale is also supported by a Chancellor's Fellowship from the School of Biological Sciences, University of Edinburgh.
Author contributions
PFV conceived the study. PFV and MDJ designed the experiment. MDJ and PFV carried out the experimental work. PFV analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript and provided all research consumables.
References
- [1].Fellous S, Duncan AB, Quillery E, Vale PF, Kaltz O. Genetic influence on disease spread following arrival of infected carriers. Ecol Lett 2012; 15:186-92; PMID:22221658; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01723.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Vale PF, Little TJ. Measuring parasite fitness under genetic and thermal variation. Heredity 2009; 103:102-9; PMID:19455181; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1038/hdy.2009.54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Susi H, Barrès B, Vale PF, Laine A-L. Co-infection alters population dynamics of infectious disease. Nat Commun 2015; 6:5975; PMID:25569306; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1038/ncomms6975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Barron D, Gervasi S, Pruitt J, Martin L. Behavioral competence: how host behaviors can interact to influence parasite transmission risk. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2015; 6:35-40; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Paull SH, Song S, McClure KM, Sackett LC, Kilpatrick AM, Johnson PT. From superspreaders to disease hotspots: linking transmission across hosts and space. Front Ecol Environ 2012; 10:75-82; PMID:23482675; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1890/110111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Webster CL, Waldron FM, Robertson S, Crowson D, Ferrari G, Quintana JF, Brouqui J-M, Bayne EH, Longdon B, Buck AH, et al.. The Discovery, Distribution, and Evolution of Viruses Associated with Drosophila melanogaster. PLOS Biol 2015; 13:e1002210; PMID:26172158; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Curtis VA. Infection-avoidance behaviour in humans and other animals. Trends Immunol 2014; 35:457-64; PMID:25256957; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.it.2014.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Parker BJ, Barribeau SM, Laughton AM, de Roode JC, Gerardo NM. Non-immunological defense in an evolutionary framework. Trends Ecol Evol 2011; 26:242-8; PMID:21435735; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.tree.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Moore J. An overview of parasite-induced behavioral alterations – and some lessons from bats. J Exp Biol 2013; 216:11-7; PMID:23225862; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1242/jeb.074088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Wolinska J, King KC. Environment can alter selection in host-parasite interactions. Trends Parasitol 2009; 25:236-44; PMID:19356982; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.pt.2009.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Vale PF, Salvaudon L, Kaltz O, Fellous S. The role of the environment in the evolutionary ecology of host parasite interactions. Infect Genet Evol 2008; 8:302-5; PMID:18346944; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.meegid.2008.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Adamo SA. Comparative psychoneuroimmunology: evidence from the insects. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2006; 5:128-40; PMID:16891555; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1177/1534582306289580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Adelman JS, Martin LB. Vertebrate sickness behaviors: Adaptive and integrated neuroendocrine immune responses. Integr Comp Biol 2009; 49:202-14; PMID:21665814; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/icb/icp028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Parker BJ, Elderd BD, Dwyer G. Host behaviour and exposure risk in an insect-pathogen interaction. J Anim Ecol 2010; 79:863-70; PMID:20384645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Neyen C, Bretscher AJ, Binggeli O, Lemaitre B. Methods to study Drosophila immunity. Methods San Diego Calif 2014; 68:116-28; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.ymeth.2014.02.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Dubnau J. Behavioral Genetics of the Fly (Drosophila Melanogaster). Cambridge University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- [17].Sokolowski MB. Drosophila: Genetics meets behaviour. Nat Rev Genet 2001; 2:879-90; PMID:11715043; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1038/35098592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Dostert C, Jouanguy E, Irving P, Troxler L, Galiana-Arnoux D, Hetru C, Hoffmann JA, Imler J-L. The Jak-STAT signaling pathway is required but not sufficient for the antiviral response of drosophila. Nat Immunol 2005; 6:946-53; PMID:16086017; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1038/ni1237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Ferreira ÁG, Naylor H, Esteves SS, Pais IS, Martins NE, Teixeira L. The Toll-Dorsal Pathway Is Required for Resistance to Viral Oral Infection in Drosophila. PLoS Pathog 2014; 10:e1004507; PMID:25473839; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.ppat.1004507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Huszar T, Imler J. Drosophila Viruses and the Study of Antiviral Host‐Defense [Internet] In: Advances in Virus Research. Academic Press; 2008. page 227-65. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065352708004065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Kapun M, Nolte V, Flatt T, Schlötterer C. Host Range and Specificity of the Drosophila C Virus. PLoS ONE 2010; 5:e12421; PMID:20865043; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0012421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Arnold PA, Johnson KN, White CR. Physiological and metabolic consequences of viral infection in Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol 2013; 216:3350-7; PMID:23685974; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1242/jeb.088138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Chtarbanova S, Lamiable O, Lee K-Z, Galiana D, Troxler L, Meignin C, Hetru C, Hoffmann JA, Daeffler L, Imler J-L. Drosophila C virus systemic infection leads to intestinal obstruction. J Virol 2014; 24:14057-69; PMID:25253354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Vale PF, Jardine MD. Sex-specific behavioural symptoms of viral gut infection and Wolbachia in Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Physiol 2015; 82:28-32; PMID:26301521; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2015.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Barber I, Dingemanse NJ. Parasitism and the evolutionary ecology of animal personality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2010; 365:4077-88; PMID:21078659; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1098/rstb.2010.0182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Ayres JS, Schneider DS. The Role of Anorexia in Resistance and Tolerance to Infections in Drosophila. PLoS Biol 2009; 7:e1000150; PMID:19597539; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Behringer DC, Butler MJ, Shields JD. Ecology: Avoidance of disease by social lobsters. Nature 2006; 441:421-421; PMID:16724051; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1038/441421a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Babin A, Kolly S, Schneider F, Dolivo V, Zini M, Kawecki TJ. Fruit flies learn to avoid odours associated with virulent infection. Biol Lett 2014; 10:20140048; PMID:24598110; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Meisel JD, Kim DH. Behavioral avoidance of pathogenic bacteria by Caenorhabditis elegans. Trends Immunol 2014; 35:465-70; PMID:25240986; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.it.2014.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Hutchings M, Knowler K, McAnulty R, McEwan J. Genetically resistant sheep avoid parasites to a greater extent than do susceptible sheep. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2007; 274:1839-44; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1098/rspb.2007.0398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Curtis V, de Barra M, Aunger R. Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:389-401; PMID:21199843; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1098/rstb.2010.0117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Kacsoh BZ, Lynch ZR, Mortimer NT, Schlenke TA. Fruit Flies Medicate Offspring After Seeing Parasites. Science 2013; 339:947-50; PMID:23430653; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1126/science.1229625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Longdon B, Cao C, Martinez J, Jiggins FM. Previous Exposure to an RNA Virus Does Not Protect against Subsequent Infection in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE 2013; 8:e73833; PMID:24040086; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0073833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Boots M, Bowers RG. Three mechanisms of host resistance to microparasites-avoidance, recovery and tolerance-show different evolutionary dynamics. J Theor Biol 1999; 201:13-23; PMID:10534432; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1006/jtbi.1999.1009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].McLeod DV, Day T. Pathogen evolution under host avoidance plasticity. In Proc. R., Soc. B, vol. 282, no. 1814, p. 20151656. The Royal Society, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Livak KJ, Schmittgen TD. Analysis of relative gene expression data using real-time quantitative PCR and the 2(-Delta Delta C(T)) Method. Methods San Diego Calif 2001; 25:402-8; http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1006/meth.2001.1262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].JMP Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc:. [Google Scholar]