Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Mar 21.
Published in final edited form as: Psychopathol Rev. 2016 May 21;4(1):52–108. doi: 10.5127/pr.038315

Aetiological influences on stability and change in emotional and behavioural problems across development: a systematic review

LJ Hannigan 1, N Walaker 1, MA Waszczuk 1, TA McAdams 1, TC Eley 1
PMCID: PMC5360234  EMSID: EMS70231  PMID: 28337341

Abstract

Emotional and behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence can be chronic and are predictive of future psychiatric problems. Understanding what factors drive the development and maintenance of these problems is therefore crucial. Longitudinal behavioural genetic studies using twin, sibling or adoption data can be used to explore the developmental aetiology of stability and change in childhood and adolescent psychopathology. We present a systematic review of longitudinal, behavioural genetic analyses of emotional and behavioural problems between ages 0 to 18 years. We identified 58 studies, of which 19 examined emotional problems, 30 examined behavioural problems, and 9 examined both. In the majority of studies, stability in emotional and behavioural problems was primarily genetically influenced. Stable environmental factors were also widely found, although these typically played a smaller role. Both genetic and environmental factors were involved in change across development. We discuss the findings in the context of the wider developmental literature and make recommendations for future research.

Keywords: Childhood, adolescence, psychopathology, systematic review, behavioural problems, emotional problems, quantitative genetics, behavioural genetics, longitudinal

Introduction

Early-life emotional and behavioural problems constitute an important risk factor for future mental and physical health problems (e.g., Bardone et al., 1998; Harrington, Fudge, Rutter, Pickles, & Hill, 1990; Hofstra, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2000, 2002; Kim-Cohen et al., 2003). However, these problems can begin to pose difficulties to individuals and their families from the moment they emerge in childhood or adolescence, with potential for associated disruption across a wide range of social, cognitive and educational domains (Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000; Hinshaw, 1992; Larsson & Frisk, 1999; McLeod & Kaiser, 2004). Studying the aetiology of the emergence and continuity of emotional and behavioural problems across development therefore has important implications for both current difficulty and future risk. First, identifying the importance of different aetiological factors at different stages of development can inform as to the optimal nature and timing of interventions. Second, understanding how childhood emotional and behavioural problems emerge and are maintained may help to clarify the pathways by which risk for future mental health problems is mediated.

Emotional and behavioural problems across childhood and adolescence

Emotional problems (often termed internalising problems) include anxiety, depression and associated symptoms and behaviours. Emotional problems are highly prevalent in childhood and adolescence (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003; Merikangas, He, Brody, et al., 2010), with recent figures estimating lifetime prevalence at age 18 as high as 14.3% for mood disorders and 31.9% for anxiety disorders (Merikangas, He, Burstein, et al., 2010). In both cases, females (18% and 28% respectively) experience a greater number of emotional problems than males (10.5% and 26.1% respectively). Emotional problems begin early, with mean onset in childhood for anxiety disorders (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005) and early adolescence for depressive disorders (Kovacs & Devlin, 1998). Furthermore, temperamental factors, such as behavioural inhibition, that are associated with the development of emotional problems, are in evidence from very early in childhood (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995; Degnan, Almas, & Fox, 2010; Rapee, Schniering, & Hudson, 2009). Individuals who experience emotional problems in childhood and adolescence are at a significantly increased risk of developing subsequent psychiatric problems in young adulthood and beyond (e.g., anxiety: Gregory et al., 2007; addiction: Lopez, Turner, & Saavedra, 2005; mood disorders: Roza, Hofstra, Van Der Ende, & Verhulst, 2003; suicidality: Weissman et al., 1999).

Behavioural (or externalising) problems include difficulties associated with attention, hyperactivity, conduct problems, aggression and antisocial behaviour. They are highly prevalent across both childhood and adolescence, with 20-23% of children estimated to experience a behavioural disorder by age 16 years (Costello et al., 2003; Merikangas, He, Burstein, et al., 2010). The high prevalence of behavioural problems is particularly driven by attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and conduct problems, which have been shown to have 12-month prevalence rates of 8.6% and 2.1% respectively during middle childhood and adolescence (Merikangas, He, Brody, et al., 2010). In contrast to emotional problems, behavioural disorders are more common in males (23.5%) than females (15.5%; lifetime prevalence of any behavioural disorder at age 18; Merikangas, He, Burstein, et al., 2010). Behavioural disorders vary relatively widely across childhood and adolescence in terms of their age-of-onset (Kessler, Berglund, et al., 2005). However, temperamental dispositions that are predictive of later behavioural problems are again reliably observable very early in development (e.g. aggression: Tremblay et al., 2005).

Stability of psychopathological traits across development, sometimes referred to as homotypic continuity, is moderate for both emotional (Ollendick & King, 1994; Weems, 2008) and behavioural problems (Hofstra et al., 2000; Klein, Otto, Fuchs, Reibiger, & von Klitzing, 2015; Verhulst & van der Ende, 1995). While homotypic continuity is evident from early in development (e.g., Anselmi et al., 2008; Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson, Rose, & Klein, 2012), it is typically thought to increase during adolescence for both emotional (Costello et al., 2003; Ferdinand, Dieleman, Ormel, & Verhulst, 2007) and behavioural problems (Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, 2009; Mesman & Koot, 2001; Sourander & Helstelä, 2005). However, the developmental presentation of emotional and behavioural problems is also characterised by change. Both childhood and adolescence are periods of extensive brain maturation, accompanied by profound changes in the social environment as well as in hormonal and physical development (Blakemore, 2008; Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, 2008). Emotional and behavioural problems in childhood may therefore present in very different ways to these same problems in adolescence. Specifically, adolescence may be a period of phenotypic differentiation for emotional problems, with the generalised emotional problems that characterise earlier ages disaggregating, increasingly, into distinguishable mood and anxiety disorders (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; Hallett, Ronald, Rijsdijk, & Eley, 2009; Waszczuk, Zavos, Gregory, & Eley, 2014). Depressive symptoms, in particular, are seen to increase after the onset of adolescence (Dekker et al., 2007; Ge, Conger, & Elder, 2001), driving an increase in the incidence of depression that is accompanied by the emergence of significant gender differences (Hankin et al., 1998; Thapar, Collishaw, Pine, & Thapar, 2012). Patterns are mixed across the traits underpinning behavioural problems in development, with mean levels of some externalising behaviours increasing in adolescence (e.g., status violations: Bongers, Koot, Ende, & Verhulst, 2004; antisocial behaviour: Moffitt, 1993) and others decreasing (e.g., aggression: Bongers et al., 2004; ADHD symptoms: Rutter, Kim-Cohen, & Maughan, 2006). In general, for both emotional and behavioural problems, there is evidence that the stability of relevant traits is disrupted during adolescence in particular (Hofstra et al., 2000).

Genetic influence on emotional and behavioural problems in development

The study of emotional and behavioural problems in childhood is complicated by the complex entanglement of genetic and environmental factors that underpin them (Rutter & Silberg, 2002; Rutter, 2004). Behavioural genetic designs can be used to unpick the aetiology of behavioural phenotypes and identify the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors (Jaffee, Price, & Reyes, 2013; Rijsdijk & Sham, 2002; Turkheimer, 2000). In the context of this review, we use 'behavioural genetic designs' to refer to studies using genetically informative data from twin, sibling or adoption samples, where information about the differing degrees of genetic relatedness between participants within a sample - for example, identical and non-identical twin pairs - can be used to decompose the sources of variance in individuals' scores on quantitative measures of behaviour. Structural equation modelling of these data is used to produce estimates of the genetic and environmental influence on a given behaviour (Plomin, DeFries, Knopik, & Neiderhiser, 2013; Rijsdijk & Sham, 2002). These estimates, including the estimate of heritability, are population-level statistics that apply to the group from which the individuals were drawn. As such, they can only be used to make inferences about the aetiological origins of differences between individuals in a given population, rather than having an interpretable meaning for any one individual.

Genetically informative studies of measures of emotional and behavioural problems are numerous, and have typically shown that both genetic and environmental factors are important in their development (Burt, 2014; Lau & Eley, 2010; Rhee & Waldman, 2002; Rice, Harold, & Thapar, 2002; Rutter, Silberg, O'Connor, & Simonoff, 1999). Cross-sectional behavioural genetic estimates for measures of both emotional and behavioural problems tend to show age-related increases in heritability, suggesting that their aetiological architecture is not fixed across development (Bergen, Gardner, & Kendler, 2007). In some cases, increases in heritability may reflect relative decreases in the influence of (especially shared) environmental factors (e.g., Lamb et al., 2010; Scourfield et al., 2003). However, changes in the specific nature of the genetic and environmental influences that operate at different ages are also possible. For example, genetic innovation is observed when new genetic factors come online to influence emotional or behavioural problems developmentally. In this way, certain genes may only function from adolescence onwards. The influence of these new genetic factors may or may not result in an increase in heritability, depending on the extent to which earlier factors become less influential over time - a process termed genetic attenuation (Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008). Different genetic influences may become influential as different behaviours and cognitive processes become involved in driving the symptoms of emotional and behavioural problems at different stages of development.

Studies that combine genetically informative data with developmental context are crucial in developing our understanding of the emergence and persistence of emotional and behavioural problems in development. Longitudinal behavioural genetic designs are, thus, ideally suited to the developmental study of psychopathology. With data collected from the same individuals at multiple time points, it is possible to estimate the extent to which various aetiological influences operate stably, contributing to continuity in the phenotype over time, or innovatively, driving phenotypic change (Boomsma, Busjahn, & Peltonen, 2002). In this way, developmental questions concerning the extent of continuity or discontinuity of various emotional and behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence can be expanded to address the aetiological nature of the developmental patterns that are observed. These longitudinal, behavioural genetic studies of emotional and behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence are the subject of this review.

The current review

The aim of the current review is to systematically collate, present and appraise the evidence regarding aetiological contributions to stability and change in emotional and behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence. In particular, this review will provide an overview of the degree of convergence of results on the sources of phenotypic stability and change across different samples, measures and modelling strategies. By reviewing studies of emotional and behavioural problems together, with a strict focus on the sources of variance in homotypic continuity and discontinuity, we aim to answer two interrelated questions. First: to what extent is phenotypic stability in childhood and adolescent psychopathology underpinned by genetic versus environmental factors? Second: to what extent are the genetic and environmental influences upon emotional and behavioural problems stable versus innovative across development?

Method

Search strategy

The literature search was performed using OvidSP on the following databases: Ovid Medline (1946 - April 2015); Embase (1947 - April 2015); PsycINFO (1806 - April 2015); Journals@Ovid Full Text (April 2015 update) and PsycARTICLES Full Text. We ran the searches for emotional and behavioural problems separately. Search terms for emotional problems were as follows: depress*, mood, emotion*, affective disorder*, internali*, anxi*, worry,fear*, obses*, compul*, OCD, panic, phobi*, inhibit*, shy*, withdrawn, somatic. Search terms for behavioural problems were: behav*, attenti*, inattenti* externalising, externalizing, conduct disorder*, ADHD*, hyperactiv*, impuls*, disruptive*, problem*, aggress*, violen*, crimin*, deviant*, delinquen*, oppositional, ODD. Both searches also included terms designed to produce a list of results that used genetic methods (genes, genetic*, aetiolog*, etiolog*, twin*, adopt*, hertiab*) applied longitudinally (longitudinal, stab*, change*, innovat*, continuity, development*, child* & adolescen*). Age criteria were only applied during manual screening to avoid missing relevant studies that did not include 'childhood' or 'adolescence' in their titles. Additional search steps included scanning of the reference lists of recent studies (since 2010), and manual searches of journals containing the more than 5 of the studies identified via the database searches. This allowed us to reduce the possibility of non-indexed studies being missed.

Inclusion criteria

Studies were required to meet the following criteria for inclusion in this review:

  1. The results must have been presented as a full paper published in a peer-reviewed journal1

  2. The study design was required to include longitudinal, behavioural genetic analysis of data. Studies using other genetic designs (e.g., molecular genetic studies) were excluded. Studies using genetically informative data with designs that do not examine genetic influence (e.g., monozygotic twin differences) were also excluded.

  3. Studies had to include any measure of at least one childhood emotional or behavioural problem. Phenotypes for inclusion were:
    • Emotional problems: depression or depressive symptoms; mood disorders; anxiety; worry; fear; obsessive-compulsive behaviours; panic symptoms; phobias; shyness; inhibition; general emotional problems, somatic complaints.
    • Behavioural problems: attention problems; hyperactivity; violent behaviour; conduct disorder; aggression; delinquency; peer deviance; criminality; general behavioural problems
  4. Phenotypes must have been measured at least twice between the ages of 0 and 18 years, with data analysed longitudinally between these ages. Studies meeting these criteria but with further waves of measurement beyond the age of 18 years were included, with only results from waves within the 0-18 range presented in the review.

  5. Studies were required to have been written in English.

Procedure

Study selection

The results of the database searches were checked for duplicates, which were removed. The titles of the remaining results were then used to identify those records that were clearly irrelevant to the current review (e.g., molecular genetic studies, review papers, commentaries). Following this process, the remaining records were exported from the database, along with their abstracts and other information. The abstracts, titles and format (e.g., full paper, conference abstract, etc.) of these records were then screened for further exclusions. All steps were carried out independently by two of the authors (LJH and NW) and, at this point, inclusion and exclusion lists were compared and discrepancies reviewed.

Final exclusions were made based on independent screening of full papers still included at this stage. Any further discrepancies and the inclusion or exclusion of studies using non- standard genetic models was discussed with a third author (TAM). The process of study selection is summarised in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Selection process for inclusion of studies in the review synthesis.

Figure 1

Data extraction

Data were extracted from studies for presentation in the results section of this review. The fields for data extraction were the following: (1) study sample, including sample name if provided; (2) number of participants, given in terms of pairs of twins or siblings where possible; (3) phenotype(s) studied and measure(s) used, including mode(s) of measurement or (if questionnaire) reporter(s); (4) mean age(s) and age range(s) of participants at each eligible wave of data collection; (5) type of genetic model(s) used in analyses; (6) stability of phenotype(s) between all eligible waves and the proportion of this stability due to genes; (7) influence of stable genetic factors, given in terms of the proportion of phenotypic variance at the later age(s) explained by genetic factors from the earlier age(s); (8) influence of stable shared (if estimated) and non-shared environmental factors; (9) influence of new genetic factors, given in terms of the amount of phenotypic variance at later ages explained by factors emerging at these ages; and (10) influence of new shared (if estimated) and non-shared environmental factors.

Data extraction was carried out and cross-checked by LH and NW. We contacted the authors of studies for which the desired information was not included in either the paper or online supplementary materials.

Data synthesis

Among studies included in the review, the twin design was the most common (see 'Study sample' in Results section for details). An introduction to the use of structural equation modelling in twin studies is presented in Box 1. The breadth of the measures of emotional and behavioural problems and methodological variation involved in the studies included in this review precluded a meta-analysis of findings. Specifically, the studies included in the review varied in phenotype, measure, reporter, age, interval of measurement, modelling strategy, and handling of sex differences. Consequently, we instead took steps to ensure that results could be presented in as comparable a manner as possible. However, studies differed in the way in which genetic and environmental influences on stability and change were presented. In part, this was due to the use of a range of different structural equation models across the included studies. The most common genetic model was the longitudinal Cholesky decomposition (Box 2, Figure B), in which components of variance from early waves are able to influence variance at later waves. Residual variance at later waves that is not explained by these earlier aetiological factors is then decomposed separately, with the resulting variance components free to explain variance at subsequent waves. Accordingly, we have presented (where possible) each study's findings regarding aetiological contributions to stability and change in the manner typically used in the description of a longitudinal Cholesky decomposition: in terms of the amount of variance at each wave (% of total) explained by stable, pre-existing factors versus emerging, new factors (see Box 2). Where modelling results could not be converted to this format, we have presented information on the aetiology of stability and change as described in Box 3 or as indicated on a case-by-case basis in the tables and text of the results section. The majority of studies included in this review used one, or a combination of the models presented in Box 2 & Box 3. In cases where the models used in a study were non-standard adaptations of the models presented in the boxes, we have used the authors' original terminology (e.g., 'Developmental model', 'Transmission model'). Space limitations preclude in-text descriptions of each individual model in this review and, as such, we refer readers to the original articles for full details.

Box 1. Structural equation modelling in twin studies.

The classical twin design involves comparing the degree of similarity between MZ (sharing 100% of their genes) and DZ (sharing on average 50% of their segregating genes) twin pairs. These relative differences in within-pair correlations allow estimations of the influences caused by additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C) and non-shared environmental (E) components of variance. The components can be estimated as follows:

A=2(rMZrDZ)C=rMZAE=1A+C

The A component of variance captures the summed influence of all additive genetic effects on the phenotype and is evident when MZ twins are more phenotypically similar than DZ twins. C captures all non-genetic (i.e., environmental) similarity and is evident when DZ twin correlations are greater than half the magnitude of MZ twin correlations. E captures all non-genetic influences that make individuals in the same family different from one another, including measurement error. E is evident in the extent to which MZ twins, sharing all their genes and rearing environment, do not correlate perfectly for a given phenotype. Figure A shows an example of a univariate twin model, in which these components are estimated.

In cases where the extent of genetic similarity for a phenotype exceeds that which would be expected from the additive genetic overlap (e.g., for the twin model, if rDZ < 0.5 * rMZ), non-additive influences (D) are indicated and can be estimated instead of C. When estimated, D component of variance captures the effect of any interactions between alleles within (dominance) and across (epistasis) loci. For a more detailed introduction to structural equation modelling in behavioural genetics, see Plomin et al. (2013) and Rijsdijk & Sham (2002).

Figure A. Univariate twin model.

Figure A

Box 2. Behavioural genetic analysis of longitudinal data: Cholesky decomposition, correlated factors solution and simplex model.

In a longitudinal Cholesky decomposition (Figure B), the relative influence of stable, pre-existing aetiological factors is equivalent to the squared parameter estimates for cross-time paths into a variable (e.g., for T3, the paths running from A1/C1/E1 and A2/C2/E2 to T3). Similarly, the influence of new aetiological factors at T3 is equal to the squared estimates of the paths between variance components A3/C3/E3 and variable T3.

Where Cholesky decompositions were not presented in the studies, several sets of results could be presented comparably, thanks to the mathematical equivalence of other commonly used models (see Loehlin, 1996 for a full explanation). For example, Cholesky-equivalent estimates of aetiological contributions to stability and change can therefore be derived from the correlated factors solution (Figure C) and the simplex model (Figure D). The simplex model differs only in its separation of innovative effects (e.g., Ai), which can be transmitted along lateral paths, and time-specific effects (e.g., As), which are unique to one wave only.

Results from these models are presented entirely comparably in the majority of cases. For an outline of the differences in the conceptualisation of stability and change associated with common and independent pathway models, see Box 3.

Figure C. Correlated factors solution.

Figure C

Figure D. Simplex model.

Figure D

Figure B. Longitudinal Cholesky decomposition.

Figure B

Box 3. Behavioural genetic analysis of longitudinal data: Common and independent pathway models.

Figure E shows an independent pathway model, which was used in several studies included in this review. In this model, aetiological influences are sub-divided into those that are common to all waves (AC, CC, EC) and those specific to each wave (AS, CS, ES). In this way, the conceptualisation of aetiological stability is subtly different to the Cholesky decomposition and related models. In the independent pathway model, aetiological stability is estimated by squaring the path coefficient from a common aetiological factor (e.g., AC) to an observed variable (e.g. T2). New aetiological influences are time specific variance components at, in this example, T2 and T3.

The common pathway model (Figure F) is a more constrained model, for which the conceptualisation of stability and change is similar to the independent pathway model. Stable variance common to all waves is modelled as an underlying latent factor (L), with one set of variance components (AC, CC, EC). Stable aetiological influence at a specific wave is therefore represented by these variance components, weighted by the path loading between the latent factor and the observed variable.

Figure E. Independent pathway model.

Figure E

Figure F. Common pathway model.

Figure F

We opted to exclude cross-lagged models from our review. This was because the stability- change interpretation sometimes applied to the results of cross-lagged model assumes that genetic influences are transmitted between waves in a manner that is directly proportionate to their relative importance at the first wave. In contrast, the Cholesky decomposition (and other included models) allows direct estimation of genetic influence on the covariation between traits (for further exploration of this issue, see Luo, Haworth, & Plomin, 2010).

Results

Study sample

In total, 58 separate eligible studies, published between 1993 and 2015, were identified and included in the review. Of these, 19 involved only emotional problems, 30 involved only behavioural problems and 9 involved at least one phenotype from each. 55 studies used twin data, while 5 used sibling or adoption data, either additionally or exclusively. Figure 2 illustrates the approximate ages spanned by studies included in the review.

Figure 2. Frequency distribution showing the number of included studies at each age between 0-18 years.

Figure 2

Table 1 and Table 2 present the results from all studies included in the review. Studies are organised alphabetically by author name and presented along with basic information. The text summary below is organised by phenotype, with a view to highlighting any emerging developmental themes in the results of studies of similar sub-types of emotional and behavioural problems. We have endeavoured to specify the age ranges of studies referred to in the text where possible. Where the terms 'childhood' and 'adolescence' are used descriptively without any age range specified in parentheses, we use these terms to refer to ages between 0-12 years and 13-18 years respectively.

Table 1. Included longitudinal quantitative genetic studies of emotional problems in childhood and adolescence.

Emotional problems
Study Sample (Name, N) Phenotype(s) (measure; reporter) Age at each wavea Genetic modelb Phenotypic stability (% due to genes where calculable) Influence of stable genetic factorsc Influence of stable environmental factorsd C / E Influence of new genetic factorse Influence of new environmental factorsf C / E
Bartels et al. (2004)g Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 1481 - 5115 twin pairs Internalising behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 3 years Developmental model W1-W2: .38 (52%) 6% 19% / 1% 38% 9% / 27%
W2: 7 years W1-W3: .34 (43%)
W2-W3: .62 (51%)
22% 25% / 3% 14% 9% / 28%
W3: 10 years W1-W4: .31 (32%)
W2-W4: .57 (40%)
W3-W4: .68 (38%)
18% 29% / 8% 19% 8% / 18%
W4: 12 years

Bolhuis et al. (2014) Genesis 1219 study (G1219): 1597 - 2651 individual twins Depressive symptoms & obsessive compulsive symptoms (SMFQ / SCAS; self / self) W1: M = 15 years (12-21) Cholesky decomposition Depressive symptoms (DS)
W2: M = 17 years (14-23) W1-W2: .44 (64%) 18% (2%)/3% (0%) (12%)/55%
Controlling for: W1 OCS Controlling for: W1 OCS

Obsessive compulsive symptoms (OCS)
W1-W2: .48 (63%) 18% (0%) / 4% (0%) (0%) / 52%
Controlling for: W1 DS Controlling for: W1 DS Controlling for: W1&2 DS Controlling for: W1&2 DS

Cherny, Fulker, Corley, Plomin, & DeFries (1994) MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study: 301 twin pairs Shyness (home observation / laboratory task; observer) W1: 1.2 years Common Pathway model (developmental) Home observation:
W2: 1.7 years W1-W2: .35 (92%) 23% 1% / 0% 0% 4% / 72%

Laboratory task:
W1-W2: .29 (55%) 10% 7% / 0% 0% 25% / 58%

Garcia et al. (2013) Minnesota Twin Family Study (MTFS): 756 twin pairs Trait anxiety (STAI-C at W1 / STAI at W2 & W3; self) W1: M = 14.18 years (SD = .51) Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .42 (58%) 39% (1%) / 4% (0%) (0%) / 55%
W2: M = 18.16 years (SD = .65)
[W3: M = 21.46 years (SD = .77)]

Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, & Hewitt (2005)g Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS): 175-252 twin pairs Internalising behaviour (TRF; teacher) W1: 7 years Developmental model (Average across sex)
W2: 8 years W1-W2: .30 (100%) 25% 0% 23% 52%
W3: 9 years W1-W3: .21 (100%)
W2-W3: .33 (100%)
23% 0% 16% 61%
W4: 10 years W1-W4: .19 (100%)
W2-W4: .28 (100%)
W3-W4: .36 (100%)
30% 0% 6% 64%
W5: 11 years
W6: 12 years W1-W5: .26 (100%)
W2-W5: .28 (100%)
W3-W5: .28 (100%)
W4-W5: .28 (100%)
25% 0% 12% 62%
W1-W6: .14 (100%)
W2-W6: .22 (100%)
W3-W6: .28 (100%)
W4-W6: .29 (100%)
W5-W6: .38 (100%)
28% 0% 10% 62%

Hoekstra, Bartels, Hudziak, Van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma (2008) Netherlands Twin Register: 14889 twin pairs Withdrawn Behaviour (CBCL; mother & father) W1: 3 years Cholesky decomposition (rater bias model) Rater-agreedh withdrawn behaviour:
W2: 7 years Males:
W3: 10 years W1-W2: .30 (87%) 77% 0% / 1% 18% 0% / 2%
W4: 12 years W1-W3: .27 (69%)
W2-W3: .56 (50%)
48%
25%
1% / 4%
0% / 0%
5% 0% / 1%
W1-W4: .28 (77%)
W2-W4: .55 (68%)
W3-W4: .63 (36%)
59%
46%
13%
1% / 1%
0% / 6%
0% / 0%
7% 0% / 1%

Females:
W1-W2: .29 (72%) 52% 1% / 1% 16% 0% / 1%
W1-W3: .27 (60%)
W2-W3: .56 (39%)
36%
15%
6% / 7%
0% / 0%
3% 0% / 1%
W1-W4: .25 (59%)
W2-W4: .48 (63%)
W3-W4: .58 (27%)
35%
39%
7%
7% / 2%
0% / 6%
1% / 0%
5% 0% / 1%

Huizink, van den Berg, van der Ende, & Verhulst (2007)g Dutch sample of adopted siblings: 106 biologically-related (BR) and 230 unrelated (BUR) pairs Internalising behaviour (CBCL; adoptive parent) W1: BR M = 12.5 years (SD = 1.2); BUR M = 12.4 years (SD = 1.2) Common pathway model (developmental) W1-W2: .57 (18%) 23% 24% / 34% (0%) (0%) / 20%
W2: BR M = 15.8 years (SD = 1.2); BUR M = 15.6 years (SD = 1.2)
[W3: 26 years]

Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al. (2008) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1237 twin pairs, 16 single twins Situational, animal, blood/injury fears (12-item rating scale; parent & self) W1: 8-9 years (parent report only) Cholesky decomposition (rater bias model) Rater-agreedh situational fears:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .44 (81%) 25% 0% / 6% 44% 5% / 20%
W3: 16-17 years W1-W3: .39 (87%)
W2-W3: .78 (76%)
23%
28%
0% / 1%
0% / 13%
18% 4% / 12%

[W4: 19-20 years] Rater-agreedh animal fears:
W1-W2: .52 (31%) 8% 14% / 10% 36% 28% / 5%
W1-W3: .53 (15%)
W2-W3: .88 (48%)
2%
41%
22% / 12%
7% / 2%
2% 9% / 3%

Rater-agreedh blood/injury fears:
W1-W2: .39 (59%) 12% 3% / 2% 55% 15% / 14%
W1-W3: .30 (90%)
W2-W3: .80 (79%)
17%
44%
0% / 0%
1% / 10%
13% 0% / 16%

Kendler, Gardner, & Lichtenstein (2008) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1237 twin pairs, 16 single twins Mixed anxiety and depression (CBCL / YSR; parent / self) W1: 8 to 9 years (parent report only) Cholesky decomposition (rater bias AE model) Rater-agreedh mixed anxiety and depression:
W2: 13 to 14 years W1-W2: .51 (77%) 21% 5% 69% 6%
W3: 16 to 17 years W1-W3: .48 (85%)
W2-W3: .83 (85%)
23%
34%
(2%)
14%
28% 0%
[W4: 19 to 20 years]

Krebs, Waszczuk, Zavos, Bolton, & Eley (2014) Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): 3644 - 7834 twin pairs Obsessive Compulsive behaviour (ARBQ; Parent) W1: M = 4.04 years (SD = .13) Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .45 (75%) 19% (1%) / (3%) 42% (1%) / 35%
W2: M = 7.07 years (SD = .25) W1-W3: .35 (80%)
W2-W3: .44 (70%)
8%
13%
(0%) / (1%)
(14%) / 4%
33% (0%) / 27%
W3: M = 9.02 years (SD = .29) W1-W4: .23 (78%)
W2-W4: .32 (59%)
W3-W4: .37 (72%)
W4: M = 16.32 years (SD = .68)
6%
(2%)
8%
(25%) / (0%)
(0%) / 1%
(0%) / 2%
17% (0%) / 38%

Lau & Eley (2006) Genesis 1219 study (G1219): 1820 twin pairs Depressive symptoms (sMFQ; self) W1: M = 14 years (12-19 years) Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .58 (68%) 26% (5%) / 4% 10% (7%) / 48%
W2: M = 15 years (12-21) W1-W3: .40 (76%)
W2-W3: .45 (62%)
16%
27%
(1%) / 3%
(1%) / (1%)
0% (0%) / 51%
W3: M = 17 years (13-23)

Lewis & Plomin (2015)g Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): >3000 twin pairs Anxiety / peer problems (SDQ; parent) W1: 4 years Cholesky decomposition Anxiety:
W2: 7 years W1-W2: .38 (69%) 14% 6% / 1% 30% 10% / 37%
W3: 9 years W1-W3: .38 (58%)
W2-W3: .51 (51%)
10%
6%
17% / 0%
(0%) / 5%
29% (1%) / 30%
W4: 12 years W1-W4: .31 (73%)
W2-W4: .45 (66%)
W3-W4: .51 (60%)
11%
10%
5%
7% / 0%
(0%) / 2%
(1%) / 4%
26% (0%) / 35%

Peer problems:
W1-W2: .29 (89%) 10% (2%) / 1% 50% (1%) / 37%
W1-W3: .26 (100%)
W2-W3: .45 (95%)
10%
21%
(2%) / (0%)
(6%) / 3%
32% (0%) / 25%
W1-W4: .24 (91%)
W2-W4: .41 (82%)
W3-W4: .48 (88%)
7%
12%
10%
(3%) / 0%
(0%) / 2%
(0%) / 2%
42% (0%) / 23%

O'Connor, Neiderhiser, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin (1998)g Nonshared Environment & Adolescent Development (NEAD) Project: 405 individuals including twins and full, half and unrelated sibling pairs Depression (composite from depression sub-scale of BPI [3 raters], CDI [3 raters], and observational report; father/mother/self & observer) W1: 10-18 years Independent pathway model W1-W2: .59 (64%) 32% (0%) / 18% 0% (8%) / 42%
W2: 12-21 years

Plomin et al. (1993) MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study (MALTS): 300 twin pairs Behavioural inhibition (IBR; observer) W1: 1.2 years Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .34 (100%) 27% (0%) / (4%) 18% (0%) / 55%
W2: 1.7 years

Nivard et al. (2015) Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 23,678 twin pairs Mixed anxiety and depression ymptoms (ASEBA & CBCL subscale / YSR; mother / self) W1: 3 years Simplex model W1-W2: .29 12% 2% / 6% 49% 0% / 32%
W2: 7 years W2-W3: .58 16% 2% / 6% 42% 0% / 34%
W3: 10 years W3-W4: .63 11% 3% / 6% 50% 0% / 31%
W4: 12 years (mother report) W4-W4a 8% 11% / 4% 26% 0% / 48%
W4a: 12 years (self report) W4a-W5 31% 5% 20% 44%
W5: 14 years W5-W6: 59 12% 5% 38% 45%
W6: 16 years W6-W7: .70 10% 4% 43% 43%
W7: 18 years
[W9-30: 20 - 62 years]

Schmitz, Fulker, & Mrazek (1995)g Colorado twin sample: 203 - 260 twin pairs Internalising (CBCL; parents) W1: 2.8 years Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .40 (10%) (7%) (26%) / (5%) (30%) (0%) / (32%)
W2: 7.6 years

Scourfield et al. (2003) Population-based South Wales twin registry: 670 twin pairs Depressive symptoms (MFQ; parent) W1: 5-14 years Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .35 (19%) (2%) 40% / 2% 23% (0%) / 33%
W1: 8-17 years

Silberg et al. (1999) Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioural Development (VTSABD): 273 female twin pairs Depression (CAPA symptom scores; child) W1: 8-14.5 years Reduced independent pathway model (incorporating life events) Females only:
W2: 9.5-16 years W1-W2: .33 (92%) 30% 0% 0% 71%

Trzaskowski, Zavos, Haworth, Plomin, & Eley (2012) Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): 3644 - 7834 twin pairs Negative cognition / negative affect / fear / social anxiety (ARBQ; parent) W1: 7 years Cholesky decomposition (correlated factors) Negative cognition:
W2: 9 years W1-W2: .50 (69%) 23% 1% / 4% 34% 10% / 27%

Negative affect:
W1-W2: .45 (59%) 14% 6% / 2% 32% 17% / 29%

Fear:
W1-W2: .52 (67%) 21% 8% / 2% 37% 11% / 20%

Social anxiety:

W1-W2: .54 (71%) 24% 6% / 4% 32% 14% / 21%

Tully, Iacono, & McGue (2010) Minnesota Twin Family Study (MTFS): 756 twin pairs Major Depressive Disorder (DSM-III-R DICA-R [W1], SCI for DSM-III-R [W2 & W3]; interview) W1: M = 14.8 years (SD = .51) Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .24 (33%) (21%) (4%) / (1%) (5%) (0%) / 68%
W2: M = 18.2 years (SD = .65)
[W3: M = 21.5 years (SD = .77)]

van den Oord & Rowe (1997)g National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY): 436 full sibling pairs; 119 half sibling pairs; 122 cousin pairs Anxious/depressed behaviour / dependent behaviour / peer conflict withdrawal (BPI; mother) W1: 4-6 years Genetic liability model (independent pathway model) Anxious/depressed behaviour

W2: 6-8 years W1-W2: .43 (60%) 28% 18% / 0% 0% 0% / 54%
W3: 8-10 years W1-W3: .42 (60%)
W2-W3: .45 (60%)
26% 18% / 0% 0% 0% / 56%

Dependent behaviour:

W1-W2: .42 (59%) 26% 19% / 0% 0% 0% / 56%
W1-W3: .42 (58%)
W2-W3: .44 (59%)
26% 18% / 0% 0% 0% / 56%

Transmisison model Peer conflict withdrawal

W1-W2: .42 (69%) 17% 14% / 0% 8% 0% / 60%
W1-W3: .43 (68%)
W2-W3: .43 (68%)
14% 15% / 0% 8% 0% / 64%

van der Valk, van den Oord, Verhulst, & Boomsma (2003)g Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 1924 - 3873 Internalising behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 3 years Indpendent pathway model Males:

W2: 7 years W1-W2: .35 (70%) 24% 8% / 4% 22% 18% / 24%

Females:

W1-W2: .41(58%) 23% 8% / 4% 11% 27% / 27%

van der Valk, Verhulst, Neale, & Boomsma (1998)g Dutch Adoption Sample: 222 biologically related adolescents adopted together (BR); 422 unrelated adolescents adopted together (BUR); 1484 adolescents adopted singly (AS) Internalising (CBCL; Parent) W1: 10-15 years
W2: 14-17 years
Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .59 (19%) (8%) 29% / 7% (0%) (4%) / 51%

van Grootheest et al. (2007) Netherlands Twin Register: 8083 twin pairs Obsessive Compulsive behaviour (CBCL subscale; parents) W1: 7 years Cholesky decomposition (rater bias model) Males:

W2: 10 years Father ratings:

W3: 12 years W1-W2: .51 (44%) Information unavailable
W1-W3: .44 (43%)
W2-W3: .54 (19%)

Mother ratings:
W1-W2: .55 (57%) Information unavailable
W1-W3: .43 (54%)
W2-W3: .58 (43%)

Females:

Father ratings:

W1-W2: .53 (34%) Information unavailable
W1-W3: .44 (35%)
W2-W3: .52 (16%)

Mother ratings:

W1-W2: .55 (40%) Information unavailable
W1-W3: .47 (27%)
W2-W3: .58 (36%)

Waszczuk, Zavos & Eley (2013) Emotions, Cognitions, Heredity and Outcome (ECHO): 300 twin pairs Anxiety symptoms (SCARED; self-report) W1: 8 years Cholesky decomposition. Panic

W2: 10 years W1-W2: .36 (65%) 28% (2%) (0%. (0%) / 67%
Controlling for: W1 Anxiety Sensitivity

Separation Anxiety

W1-W2: .35 (89%) 35% (1%) (0%) (0%) / 61%
Controlling for: W1 Anxiety Sensitivity

Generalized Anxiety

W1-W2: .37 (66%) 29% (1%) (0%) (0%) / 67%
Controlling for: W1 Anxiety Sensitivity

Waszczuk et al. (2016) Genesis 12-19: 896-1,372 twin and sibling pairs Depression / anxiety disorder symptoms: panic; generalised anxiety; separation anxiety; social anxiety (SMFQ / SCAS; self) W1: 12-21 years Cholesky decomposition Depression:

W2: 14-23 years W1-W2: .47 (83%) 29% (0%) / 2% 18% (0%) / 51%

[W3: 18-27 years] Panic:

W1-W2: .43 (60%) 17% (0%) / 3% 25% (0%) / 54%

Generalized anxiety:

W1-W2: .47 (66%) 21% (0%) / 5% 18% (0%) / 56%

Separation anxiety:

W1-W2: .36 (67%) 14% (0%) / (1%) 27% (0%) / 59%

Social anxiety:

W1-W2: .53 (58%) 23% (0%) / 8% 15% (0%) / 54%

Zavos, Gregory, & Eley (2012) Genesis 1219 study (G1219): > 1300 twin pairs Anxiety sensitivity (CASI at W1&2 / ASI W3; self) W1: M = 14 years (12-19) Cholesky decomposition (AE model) W1-W2: .47 (51%) 13% 9% 19% 59%
W2: M = 15 years (12-21) W1-W3: .37 (81%)
W2-W3: .48 (67%)
20%
14%
1%
3%
(1%) 61%
W3: M = 17 years (14-23)
[W4: M = 20 years (19-27)]

Notes –

a

Mean (M) age given with range and standard deviation (SD) if available; where no range is presented this is because the information was unavailable and is not necessarily indicative of participants being the same age

b

Models estimated additive genetic (A), common environmental (C) and unique environmental (E) components of variance unless otherwise stated;

c / d

Results for influence of stable genetic and environmental factors given as % phenotypic variance in later wave(s) explained by factors from earlier wave(s) where possible; values are aligned with phenotypic stability coefficients in adjacent column – i.e. for values aligned with W2-W3 phenotypic stability, % variance explained is at W3 by stable factors from W2; where single values align with a group of stability coefficients, % variance explained is by stable factors from all prior waves; environmental factors are presented as C / E – where C was not estimated at all in the presented model, only one value is given; non-significant values are given in parentheses where known;

e / f

Results for influence of new genetic and environmental factors given in same format as above; only one value is presented per parameter per wave, in alignment with the appropriate group of phenotypic correlations, i.e. values centrally aligned with W1-W3 and W2-W3 stability coefficients are new aetiological factors at W3;

g

Study includes measures of both internalising and externalising and is therefore included in both tables

h

All results (including correlations) pertain to latent factor rather than observed variables; rater-specific (in case of rater bias models) and scale-specific (in case of common pathway – phenotypic models) error not included

Measures: CBCL: Child Behaviour Checklist; TRF: Teacher Report Form; YSR: Youth Self Report form; SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; BPI: Behaviour Problem Index; (S)MFQ: (Short) Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; SCAS: Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; STAI-C: Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children; ARBQ: Anxiety-Related Behaviours Questionnaire; CDI: Child Depression Inventory; IBR: Infant Behaviour Record; ASEBA: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Measurement; CAPA: Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment; SCI: Structure Clinical Interview; DSM-III-R: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual III (Revised); DICA-R: Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (Revised); (C)ASI: (Child) Anxiety Sensitivity Index

Table 2. Included longitudinal quantitative genetic studies of behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence.

Behavioural problems
Study Sample (Name, N) Phenotype(s) (measure; reporter) Age at each wavea Genetic modelb Phenotypic stability (% due to genes where calculable) Influence of stable genetic factorsc Influence of stable environmental factorsd C / E Influence of new genetic factorse Influence of new environmental factorsf C / E
artels et al. (2004)g Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 1481 - 5115 twin pairs Externalising behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 3 years Developmental model Males:
W2: 7 years
W1-W2: .54 (64%) 20% 15% / 1% 39% 11% / 14%
W3: 10 years W1-W3: .50 (66%)
W2-W3: .73 (74%)
49% 13% / 1% 16% 7% / 13%
W4: 12 years W1-W4: .49 (57%)
W2-W4: .69 (69%)
W3-W4: .76 (73%)
48% 16% / 4% 16% 7% / 9%

Females:

W1-W2: .57 (54%) 19% 20% / 1% 40% 7% / 13%
W1-W3: .48 (45%)
W2-W3: .70 (60%)
29% 25% / 3% 16% 11% / 16%
W1-W4: .46 (44%)
W2-W4: .66 (58%)
W3-W4: .76 (51%)
38% 24% / 4% 13% 9% / 12%

Bezdjian, Tuvblad, Wang, Raine, & Baker (2014) University of Southern California Twin Study of Rsik Factors for Anti-social Behaviour (RFAB): 179 - 560 twin pairs Motor impulsivity (Go No-Go Task; laboratory) W1: M= 9.60 years (9-10) Independent Pathway model W1-W2: .53 (61%) 40% 19% 0% 40%
W2: M= 11.79 years (11-13) W1-W3: .41 (55%)
W2-W3: .63 (45%)
19% 34% 0% 46%
W3: M= 14.87 years (14-15) W1-W4: .31 (56%)
2-W4: .62 (35%)
W3-W4: .60 (25%)
12% 52% 12% 25%
W4: M= 17.28 years (16-18)

Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson, & Larsson (2013) The Swedish Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1480 twin pairs Attention problems (CBCL / YSR; parent / self) W1: 8-9 years Cholesky decomposition (rater bias model) Rater-agreedh attention problems:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .64 (88%) 41% (2%) 41% (15%)
W3: 16-17 years W1-W3: .56 (93%)
W2-W3: .85 (80%)
35%
22%
(1%)
(15%)
26% (1%)

Ebejer et al. (2010) Australian Twin Registry / Colorado Birth Registry / Birth Registries of Norway & Sweden: 989 twin pairs Inattention / hyperactivity-impulsivity (DBRS; parent) W1: M= 6.3 years (SD = 0.3) Inattention:
(% = A+D) (A / D) (A / D)

W2: M= 7.4 years (SD = 0.3) Cholesky decomposition (ADE model) W1-W2: .71 (76%) (1%) / 46% 8% (5%) / (17%) 22%
W3: M= 8.4 years (SD = 0.3) W1-W3: .61 (71%)
W2-W3: .81 (77%)
(0%) / 32%
(6%) /
(19%)
10%
4%
(0%) / (0%) 28%

(Age information from largest [Colorado; 489 pairs] sub-sample) Hyperactivity-impulsivity:
Cholesky decomposition
W1-W2: .65 (91%) 44% (0%) / 2% 24% (8%) / 20%
W1-W3: .61 (88%)
W2-W3: .69 (90%)
37% 18% (0%) / 3% (0%) / (1%) 18% (0%) / 21%

Eley, Lichtenstein, & Moffitt (2003) Young Twins Study (YTS - Sweden): 1186 twin pairs Aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial behaviour (ASB; CBCL; parent) W1: 8-9 years Cholesky decomposition (correlated factors; liability threshold model) Aggressive ASB:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .61 (84%) 45% 1% / 0% 1% 30% / 24%

Non-aggressive ASB:

W1-W2:. 49 (44%) 9% 21% / 0% 35% 21% / 14%

Gagne & Hill Goldsmith (2011) Wisconsin twin sample: 735 - 1000 individual twins Anger (Infant Behaviour Questionnaire / Locomotor Lab TAB; mother/laboratory) W1: 1 year Cholesky decomposition Parent report:

W2: 3 years W1-W2: .34 (85%) 12% 1% 33% 55%

Laboratory measure:

W1-W2: .03 (0%) 0% 0% / 0% 32% 23% / 45%

Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, & Hewitt, (2005)g Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS): 175-252 twin pairs Externalising behaviour (TRF; teacher) W1: 7 years Developmental model (Average acr. sex)
W2: 8 years W1-W2: .49 38% 5% 22% 36%
W3: 9 years W1-W3: .49
W2-W3: .59
40% 12% 13% 35%
W4: 10 years W1-W4: .43
W2-W4: .42
W3-W4: .60
38% 11% 2% 49%
W5: 11 years W2-W4: .42
W3-W4: .60
38% 11% 2% 49%
W6: 12 years W1-W5: .48
W2-W5: .44
W3-W5: .53
W4-W5: .46
43% 0% 8% 49%
W1-W6: .35
W2-W6: .37
W3-W6: .38
W4-W6: .43     W5-
W6: .50
37% 3% 12% 48%

Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, & Hewitt (2006) Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS): 366 twin pairs Aggressive behaviour (CBCL/TRF; parent / teacher) W1: 7 years Developmental model (AE only) Parent report:
W2: 8 years [teacher-report] W1-W3: .69 (86%) 66% 4% 10% 20%
W3: 9 years W1-W4: .64 (92%)
W3-W4: .78 (85%)
66% 6% 10% 18%
W4: 10 years
W5: 11 years
W1-W5: .64 (98%)
W3-W5: .74 (95%)
W4-W5: .77 (91%)
73% 2% 11% 14%
W6: 12 years W1-W6: .61 (99%)
W3-W6: .69 (98%)
W4-W6: .70 (96%)
W5-W6: .77 (91%)
69% 3% 10% 18%

Teacher report:

W1-W2: .48 (79%) 40% 3% 21% 36%
W1-W3: .45 (92%)
W2-W3: .58 (76%)
45% 8% 12% 35%
W1-W4: .39 (98%)
W2-W4: .45 (91%)
W3-W4: .57 (78%)
41% 5% 1% 53%
W1-W5: .42 (100%)
W2-W5: .44 (100%)
W3-W5: .49 (99%)
W4-W5: .46 (98%)
48% 2% 3% 47%
W1-W6: .33 (100%)
W2-W6: .35 (100%)
W3-W6: .37 (100%)
W4-W6: .35 (100%)
W5-W6: .45 (85%)
30% 2% 12% 56%

Harden, Quinn, & Tucker-Drob (2012) National Longitudinal Study of Youth: Children and Young Adults (CNLSY): 2562 sibling pairs Delinquency (CSAS-SRD/YA-SRD; self) W1: 10-11 years Latent growth curve model Range: .32 -.39 81% (intercept) 19% / 0% (intercept) 30% (slope) 0%/ 70% (slope)
W2: 12-13 years
W3: 14-15 years
W4: 16-17

Huizink, van den Berg, van der Ende, & Verhulst (2007)g Dutch sample of adopted siblings:106 biologically-related (BR) and 230 unrelated (BUR) pairs Externalising behaviour (CBCL; adoptive parent) W1: BR M= 12.5 years (SD= 1.2); BUR M= 12.4 years (SD= 1.2) Common pathway model (developmental) W1-W2: .64 (92%) 92% 1% / 0% 0% 0% / 7%
W2: BR M= 15.8 years (SD= 1.2); BUR M= 15.6 years (SD= 1.2)
[W3:26 years]

Jacobson, Prescott, & Kendler (2002) Virginia Twin Registry (VTR): 2580 twin pairs Antisocial behaviour (11-item questionnaire [DSM-III-R conduct disorder symptoms]; self) W1: <15 years Cholesky decomposition Males:
W2: 15-17 years W1-W2: .56 (18%) 17% 6% / 16% 25% 0% / 36%

[W3: >18 years] Females:
W1-W2: .51 (39%) 14% 7% / 7% 36% 0% / 35%

Jaffee, Hanscombe, Haworth, Davis, & Plomin (2012) Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): 6286 twin pairs Disruptive behaviour (hyperactivity and conduct sub-scales of SDQ; parent) W1: 9 years Cholesky decomposition (genetic cross-lag with household chaos) Conduct:
W2: 12 years W1-W2: .56 (68%) 29% 0% / 2% 21% (controlling for household chaos) 18% / 19% (controlling for household chaos)

Hyperactivity:
W1-W2: .66 (63%) 34% 2% / 7% 36% (controlling for household chaos) 0% / 17% (controlling for household chaos)

Kan et al. (2013) Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 2375 - 16103 twin pairs Attention problems (Dutch OAS at W1 / CBCL W2-4 / YSR W5-7; parent / parent / self) W1: 3 years Simplex (ADE) model W1-W2: .41 Proportion of genetic influences present at previous wave (A / D): Proportion of environmental influences present at previous wave (E): Proportion of genetic influences not present at previous wave (A / D): Proportion of environmental influences not present at previous wave (E):
W2: 7 years W1-W3: .29
W2-W3: .71
W3: 10 years W1-W4: .18
W2-W4: .44
W3-W4: .62
W4: 12 years W2: 100% / 25% 4% 0% / 75% 96%
W5: 13-15 years (self-report) *W1-W5: .05
*W2-W5: .11
*W3-W5: .15
*W4-W5: .25
W3: 100% / 77% 59% 0% / 23% 41%
W6: 15-17 years (self-report) W4: 34% / 92% 50% 66% / 8% 50%
W7: 17-19 years (self-report) *W1-W6: .03
*W2-W6: .06
*W3-W6: .09
*W4-W6: .14
W5-W6: .58
W5: 37% / 69% 52% 63% / 31% 48%
[Subsequent waves 19-60 years] W6: 100% / 53% 53% 0% / 47% 47%
W7: 49% / 100% 60% 51% / 100% 40%
*W1-W7: .01
*W2-W7: .03
*W3-W7: .05
*W4-W7: .07
W5-W7: .29 W6-W7: .51
*denotes cross-rater correlations

Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson, & Plomin (2005) Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): 4719 twin pairs ADHD symptoms (RRPSPC at W1 & (with additional items) W2 / SDQ hyperactivity-inattention sub-scale at W3 & W4 / CPRS at W5; parent) W1: 2 years Cholesky decomposition (AE only, with contrast effects) W1-W2: .47 (94%) 25% 0% 54% 21%
W2: 3 years W1-W3: .39 (92%)
W2-W3: .58 (83%)
17%
14%
0%
4%
45% 19%
W3: 4 years W1-W4: .46 (59%)
W2-W4: .35 (91%)
W3-W4: .45 (87%)
10%
5%
7%
0%
0%
1%
57% 19%
W4: 7 years
W5: 8 years W1-W5: .27 (96%)
W2-W5: .40 (93%)
W3-W5: .46 (89%)
W4-W5: .49 (86%)
9%
9%
7%
6%
0%
0%
1%
2%
55% 11%

Lacourse et al. (2014) Quebec Newborn Twin Study: 667 twin pairs Physical aggression (3-item scale; mother) W1: M = 1.6 years (1.5-2) Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .32 (82%) 12% (1%) / (0%) 51% (3%) / 31%
W2: M = 2.7 years (2.5-3.1) W1-W3: .25 (71%)
W2-W3: .39 (58%)
5%
11%
(3%) / (0%)
(2%) / (0%)
34% 17% / 28%
W3: M = 4.2 years (3.9-4.6)

Latent growth curve model W1-W2: .32 (100%) 25%
(intercept)
0% / 0% 9% / 33%
(slope / resid.)
0% / 49%
W1-W3: .25 (100%)
W2-W3: .39 (100%)
26%
(intercept)
0% / 0% 24% / 6%
(slope / resid.)
20% / 24%

Larsson, Larsson, & Lichtenstein (2004) Young Twins Study (YTS - Sweden): 1063 - 1106 twin pairs ADHD symptoms (binary scaled 14-item checklist based on DSM-III-R; parent) W1: 8-9 years Cholesky decomposition Males:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .51 (74%) 36% (2%) / 1% 38% (0%) / 24%

Females:
W1-W2: .51 (79%) 21% (4%) / 3% 40% (0%) / 32%

Larsson, Lichtenstein, & Larsson (2006) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1480 twin pairs ADHD symptoms: inattention / hyperactivity-impulsivity (binary scaled 21-24 item checklist based on DSM-III-R; parent) W1: 8-9 years Independent pathway model Inattention:
W2: 13-14 years Males:
W3: 16-17 years Range (across sex):
.34-.60        W2: 51% Information unavailable 18% Information unavailable
W3: 45% 27%

Females:
W2: 56% Information unavailable 6% Information unavailable
W3: 41% 21%

Hyperactivity-impulsivity:
Males:
W2: 54% Information unavailable 18% Information unavailable
W3: 44% 22%

Females:
W2: 41% Information unavailable 14% Information unavailable
W3: 33% 31%

Lewis & Plomin (2015)g Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): >3000 twin pairs Conduct problems / hyperactivity (SDQ; parent) W1: 4 years Cholesky decomposition Conduct problems:
W2: 7 years W1-W2: .47 (77%) 22% 10% / 1% 40% 3% / 23%
W3: 9 years W1-W3: .43 (72%)
W2-W3: .55 (67%)
16%
8%
17% / 0%
(1%) / 3%
28% 8% / 18%
W4: 12 years W1-W4: .40 (71%)
W2-W4: .52 (73%)
W3-W4: .56 (70%)
14%
11%
8%
14% / 0%
(3%) / 1%
(4%) / 2%
23% (0%) / 19%
W5: 16 years
W1-W5: .28 (96%)
W2-W5: .37 (85%)
W3-W5: .39 (74%)
W4-W5: .48 (80%)
12%
6%
2%
8%
3% / (0%)
(0%) / 0%
(1%) / 1%
(0%) / 1%
41% (0%) / 25%

Cholesky decomposition (AE model) Hyperactivity:
W1-W2: .54 (52%) 22% 11% 27% 40%
W1-W3: .51 (64%)
W2-W3: .65 (65%)
30%
11%
5%  8% 28% 19%
W1-W4: .43 (73%)
W2-W4: .57 (71%)
W3-W4: .64 (79%)
27%
10%
5%
2%  5%
3%
31% 17%
W1-W5: .31 (89%)
W2-W5: .41 (84%)
W3-W5: .45 (89%)
W4-W5: .55 (88%)
21%
6%
2%
6%
0%  2%
1%
1%
38% 22%

Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, & Baker (2013) University of Southern California Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior Twin Study: 1204 individuals (750 participating families in the extended study) Anti-social behaviour (common factor from: aggression & rule-breaking CBCL sub-scales; parent) W1: 9-10 years Common pathway model (phenotypic) Anti-social behaviour (latent factors)h:
W2: 14-15 years W1-W2: .77 (41%) 23% 24% / 11% 42% (0%) / (0%)

Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, Wang, & Baker (2012) University of Southern California Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior Twin Study: 1204 individuals (750 participating families in the extended study) Impulsiveness (common factor from inattention, motor & non-planning BIS sub-scales; self) W1: M = 11.89 years (SD = .69) Common pathway model (phenotypic; AE model) Impulsiveness (latent factors)h:
W2: M = 14.69 (SD = .63) W1-W2: .43 (76%) 18% 3% 32% 48%

O'Connor, Neiderhiser, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin (1998)g
- see also Neiderhiser, Reiss, & Hetherington (1996)
Nonshared Environment and Adolescent Development (NEAD) Project: 405 individuals including twins and full, half and unrelated sibling pairs Antisocial behaviour (composite from BPI [3 raters] and observational report; father / mother / self & observer) W1: 10-18 years Independent pathway model W1-W2: .63 (52%) 32% 18% / 11% 18% (0%) / 20%
W2: 12-21 years

Polderman et al. (2011) Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR):445 twin pairs Attention problems (CBCL; mother) W1: 5 years Cholesky decomposition (correlated factors) W1-W2: .39 (91%) 22% 0% / 1% 40% 3% / 34%
W2: 7 years

W1: 2 years W1-W2: .55 (94%) 68% 6% 9% 17%
Price et al. (2005) Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): 6840 twin pairs ADHD symptoms (RRPSPC at W1 & [with additional items] W2 / SDQ hyperactivity-inattention sub-scales at W3; parent) W2: 3 years Common pathway model (developmental; AE model with contrast effects) W1-W3: .46 (96%)
W2-W3: .60 (98%)
50% 5% 27% 19%
W3: 4 years

Rietveld, Hudziak, Bartels, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma (2004) Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): > 1096 twin pairs Attention problems (Dutch OAS at W1 / CBCL at other waves; parent) W1: 3 years Cholesky decomposition (correlated factors; ADE model) Males:
W2: 7 years
(% = A+D) (A / D) (A / D)
W3: 10 years W1-W2: .41 (90%) 11% / 7% 1% 22% / 32% 27%
W4: 12 years W1-W3: .38 (92%)
W2-W3: .69 (82%)
19% / 0%
13% / 27%
0%
5%
9% / 3% 22%
W1-W4: .35 (87%)
W2-W4: .67 (79%)
W3-W4: .75 (76%)
10% / 1%
5% / 28%
7% / 1%
1%
6%
8%
8% / 0% 15%

Females:
W1-W2: .41 (90%) 21% / 1% 1% 36% / 15% 26%
W1-W3: .37 (92%)
W2-W3: .68 (81%)
21% / 1%
18% / 25%
0%
6%
9% / 0% 21%
W1-W4: .38 (89%)
W2-W4: .65 (81%)
W3-W4: .72 (79%)
18% / 1%
12% / 17%
9% / 0%
1%           5%
6%
15% / 0% 17%

Schmitz, Fulker, & Mrazek (1995)g Colorado twin sample: 203 - 260 twin pairs Externalising (CBCL; parents) W1: 2.8 years Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .40 (82%) (45%) (3%) / (2%) (12%) (19%) / (19%)
W2: 7.6 years

Taylor et al. (2013) Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS): 5356 - 5983 twin pairs ADHD traits (CPRS; parent) W1: 8 years Cholesky decomposition (correlated factors) Males:
W2: 12 years
W1-W2: .67 (81%) 42% 3% / 2% 32% 9% / 12%

Females:
W1-W2: .65 (78%) 38% 4% / 3% 29% 13% / 13%

Tuvblad, Eley, & Lichtenstein (2005) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1226 twin pairs Delinquency (sub-scale of CBCL / 34-item delinquency questionnaire; parent / self) W1: M = 8.7 years (SD = 0.47) Cholesky decomposition Males:
W2: M = 13.7 years (SD = 0.47)
W1-W2: .16 (40%) 1% 4% / (0%) 28% 38% / 30%

Females:
W1-W2: .19 (100%) 10% (0%) / (0%) (21%) 36% / 32%

Tuvblad, Narusyte, Grann, Sarnecki, & Lichtenstein (2011) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 2600 individual twins Antisocial behaviour (externalising sub-scale of CBCL [W1] / 31-34 item delinquency questionnaire; parent / self) W1: 8-9 years Common pathway model (developmental) Males:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .11 (53%) 30% 12% / 3% (3%) 22% / 30%
W3: 16-17 years
W1-W3: .09 (77%)
W2-W3: .55 (65%)
42% 16% / 4% (0%) (4%) / 34%

[W4: 19-20 years] Females:
W1-W2: .14 (42%) 32% 12% / 3% 17% 12% / 24%
W1-W3: .12 (69%)
W2-W3: .52 (68%)
46% 17% / 5% (5%) (4%) / 50%

Tuvblad, Raine, Zheng, & Baker (2009) University of Southern California Twin Study of Risk Factors for Anti-social Behaviour (RFAB): 615 twin pairs Reactive and proactive aggression (RPAQ; parent) W1: M = 9.6 years (SD = 0.58)
W2: M = 11.8 years (SD = 0.90)
Cholesky decomposition Reactive agression:
W1-W2: .54 (48%) 23% (1%) / 10% (24%) (9%) / 31%

Proactive aggression:
W1-W2: .50 (85%) 53% (0%) / 1% (0%) (3%) / 41%

van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Hudziak, & Boomsma (2003) Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 1509 - 6488 twin pairs Aggressive behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 3 years Simplex model Males:
W2: 7 years W1-W2: .48 (65%) 18% 10% / 0% 48% 8% / 16%
W3: 10 years W1-W3: .43 (64%)
W2-W3: .73 (78%)
49% 9% / 3% 18% 7% / 14%
W4: 12 years W1-W4: .42 (58%)
W2-W4: .67 (76%)
W3-W4: .77 (78%)
53% 12% / 4% 14% 8% / 10%

Females:
W1-W2: .48 (60%) 14% 14% / 0% 50% 6% / 16%
W1-W3: .43 (50%)
W2-W3: .73 (67%)
34% 19% / 3% 10% 7% / 18%
W1-W4: .42 (52%)
W2-W4: .67 (68%)
W3-W4: .77 (66%)
46% 17% / 3% 12% 8% / 14%

van den Oord & Rowe (1997)g National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY): 436 full sibling pairs; 119 half sibling pairs; 122 cousin pairs Antisocial behaviour / headstrong behaviour / hyperactivity (BPI; mother W1: 4-6 years Genetic liability model (independent pathway model) Antisocial behaviour:
W2: 6-8 years W1-W2: .46 (76%) 28% 13% / 0% 0% 49%
W3: 8-10 years W1-W3: .44 (73%)
W2-W3: .50 (70%)
32% 17% 0% 51%

Headstrong behaviour:
W1-W2: .48 (67%) 32% 16% / 0% 0% 52%
W1-W3: .48 (67%)
W2-W3: .48 (67%)
32% 16% / 0% 0% 52%

Hyperactivity
W1-W2: .48 (81%) 39% 9% / 0% 0% 52%
W1-W3: .48 (81%)
W2-W3: .48 (81%)
39% 9% / 0% 0% 52%

van der Valk, van den Oord, Verhulst, & Boomsma (2003)g Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR): 1924 - 3873 Externalising behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 3 years Indpendent pathway model Males:
W2: 7 years W1-W2: .55 (53%) 33% 23% / 5% 20% 7% / 12%

Females:
W1-W2: .53 (55%) 36% 25% / 5% 14% 9% / 11%

van der Valk, Verhulst, Neale, & Boomsma (1998)g Dutch Adoption Sample: 222 biologically related adolescents adopted together (BR); 422 unrelated adolescents adopted together (BUR); 1484 adolescents adopted singly (AS) Externalising behaviour (CBCL; parent) W1: 10-15 years
W2: 14-17 years
Cholesky decomposition W1-W2: .66 (57%) 26% 11% / 8% 22% 6% / 27%

Van Hulle et al. (2009) - see also Lahey et al. (2009) Offspring of National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY79) sample Antisocial behaviour: conduct problems / delinquency (BPI / SRD; mother / self) W1: 4-9 years (mother-reported conduct problems only) Cholesky decomposition Conduct problems:
Males:
W2: 10-11 years W1-W2: .59 (85%) 43% 5% / (ns) 2% 8% / 39%
W3: 12-13 years W1-W3: .52 (90%)
W2-W3: .56 (79%)
38%
4%
14% / 6%
4% / (ns)
(ns) 6% / 29%
W4: 14-17 years self-reported delinquency only)
Females:
W1-W2: .47 (87%) 25% 15% / (ns) 1% 16% / 41%
W1-W3: .46 (92%)
W2-W3: .44 (66%)
27%
8%
(ns) / 10%
(ns) / (ns)
(ns) 13% / 35%

Delinquency (controlling for conduct problems at W1):
W2-W3: .33 (45%) 29% 4% / (ns) (ns) 61%
W2-W4: .25 (54%)
W3-W4: .38 (68%)
23% (ns) / (ns) (ns) 58%
(ns) = non-significant estimate, value not reported

Vierikko, Pulkkinen, Kaprio, & Rose (2006) Finnish twin-family study (FinnTwin12): 1,041 twin pairs Aggression (sub-scale of MPNI; teacher) W1: 12 years Cholesky decomposition (including sex-specific C) Males:
W2: M = 14.3 years (14 - 15.6) W1-W2: .35 (38%) 7% 6% (sex-specific C)/ 0% 35% 0% / 52%

Females:
W1-W2: .26 (0%) 0% 31% / 1% 40% 0% / 28%

Wichers et al. (2013) Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development (TCHAD): 1480 twin pairs Externalising behaviour (CBCL / YSR; parent / self) W1: 8-9 years Rater bias model with Cholesky decomposition Rater-agreedh externalising behaviour:
W2: 13-14 years W1-W2: .73 (94%) 61% (0%) / (1%) 26% (4%) / 7%
W3: 16-17 years W1-W3: .71 (94%)
W2-W3: .81 (86%)
58%
(4%)
(0%) / (0%)
(4%) / 6%
26% (0%) / (0%)
[W4: 19-20 years]

Wang et al. (2012) Western Reserve Reading Project: 204 twin pairs Externalising behaviour + attention problems (CBCL; parent) W1: M = 6.07 years (4.33-7.92) Independent pathway model (combined externalising and attention problems) Externalising:
W2: M = 7.15 years (6.00-8.83) W1-W2: .71(82%) 44% (0%) / 12% 32% (0%) / 12%
W3: M = 8.30 years (6.50-10.00) W1-W3: .68 (83%)
W2-W3: .76 (86%)
41%
27%
1% / 9% (0%)
/ (0%)
(16%) (0%) / 7%

Attention problems:
W1-W2: .47 (7%) 4% 44% / (0%) (0%) (0%) / 53%
W1-W3: .35 (0%)
W2-W3:.44 (0%)
(0%)
(0%)
29% / 12%
(0%) / 7%
(20%) (0%) / (32%)

Notes –

a

Mean (M) age given with range and standard deviation (SD) if available; where no range is presented this is because the information was unavailable and is not necessarily indicative of participants being the same age

b

Models estimated additive genetic (A), common environmental (C) and unique environmental (E) components of variance unless otherwise stated;

c / d

Results for influence of stable genetic and environmental factors given as % phenotypic variance in later wave(s) explained by factors from earlier wave(s) where possible; values are aligned with phenotypic stability coefficients in adjacent column – i.e. for values aligned with W2-W3 phenotypic stability, % variance explained is at W3 by stable factors from W2; where single values align with a group of stability coefficients, % variance explained is by stable factors from all prior waves; environmental factors are presented as C / E – where C was not estimated at all in the presented model, only one value is given; non-significant values are given in parentheses where known;

e / f

Results for influence of new genetic and environmental factors given in same format as above; only one value is presented per parameter per wave, in alignment with the appropriate group of phenotypic correlations, i.e. values centrally aligned with W1-W3 and W2-W3 stability coefficients are new aetiological factors at W3;

g

Study includes measures of both internalising and externalising and is therefore included in both tables

h

All results (including correlations) pertain to latent factor rather than observed variables; rater-specific (in case of rater bias models) and scale-specific (in case of common pathway – phenotypic models) error not included

Measures: CBCL: Child Behaviour Checklist; TRF: Teacher Report Form; YSR: Youth Self Report form; SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; OAS: Overactive Syndrome questionnaire; RRPSPC: Revised Rutter Parent Scale for Pre-school Children; CPRS-R: Conners Parent Rating Scale (Revised); BPI: Behaviour Problem Index; RPAQ: Reactive and Proactive Aggression Questionnaire; MPNI: Multidimensional Peer Nomination Inventory; DBRS: Disruptive Behaviour Rating Scale; CSAS: Child Self-Administered Supplement; (YA-)SRD: (Young Adult) Self-Report of Delinquency: BIS: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

Emotional Problems

Twenty-eight (28) studies of emotional problems included in this review are presented in Table 1. As noted in the methods section above, 9 studies included longitudinal analyses of both emotional and behavioural problems. These studies are included in both results tables, and are indicated by a superscript (g). The results of all studies involving analyses of emotional problems are described in the text below, grouped by phenotype as follows: (1) anxiety, fear and obsessive-compulsive behaviour; (2) depression and depressive symptoms; and (3) temperament and broad measures of emotional problems.2

Anxiety, fear and obsessive-compulsive behaviour

Eleven studies focused specifically on aspects of anxiety, fear, obsessive-compulsive and other related behaviours. The aetiology of stability and change in measures of anxiety was investigated in six of these studies, collectively spanning an age range of 4 to 18 years (Garcia et al., 2013; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Trzaskowski, Zavos, Haworth, Plomin, & Eley, 2012; Waszczuk, Zavos, & Eley, 2013; Waszczuk et al., 2016; Zavos, Gregory, & Eley, 2012). Overall, the stability of anxiety and anxiety-related behaviours in childhood and adolescence tended to be accounted for by genetic factors, and change by environmental factors. In all six studies, wave-to-wave stability was moderate (rPh range: .33-.54) and predominantly genetically- influenced (>50% rPh due to genes; Garcia et al., 2013; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Trzaskowski et al., 2012; Zavos et al., 2012; Waszczuk, Zavos, & Eley, 2013; Waszczuk et al., 2016). New genetic factors were also found to account for change across both childhood (Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Trzaskowski et al., 2012) and adolescence (Waszczuk et al., 2016). However, in two (Garcia et al., 2013; Zavos et al., 2012) of the three studies incorporating data from late adolescence (17-18 years; Garcia et al., 2013; Waszczuk et al., 2016; Zavos et al., 2012) no significant genetic innovation was found, suggesting that the involvement of new genetic factors for anxiety-related behaviours may wane later in development. Environmental influences were mainly found to contribute to change in anxiety. While these influences were non-shared (E) in most cases, in one study (Trzaskowski et al., 2012), longitudinal analyses produced evidence of shared (C) environmental influences playing a small but significant role in driving change in anxiety-related behaviours across middle childhood. Three studies, collectively spanning an age range of 4-17 years, analysed obsessive- compulsive symptoms (OCS; Bolhuis et al., 2014; Krebs, Waszczuk, Zavos, Bolton, & Eley, 2014; van Grootheest et al., 2007). Results from this group of studies were mixed. Stability in OCS was primarily underpinned by genetic factors in two studies, both of which included adolescents (Bolhuis et al., 2014; Krebs et al., 2014). However, in one study environmental factors were of equal importance for stability during middle childhood (van Grootheest et al., 2007). Non-shared environmental factors were found to drive change in all studies, while new genetic factors were again implicated in middle childhood but not late adolescence (17 years; Bolhuis et al., 2014). The aetiology of stability and change in fear was investigated in two studies, which used data collected in middle childhood and middle adolescence (Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008; Trzaskowski et al., 2012). Genetic factors were found to be important for both developmental stability and change in fear both as an anxiety sub-type (Trzaskowski et al., 2012) and in terms of specific phobias (Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008). In the case of the former, moderately high phenotypic stability (rPh: .52) was found between the ages of 7 and 9, and this was predominantly explained (67%) by genetic factors. New genetic factors explained 34% of variance at age 9 (Trzaskowski et al., 2012). A similar pattern was found to extend into adolescence for situational and blood/injury fears but not animal fears, which displayed less overall (and genetic) stability (Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008). Age- specific environmental influences were also found to drive change in both studies. These were primarily non-shared, although new shared environmental influences were also significant and explained, on average, more than 15% of new variance in middle childhood and early adolescence (Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008; Trzaskowski et al., 2012).

Depression and depressive symptoms

Depression-related phenotypes were analysed in seven studies, with participants ranging from 5 to 18 years of age (Bolhuis et al., 2014; Lau & Eley, 2006; O'Connor, Neiderhiser, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1998; Scourfield et al., 2003; Silberg et al., 1999; Tully, Iacono, & McGue, 2010; Waszczuk et al., 2016). These studies showed a broadly consistent pattern of results for the aetiology of stability in depressive symptoms across adolescence. This stability was moderate in six studies (rPh range: .33-.59) and low in just one (.24; Tully et al., 2010). In five studies, genetic factors were predominant in driving stability in depression and depressive symptoms across a range of ages from middle childhood (Bolhuis et al., 2014; Lau & Eley, 2006; Silberg et al., 1999; Tully et al., 2010; Waszczuk et al., 2016). In contrast, substantial environmental contributions to stability were found in only two studies, both measuring depressive symptoms in early-to-middle adolescence. Shared environmental factors accounted for more than 70% of the continuity (rPh: .39) in one (Scourfield et al. 2003) and non-shared environmental factors accounted for 46% of the continuity (rPh: .59) in the other (O'Connor et al., 1998). Notably, both of these two-wave studies had wide within-wave age ranges, including children up to 8-9 years apart. Results from the overall group of studies of depression were also consistent in their estimation of the relative aetiological influences on change. New non-shared environmental factors were found to drive change in depressive symptoms, doing so predominantly in all studies, and exclusively in all but three. These three studies (Lau & Eley, 2006; Scourfield et al., 2003; Waszczuk et al., 2016) also found evidence of new genetic factors driving change. None of the studies found significant new shared environmental influences across development.

Temperament and broad measures of emotional problems

Temperament, in the form of shy, withdrawn or inhibited behaviour, was analysed in four studies, across a range of ages in childhood (1-12 years; Cherny, Fulker, Corley, Plomin, & DeFries, 1994; Hoekstra, Bartels, Hudziak, Van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2008; Plomin et al., 1993; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997). Genetic factors were again found to be important for stability and, to a lesser but still significant extent, change. Environmental factors were found to be substantially involved in the stability of withdrawn behaviour in only one of the studies (van den Oord & Rowe, 1997). Large contributions to change from non-shared environmental factors were observed in three studies (Cherny et al., 1994; Plomin et al., 1993; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997). In the remaining study (Hoekstra et al., 2008), environmental factors were found to play only a minimal aetiological role overall. However, it should be noted that this study involved the decomposition of a rater-agreed phenotype, which somewhat limits the extent to which the results from this study are directly comparable with the other studies in this subset. Nine studies included broader measures of emotional problems, such as mixed anxiety and depression symptoms (Bartels, van den Oord, et al., 2004; Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, & Hewitt, 2005; Huizink, van den Berg, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2007; Kendler, Gardner, & Lichtenstein, 2008; Nivard et al., 2015; Schmitz, Fulker, & Mrazek, 1995; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997; van der Valk, van den Oord, Verhulst, & Boomsma, 2003; van der Valk, Verhulst, Neale, & Boomsma, 1998). These studies collectively covered an age range of 3-17 years. In this group of studies, stability was again predominantly underpinned by genetic factors in the majority of cases. However, moderately influential stable shared environmental factors were also found in early childhood (Bartels, van den Oord, et al., 2004), middle childhood (van den Oord & Rowe, 1997) and early-/mid-adolescence (Huizink et al., 2007; van der Valk et al., 1998). Interestingly, in a study of teacher-reported internalising symptoms in children aged 7-12 years, no such environmental influences on stability were found. Instead this stability, which was found to be modest-to-moderate (rPh range: .14-.38), was entirely genetic (Haberstick et al., 2005). Across this group of studies of broadly assessed emotional problems, phenotypic change resulted from both new genetic and new (primarily non-shared) environmental factors. New genetic factors tended to be less influential later in adolescence, although results of Nivard et al. (2015) are a notable exception. In three studies, collectively spanning ages 4-17 years, new genetic factors were not at all influential (Huizink et al., 2007; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997; van der Valk et al., 1998). Findings of moderate-to-large non-shared environmental contributions to change were more consistent across studies and ages.

Summary

Broad measures were used in the majority of studies of emotional problems, with specific studies of anxiety, depression and other symptoms of childhood emotional problems relatively fewer in comparison. Overall, in these studies of specific emotional problems such as anxiety, obsessive-compulsive symptoms and depressive symptoms spanning childhood and adolescence from age 4-18 years, stability was largely underpinned by genetic influences. Results were particularly consistent for studies of depressive symptoms in adolescence. A similar pattern was evident in studies of broader emotional problems, which covered almost the entire eligible age range (1-17 years), with some additional evidence that shared environmental influences may also play a role in producing stability in measures of these phenotypes. In contrast, non-shared environmental influences were typically found to drive change across all emotional phenotypes and all ages. In some specific phenotypes, such as fear, new shared environmental factors were also influential. There was some evidence that the transition from childhood to adolescence may be characterised by the emergence of new genetic influences. Where present, genetic innovation tended to disappear by late adolescence, although this was not true in all cases.

Behavioural Problems

Thirty-nine (39) studies of behavioural problems are presented in Table 2. This table is structured identically to Table 1, and similarly includes all studies with a measure of behavioural as well as emotional problems (denoted by superscript g). The results of all studies of behavioural problems are summarised below in three groups: (1) ADHD, attention problems and impulsivity (2) aggression, anger, antisocial behaviour and conduct disorder; and (3) general behavioural problems.3

ADHD, attention problems and impulsivity

Sixteen studies reported analyses of a measure of either attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or related symptoms and behaviours. Nine of these studies included data from at least two ages between 2 and 8 years (Ebejer et al., 2010; Kan et al., 2013; Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson, & Plomin, 2005; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Polderman et al., 2011; Price et al., 2005; Rietveld, Hudziak, Bartels, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2004; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997; Wang, Deater-Deckard, Petrill, & Thompson, 2012). Overall, stability in measures of ADHD symptoms was found to be largely genetic in these childhood studies. Additive genetic factors explained more than 65% of the wave-to-wave stability of ADHD symptoms in childhood in six of these studies. However, in three studies where non-additive (dominant) genetic effects (D) were also estimated (Ebejer et al., 2010; Kan et al., 2013; Rietveld et al., 2004), these also contributed to stability. Indeed, in one study (Ebejer et al., 2010), of inattention, non-additive genetic factors were by far the greatest source of phenotypic stability during this period, although they were found to be more influential in driving change elsewhere (age 3-7; Kan et al., 2013; Rietveld et al., 2004). Notably, new additive genetic influences on ADHD symptoms across this period were also found and, in some cases, these too were greater than the stable genetic factors that preceded them (e.g., Kuntsi et al., 2005; Polderman et al., 2011). Environmental influences on change in childhood ADHD-related symptoms were predominantly age-specific and non-shared. Where environmental influences on stability were found, the effects were limited, with stable shared and non-shared factors explaining between 3-17% later variance in childhood ADHD-related symptoms (e.g., Ebejer et al., 2010; Kan et al., 2013; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Price et al., 2005; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997). The single exception to the overall trend in this group was a study by Wang et al. (2012), in which stability from shared environmental influences and change from non-shared environmental influences was found, with only small or non-significant genetic influences on attention regulation problems at ages 7 and 8 years. Eleven studies that reported analyses of ADHD-related symptoms used data from across middle childhood and adolescence (9-18 years; Bezdjian, Tuvblad, Wang, Raine, & Baker, 2014; Jaffee, Hanscombe, Haworth, Davis, & Plomin, 2012; Kan et al., 2013; Larsson, Lichtenstein, & Larsson, 2006; Larsson, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, 2004; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, Wang, & Baker, 2012; Rietveld et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 2013; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997). Wave-to-wave stability in attention problems across middle childhood was accounted for by a mixture of genetic and environmental stability (e.g., Kan et al., 2013; Rietveld et al., 2004). However, stability in broader ADHD phenotypes remained primarily genetically influenced both in this period and into adolescence. For example, across five studies (Jaffee et al., 2012; Larsson et al., 2006; Larsson et al., 2004; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Taylor et al., 2013), stable genetic factors from earlier in development explained more than one third of variance, on average, in ADHD-related phenotypes at ages between 12 and 16 years. In each case, genetic factors accounted for more stability than any environmental factors. This pattern was replicated at age 12 in a study of a laboratory-based measure of impulsivity, but, notably, reversed at later ages where non- shared environmental factors accounted for most (55% and 75% respectively) of the wave-to-wave stability at ages 15 and 17 (Bezdjian et al., 2014). Non-shared environmental factors also became increasingly stable across adolescence in one study (Kan et al., 2013). Although phenotypic stability in ADHD and related behaviours generally remained moderate to high into adolescence, change was evident and was predominantly accounted for by a combination of genetic and non-shared environmental factors. In studies where estimates of shared environmental factors were significant, their contribution to change was generally small or non-significant (e.g., Larsson et al., 2004; Lewis & Plomin, 2015). The results regarding the relative magnitudes of new genetic and non-shared environmental influences emerging for ADHD symptoms across adolescence were mixed. In some studies (Bezdjian et al., 2014; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997) few or no new genetic influences were found, while in others (Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson, & Larsson, 2013; Jaffee et al., 2012; Lewis & Plomin, 2015) new genetic influences were of greater magnitude than contemporary non- shared environmental influences.

Aggression, anger, antisocial behaviour and conduct problems

Twenty studies involved phenotypes relating to aggression, anger, antisocial behaviour (ASB), or conduct problems. Of these, seven involved longitudinal data on aggression or anger, incorporating data from children between the ages of 1 and 14 years (Eley, Lichtenstein, & Moffitt, 2003; Gagne & Hill Goldsmith, 2011; Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, & Hewitt, 2006; Lacourse et al., 2014; Tuvblad, Raine, Zheng, & Baker, 2009; van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Hudziak, & Boomsma, 2003; Vierikko, Pulkkinen, Kaprio, & Rose, 2006). In early childhood, anger and aggression were found to be only moderately stable (rPh range: .03-.48 up to age 7; Gagne & Hill Goldsmith, 2011; Lacourse et al., 2014; van Beijsterveldt et al., 2003). Stability was again predominantly genetic in this period, although stable shared environmental factors of roughly equivalent magnitude were found in one study (van Beijsterveldt et al., 2003). New genetic, shared and non-shared environmental factors influenced change in infant anger (Gagne & Hill Goldsmith, 2011) and aggression (Lacourse et al., 2014; van Beijsterveldt et al., 2003) in this period. In the remaining four studies of aggression (Eley et al., 2003; Haberstick et al., 2006; Tuvblad et al., 2009; Vierikko et al., 2006), from age 8 onwards, stability was generally higher, with genetic factors again accounting for the majority of this continuity. For example, in middle childhood, Haberstick et al. (2006) found that more than 75% of the stability in parent (rPh range: .61-.78) and teacher (rPh range: .33-.58) reports of aggressive behaviour between the ages of 8 and 12 years was explained by stable genetic factors. Two studies (Eley et al., 2003; Vierikko et al., 2006) included data from mid-adolescence, but these studies had very different findings. In the first, stable genetic factors accounted for a substantial amount of the moderate-to-high stability of aggressive behaviour (84% of rPh: .61) but little of the phenotypic change, which was attributable to both shared and non-shared environmental factors (Eley et al., 2003). In the other, aggression showed only low stability, influenced by stable environmental factors, with change resulting from new genetic factors (Vierikko et al., 2006). Methodological differences between these studies may offer a partial explanation for their discrepant findings. Specifically, the use of parent- and teacher-report respectively raises the possibility that these measures used in these studies may tap different, situation-specific behaviours. Nine studies, with a collective age range of 4-17 years, included analyses of aetiological stability and change in measures of generalised or nonaggressive antisocial behaviour (ASB; Eley et al., 2003; Harden, Quinn, & Tucker-Drob, 2012; Neiderhiser, Reiss, & Hetherington, 1996; Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, & Baker, 2013; O'Connor et al., 1998; Tuvblad, Eley, & Lichtenstein, 2005; van den Oord & Rowe, 1997; Van Hulle et al., 2009). One of these studies (Neiderhiser et al., 1996) presents analyses that are also included, in greater detail, in another study (O'Connor et al., 1998). Accordingly, only the latter is presented in the table and included in this overview. The results of the studies of antisocial behaviour suggest a pattern of moderate stability underpinned by both genetic and shared environmental influences (e.g. Niv et al., 2013; O'Connor et al., 1998; Tuvblad, Narusyte, Grann, Sarnecki, & Lichtenstein, 2011). To the extent that phenotypic change occurred across development, environmental factors - both shared and non-shared - were found to play a driving role. New genetic influences were generally of a smaller magnitude, but were present in mid-adolescence in particular. For example, in a study by Tuvblad, Eley, & Lichtenstein (2005), variance in boys' non-aggressive antisocial behavior at age 14 was primarily explained by new aetiological factors, with new genetic factors explaining 28% of the variance and new shared and non-shared environmental factors explaining 38% and 30% respectively. One notable exception was in Niv et al. (2013), where new genetic factors were more influential at age 14-15 than new non-shared environmental factors. Five studies used measures of conduct problems, spanning an age range of 4-17 years (Jacobson, Prescott, & Kendler, 2002; Jaffee et al., 2012; Lahey et al., 2009; Lewis & Plomin, 2015; Van Hulle et al., 2009). As in the group of studies of antisocial behaviour, significant duplication between two of these studies (Lahey et al., 2009; Van Hulle et al., 2009) mean that only the one with the greatest detail is presented in the table (Van Hulle et al., 2009). Overall, stability in conduct problems in childhood and adolescence was typically moderate-to-substantial, with wave-to-wave phenotypic correlations exceeding .45 in three studies (Jacobson et al., 2002; Jaffee et al., 2012; Lewis & Plomin, 2015). This stability was primarily genetically mediated, although stable shared and non-shared environmental factors were sporadically implicated across all studies. In terms of change, one study (Van Hulle et al., 2009) found that change in conduct problems in middle childhood and adolescence was entirely explained by a combination of shared and (primarily) non-shared environmental factors, with no new genetic influences coming online. However, the remaining studies all found additional genetic (as well as environmental) contributions to change, with new genetic factors explaining at least one fifth of variance at each wave, in all cases (Jacobson et al., 2002; Jaffee et al., 2012; Lewis & Plomin, 2015).

Behavioural problems (general)

Eight studies included general measures of behavioural problems, typically described as 'externalising behaviour', collectively spanning the ages of 2-17 years (Bartels, Boomsma, et al., 2004; Haberstick et al., 2005; Huizink et al., 2007; Schmitz et al., 1995; van der Valk et al., 2003, 1998; Wang et al., 2012; Wichers et al., 2013). In the studies that investigated the childhood aetiology of general behavioural problems, a pattern of mixed genetic and environmental stability was found. Evidence that stability in externalising behaviour between the ages of 3 and 10 years was the result of substantial stable shared environmental and genetic factors was found in three separate studies (Bartels, van den Oord, et al., 2004; Haberstick et al., 2005; van der Valk et al., 2003). Somewhat contrasting evidence was found in the one remaining study with data from this developmental period, in which environmental contributions to stability were non-shared, and relatively minor (Wang et al., 2012). Genetic contributions to stability were estimated to be substantial (explaining >30% variance) in all four studies, while the aetiology of phenotypic change was predominantly genetic and non-shared environmental in all cases. The results for general behavioural problems across adolescence were similarly consistent with respect to the origins of phenotypic stability. Genetic factors were again predominant, accounting for, on average, 79% of the phenotypic stability across three separate studies incorporating data from individuals aged 10-17 years (Huizink et al., 2007; van der Valk et al., 1998; Wichers et al., 2013). Genetic innovation was found to be important for driving phenotypic change in two of these three studies (van der Valk et al., 1998; Wichers et al., 2013), and also earlier, at age 12, in two further studies (Bartels, van den Oord, et al., 2004; Haberstick et al., 2005). Non-shared environmental contributions to change were found to be significant in all but one study (Schmitz et al., 1995), and were generally small-to-moderate in magnitude.

Summary

Studies of behavioural problems were typically designed to investigate either attention/hyperactivity related problems or aggressive/antisocial behaviour and conduct problems. Generalised measures of behavioural problems, such as externalising, were used in comparatively fewer studies. Overall, a pattern of predominant genetic influence on stability across development, for all behavioural problem phenotypes, was observed. The magnitude of this stability varied between phenotypes and across different ages. Additionally, environmental influences on stability were observed relatively consistently for some phenotypes (e.g., antisocial behaviour) and not for others (e.g. attention problems). Change was underpinned, variously, by genetic, shared and non-shared environmental factors, with limited consistency across different behavioural problem phenotypes. For the broadest measures of behavioural problems, including non-aggressive antisocial behaviour, new variance across childhood and adolescence was predominantly accounted for by genetic and non-shared environmental factors.

Discussion

Summary of findings and relation to the broader literature

We identified 58 longitudinal behavioural genetic studies of emotional and behavioural problems in this systematic review. Overall, the findings from the studies included were very consistent. The majority found stability of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence to be primarily genetically mediated. This is in keeping with the aetiological and developmental picture established by previous work in the field, notably the significant genetic influence found on emotional and behavioural phenotypes from cross-sectional genetic studies at all ages (Lau & Eley, 2010; Middeldorp, Cath, Van Dyck, & Boomsma, 2005; Rhee & Waldman, 2002; Rice et al., 2002; Rutter et al., 1999); and the moderate stability of emotional and behavioural problems across childhood and adolescence (Costello et al., 2003; Hofstra et al., 2000; Ollendick et al., 1994).

Where stability due to environmental influences was found, it was generally of smaller magnitude (e.g. generalised internalising: Bartels, van den Oord, et al., 2004; Huizink et al.,2007; Kendler, Gardner, & Lichtenstein, 2008; antisocial behaviour: Eley et al., 2003; Harden et al., 2012; Jacobson et al., 2002; Niv et al., 2013). In the case of the shared environment, these results should be considered with respect to power demands, which are particularly high for estimating 'C' as has been discussed in detail elsewhere (Burt, 2009, 2014). Stability from both shared and non-shared environmental factors was seen, across results from studies of both emotional and behavioural problems.

This affirms that environmental factors can operate to produce phenotypic stability irrespective of whether they make individuals in the same family more or less similar. Previous research has identified some specific, stable sources of environmental influence on children's behaviour (e.g., SES, neighbourhood characteristics, parenting characteristics; Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Coll, 2001; Dumas et al., 2005; see Burt, 2014 for a review). Systematic rater effects could also play a role in producing phenotypic stability in some cases, although evidence from studies using multiple raters confirms that this does not account for all environmental stability.

Most studies found substantial phenotypic change, as well as stability, across development. In many cases, this was partially underpinned by genetic innovation. Genetically influenced phenotypic change can be the result of age-related biological changes, whether relating to brain development or hormonal variations (Blakemore, 2008; Kadosh, Linden, & Lau, 2013; Paus et al., 2008). In addition, the shifting importance of various social and environmental contexts for developing children and adolescents provides opportunities for new genetic influence to be mediated through gene-environment interplay - processes by which individuals' exposure and/or sensitivity to environmental factors is influenced genetically (Knafo & Jaffee, 2013; Rutter & Silberg, 2002; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Aspects of this interplay have also been hypothesised (Scarr & McCartney, 1983), and shown (e.g., Brody et al., 2009; Elkins, McGue, & Iacono, 1997; Hannigan, McAdams, Plomin & Eley, 2016), to change across development. The idea that these processes may be especially important for phenotypic change in childhood and adolescence is supported by the relative lack of genetic innovation found in similar studies of adults (Gillespie et al., 2004; Kan et al., 2013; Nes, Røysamb, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Harris, & Tambs, 2007; Nivard et al., 2015; Takahashi et al., 2007; Van Den Berg, Willemsen, De Geus, & Boomsma, 2006). However, it is noteworthy that significant new genetic influences are found in young adulthood for some phenotypes (e.g. antisocial behaviour and alcohol abuse: Malone, Taylor, Marmorstein, McGue, & Iacono, 2004; obsessive-compulsive symptoms: van Grootheest, Cath, Hottenga, Beekman, & Boomsma, 2009), suggesting some persistence of these processes beyond what is generally regarded as the end of the adolescent developmental period.

Phenotypic change was driven by non-shared environmental factors in the majority of studies. The non-shared environmental parameter in behavioural genetic studies includes time-specific measurement error, which could play a role in increasing the appearance of phenotypic discontinuity. However, non-shared environmental innovation was found to be substantial across studies using a range of measurement techniques in several different samples. Furthermore, the influence of 'true' non-shared environmental factors in driving phenotypic change is consistent with theoretical accounts of development, which emphasise the role of both stochastic and specific (e.g. illness, accidents, clinical interventions, different school experiences) influences in making children in the same family different from one another (Asbury, Dunn, Pike, & Plomin, 2003; Asbury, Dunn, & Plomin, 2006; Plomin, 2011; Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000).

Implications for researchers and clinicians

The predominance of genetic influence on stability in specific childhood and adolescent emotional and behavioural problems, as outlined in this review, has implications for genetic and developmental researchers alike. It is likely that the majority of genes affecting psychopathology in development have a broad effect across multiple traits and different life stages. As a result, extracting factors that reflect common variance across different traits and times may be beneficial in aiding the identification of these “general” genes (Eley, 1997). Shared genetic liability has long been viewed as at least a partial explanation for the widespread comorbidity in childhood psychopathology, including between emotional and behavioural problems (Caspi et al., 2013; Nadder, Rutter, Silberg, Maes, & Eaves, 2002; Silberg, Rutter, & Eaves, 2001). Substantial evidence for pleiotropy - whereby genes affect multiple different traits - has also emerged from molecular genetic studies (Hettema, Chen, Sun, & Brown, 2015; Trzaskowski et al., 2013). To date only a handful of studies have examined the extent to which this shared genetic influence operates developmentally to produce heterotypic continuity in emotional and behavioral problems (e.g. Roberson-Nay, Eaves, Hettema, Kendler, & Silberg, 2012; Silberg, Rutter, & Eaves, 2001; Waszczuk et al., 2016; Wertz et al., 2015). Future work exploring the overlap, and potential distinction, between genetic influences on homotypic and heterotypic continuity in these phenotypes is warranted. Identifying sources of common genetic liability to childhood and adolescent psychopathology and subsequent problems in adulthood also remains a priority. In the context of developmental psychopathology more broadly, researchers should aim to incorporate an awareness of the underlying genetic stability of many aspects of childhood and adolescent psychopathology into study design and the interpretation of results wherever possible.

While genetic innovation was found to be less prevalent than stability, it is of comparable developmental significance, providing an important source of change in symptomatology (Kendler, Gardner, & Lichtenstein, 2008; Kendler, Gardner, Annas, et al., 2008). Similarly, attenuation of genetic factors from early in development is also commonly seen alongside genetic innovation, in spite of the overall trend for heritability to increase with age (Bergen et al., 2007). This combination of innovation and attenuation suggests that, despite early temperamental and behavioural factors predicting later life problems, their respective genetic aetiologies may still differ to a substantial degree. This has wide-ranging implications for research. For example, for molecular genetic approaches, which aim to identify specific regions of the genome associated with mental health problems, this may indicate that some genes are important only at one developmental stage. In order to identify such genes, approaches involving longitudinal data or the stratification of cross-sectional samples by age will be necessary (Traylor, Markus, & Lewis, 2014). Furthermore, the mechanisms through which genetic factors 'coming online' throughout development may contribute to the emergence of clinical psychopathology in late adolescence and young adulthood are not well understood, and could prove to be fruitful areas for further work.

The evidence presented in this review also has implications for clinicians. Most importantly, from a clinical perspective, the finding of genetic stability underpinning the development of emotional and behavioural problems should not be viewed deterministically. Genetic influence on individual differences in measures of psychopathology is indicative of the different levels of risk associated with inherited genetic factors. Understanding the extent to which these risk factors are the same across development is an important step in uncovering the mechanisms that underpin the emergence of psychiatric illnesses. However, genetic influence, stable or otherwise, does not preclude the possibility of effective treatment. This is most clearly exemplified by entirely genetic conditions, such as phenylketonuria (PKU), for which the routine and successful treatment is entirely environmental (limitations to diet). This example also demonstrates the benefit of being able to identify individuals who are at an increased genetic risk for a particular disorder. Although this is not yet a possibility for the kind of psychiatric problems associated with childhood and adolescent psychopathology, evidence of substantial genetic stability across development, as reviewed here, indicates that such an approach could facilitate early intervention in the future. Furthermore, recent work has begun to explore the possibility of using genetic information to predict the kind of treatment to which individuals may respond best (Eley et al., 2012; Lester & Eley, 2013). The potential value of this approach is again underlined by predominant genetic stability in developmental psychopathology. Environmental stability, where evident, should also be considered as important from a clinical perspective. To the extent that these stable environmental factors can be identified and are modifiable, interventions targeting them can be expected to have a lasting effect across development.

Understanding the aetiology of developmental change in psychopathology, in adolescence in particular, may provide clinically-relevant insights as to how risk for psychiatric disorders in young adulthood and beyond is mediated (Gregory et al., 2007; Moffitt & Caspi, 2001; Rutter et al., 2006). Genetic innovation for behavioural and emotional problems suggests that there are periods during development where individual differences in psychopathology are the result of genetically influenced behavioural or physiological changes. The evidence from studies in this review indicates that these potentially 'sensitive' periods occur throughout development, and certainly not limited to adolescence. This may indicate that the window for effective clinical intervention in childhood and adolescence may be equally wide. Understanding the nature of the underlying behavioural changes that result in genetic innovation in longitudinal genetic studies may also allow us to highlight specific behaviours as targets for interventions. The widespread evidence of environmental influences on change in measures of child and adolescent psychopathology could also have potential clinical significance. Firstly, this is because specific environmental factors may again be identifiable and modifiable. Secondly, because evidence of variability in these measures in childhood and adolescence reinforces the notion that, even in the context of genetic predisposition for psychiatric disease, external (i.e. environmental) factors can account for a substantial amount of variance in these phenotypes. The effects of a clinical intervention would likely appear in a similar way, if modelled in a genetically informative design. However, the largely time- specific nature of environmental influence on developmental psychopathology suggests that, for such interventions to be successful in the long-term, they may need to be actively maintained.

Strengths and limitations of the review

This review has several strengths, most notably that it is (to our knowledge) the first review of longitudinal behavioural genetic results for both emotional and behavioural problems. It thus provides a comprehensive overview of many years of research into the developmental aetiology of child and adolescent psychopathology. The coverage of the age span specified (0-18 years) is also unusually broad (see Figure 2). We believe that this makes the review a particularly useful resource for both genetic and developmental research and clinical practice.

The main limitation of this review concerns the different approaches taken to analysis in the studies, combined with the wide variety of measures used, which precluded meta-analysis. Our decision to incorporate a wide range of phenotypes from the field of developmental psychopathology made this limitation somewhat unavoidable, but we believe that it is largely counter-balanced by the benefits of presenting and reviewing these studies together. In particular, the broadly consistent nature of findings, in spite of considerable methodological heterogeneity (even within phenotypes), is evident and quite striking - even without the benefits of a meta-analysis. Other issues include methodological limitations of the individual studies, such as the heavy reliance in this field on questionnaire data, and related issues such as rater-bias and variations in reliability. Addressing such issues is beyond the scope of this review. More fundamental limitations and assumptions associated with twin and adoption studies and structural equation modelling remain as relevant as in the individual studies themselves. These have been explored and discussed in detail elsewhere (Derks, Dolan, & Boomsma, 2006; LoParo & Waldman, 2014; Plomin, Willerman, & Loehlin, 1976; Tomarken & Waller, 2005).

Conclusion and future directions

Overall, this review highlights several key ways in which genetically informative, developmentally-contextualised research into child and adolescent emotional and behavioural problems can inform the wider study of psychopathology. The morbidity and disruption associated with these problems at the time they present may be justification enough for studying their aetiologies, and the fact that they also represent significant risk factors for later life psychopathology only strengthens the case for this. However, ascertaining the mechanisms and pathways by which this risk is mediated will necessarily require developmentally-contextualised approaches. The results of investigations into the aetiological architecture of stability and change in emotional and behavioural problems provide a strong empirical base for this process and, used in combination with findings from molecular, epidemiological, cognitive-experimental and clinical studies, can continue to play an important role in future.

Acknowledgements

This study presents independent research part-funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. LJH is supported by a 1+3 PhD studentship from the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC). MW is supported by The Alexander von Humboldt Foundation. TAM was first supported by a grant to TCE from the Leverhulme Trust [RPG-210] then by a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, jointly funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (107706/Z/15/Z)!during the preparation of this manuscript. We would like to thank those authors who provided additional results or information pertaining to their studies at our request. We would also like to thank Giorgia Michelini for her assistance in the preparation of this article.

Footnotes

1

One exception was made to this criterion, in the case of a relevant study (Waszczuk, Zavos, Gregory, & Eley, 2016), the forthcoming publication of which we were aware due to the involvement of some of the authors of this review. We opted to include the results of this study in anticipation of publication at a similar time to this review.

2

This category incorporates those studies that use a measure of temperament or a broader or more general emotional problems phenotype (usually 'internalising behaviour'). Therefore, it overlaps with the previous categories phenotypically, but not in terms of the studies included.

3

This category incorporates only those studies that use a broader or more general phenotype of behavioural problems (usually 'externalising behaviour'). Therefore, it overlaps with the previous categories phenotypically, but not in terms of the studies included.

References

  1. Anselmi L, Barros FC, Teodoro MLM, Piccinini CA, Menezes AMB, Araujo CL, Rohde LA. Continuity of behavioral and emotional problems from pre-school years to pre-adolescence in a developing country. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2008;49:499–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01865.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asbury K, Dunn J, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on individual differences in early behavioral development: a monozygotic twin differences study. Child Development. 2003;74(3):933–43. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asbury K, Dunn J, Plomin R. The use of discordant MZ twins to generate hypotheses regarding non-shared environmental influence on anxiety in middle childhood. Social Development. 2006 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00356.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bardone A, Moffitt T, Caspi A, Dickson N, Stanton W, Silva P. Adult Physical Health Outcomes of Adolescent Girls With Conduct Disorder, Depression, and Anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37(6):594–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00356.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bardone A, Moffitt T, Caspi A, Dickson N, Stanton W, Silva P. Adult Physical Health Outcomes of Adolescent Girls With Conduct Disorder, Depression, and Anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37(6):594–601. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199806000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartels M, Boomsma DI, Hudziak JJ, Rietveld MJH, van Beijsterveldt CEM, van den Oord EJCG. Disentangling genetic, environmental, and rater effects on internalizing and externalizing problem behavior in 10-year-old twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2004;7(2):162–175. doi: 10.1375/twin.7.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartels M, van den Oord EJCG, Hudziak JJ, Rietveld MJH, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental mechanisms underlying stability and change in problem behaviors at ages 3, 7, 10, and 12. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(5):852–67. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergen SE, Gardner CO, Kendler KS. Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10(3):423–33. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.3.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bezdjian S, Tuvblad C, Wang P, Raine A, Baker LA. Motor impulsivity during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal biometric analysis of the go/no-go task in 9- to 18-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50(11):2549–2557. doi: 10.1037/a0038037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blakemore S-J. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9(4):267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bolhuis K, McAdams Ta, Monzani B, Gregory AM, Mataix-Cols D, Stringaris A, Eley T. Aetiological overlap between obsessive-compulsive and depressive symptoms: a longitudinal twin study in adolescents and adults. Psychological Medicine. 2014:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bongers IL, Koot HM, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Developmental Trajectories of Externalizing Behaviors in Childhood and Adolescence. Child Development. 2004;75(5):1523–1537. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Boomsma DI, Busjahn A, Peltonen L. Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2002;3(11):872–82. doi: 10.1038/nrg932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1844–67. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brody GH, Beach SRH, Philibert R, Chen Y-F, Lei M-K, Murry VM, Brown AC. Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability factor in longitudinal increases in youths' substance use. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77(1):1–11. doi: 10.1037/a0012996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bufferd S, Dougherty L, Carlson G, Rose S, Klein D. Psychiatric Disorders in Preschoolers: Continuity From Ages 3 to 6. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169(11):1157–1164. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Burt SA. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(4):608–37. doi: 10.1037/a0015702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Burt SA. Research Review: The shared environment as a key source of variability in child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55(4):304–12. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Caspi A, Henry B, McGee R, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: from age three to age fifteen. Child Development. 1995;66(1):55–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00855.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, Moffitt TE, et al. The p Factor: One General Psychopathology Factor in the Structure of Psychiatric Disorders? Clinical Psychological Science. 2013 doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, Asherson PJ, Larsson H. Developmental Twin Study of Attention Problems. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(3):311. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cherny SS, Fulker D, Corley R, Plomin R, DeFries JC. Continuity and change in infant shyness from 14 to 20 months. Behavior Genetics. 1994;24(4):365–379. doi: 10.1007/BF01067538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cicchetti D, Rogosch F. A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(1):6–20. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A. Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66(7):764–772. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):837–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Degnan Ka, Almas AN, Fox N. Temperament and the environment in the etiology of childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51(4):497–517. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02228.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dekker MC, Ferdinand RF, van Lang NDJ, Bongers IL, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Developmental trajectories of depressive symptoms from early childhood to late adolescence: Gender differences and adult outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2007;48(7):657–666. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01742.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Derks EM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI. A test of the equal environment assumption (EEA) in multivariate twin studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2006;9(3):403–11. doi: 10.1375/183242706777591290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dumas JE, Nissley J, Nordstrom A, Smith EP, Prinz RJ, Levine DW. Home chaos: sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34(1):93–104. doi: 10.1080/10888430903150642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ebejer JL, Coventry WL, Byrne B, Willcutt EG, Olson RK, Corley R, Samuelsson S. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Inattention, Hyperactivity-Impulsivity, and Reading: Kindergarten to Grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2010;14(4):293–316. doi: 10.1080/10888430903150642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Guthrie IK, Reiser M. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78(1):136–157. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eley T. General Genes: A New Theme in Developmental Psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1997;6(4):90–95. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512831. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eley T, Hudson JL, Creswell C, Tropeano M, Lester KJ, Cooper P, Collier Da, et al. Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Molecular Psychiatry. 2012;17(3):236–237. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Eley T, Lichtenstein P, Moffitt TE. A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:383–402. doi: 10.1017/S095457940300021X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Elkins IJ, McGue M, Iacono WG. Genetic and environmental influences on parent-son relationships: evidence for increasing genetic influence during adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(2):351–363. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ferdinand RF, Dieleman G, Ormel J, Verhulst FC. Homotypic versus heterotypic continuity of anxiety symptoms in young adolescents: Evidence for distinctions between DSM-IV subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35:325–333. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9093-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gagne JR, Hill Goldsmith H. A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. Developmental Science. 2011;14(1):112–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00969.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Garcia SE, Tully EC, Tarantino N, South S, Jacono WG, McGue M. Changes in genetic and environmental influences on trait anxiety from middle adolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;151(1):46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ge X, Conger RD, Elder GH. Pubertal transition, stressful life events, and the emergence of gender differences in adolescent depressive symptoms. Developmental Psychology. 2001;37(3):404–417. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200110000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gillespie NA, Kirk KM, Evans DM, Heath AC, Hickie IB, Martin N. Do the genetic or environmental determinants of anxiety and depression change with age? A longitudinal study of Australian twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2004;7(1):39–53. doi: 10.1375/13690520460741435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gregory AM, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Koenen KC, Eley T, Poulton R. Juvenile mental health histories of adults with anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:301–308. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.301. February. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Haberstick BC, Schmitz S, Young SE, Hewitt JK. Contributions of genes and environments to stability and change in externalizing and internalizing problems during elementary and middle school. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35(4):381–96. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-1747-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Haberstick BC, Schmitz S, Young SE, Hewitt JK. Genes and developmental stabiltiy of aggressive behavior problems at home and school in a community sample of twins aged 7-12. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36:809–819. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9092-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hallett V, Ronald A, Rijsdijk FV, Eley T. Phenotypic and genetic differentiation of anxiety-related behaviours in middle childhood. Depression and Anxiety. 2009;324:316–324. doi: 10.1002/da.20539. September 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R, Angell KE. Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: eme. 1998 doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.1.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hannigan LJ, McAdams TA, Plomin R, Eley TC. Etiological Influences on Perceptions of Parenting: A Longitudinal, Multi-Informant Twin Study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0419-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Harden KP, Quinn PD, Tucker-Drob EM. Genetically influenced change in sensation seeking drives the rise of delinquent behavior during adolescence. Developmental Science. 2012;15:150–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01115.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Harrington R, Fudge H, Rutter M, Pickles A, Hill J. Adult outcomes of childhood and adolescent depression. I. Psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1990;47(5):465–473. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810170065010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hettema JM, Chen X, Sun C, Brown TA. Direct, indirect and pleiotropic effects of candidate genes on internalizing disorder psychopathology. Psychological Medicine. 2015:1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hinshaw SP. Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111(1):127–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Hudziak JJ, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental influences on the stability of withdrawn behavior in children: a longitudinal, multi-informant twin study. Behavior Genetics. 2008;38(5):447–61. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9213-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Continuity and change of psychopathology from childhood into adulthood: a 14-year follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39(7):850–858. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200007000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Child and adolescent problems predict DSM-IV disorders in adulthood: a 14-year follow-up of a Dutch epidemiological sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(2):182–189. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200202000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Huizink AC, van den Berg MP, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Longitudinal genetic analysis of internalizing and externalizing problem behavior in adopted biologically related and unrelated sibling pairs. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10(1):55–65. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14(2):395–416. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402002110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jaffee S, Hanscombe KB, Haworth CMA, Davis OSP, Plomin R. Chaotic homes and children's disruptive behavior: a longitudinal cross-lagged twin study. Psychological Science. 2012;23(6):643–50. doi: 10.1177/0956797611431693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jaffee S, Price TS, Reyes TM. Behavior genetics: Past, present, future. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(4pt2):1225–1242. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kadosh KC, Linden DEJ, Lau JYF. Plasticity during childhood and adolescence: Innovative approaches to investigating neurocognitive development. Developmental Science. 2013;16(4):574–583. doi: 10.1111/desc.12054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kan K-J, Dolan CV, Nivard MG, Middeldorp CM, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Willemsen G, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental stability in attention problems across the lifespan: evidence from the Netherlands twin register. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(1):12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Annas P, Michael C, Eaves LJ, Lichtenstein P. A Longitudinal Twin Study of Fears From Middle Childhood to Early Adulthood: Evidence for a Developmentally Dynamic Genome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(4):421–429. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.4.421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Lichtenstein P. A developmental twin study of symptoms of anxiety and depression: evidence for genetic innovation and attenuation. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(11):1567–75. doi: 10.1017/S003329170800384X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime Prevalence and Age-of-Onset Distributions of DSM-IV Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):593. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(7):709–717. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Klein AM, Otto Y, Fuchs S, Reibiger I, von Klitzing K. A prospective study of behavioral and emotional symptoms in preschoolers. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;24(3):291–299. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0575-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Knafo A, Jaffee S. Gene-environment correlation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(1):1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kovacs M, Devlin B. Internalizing disorders in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39(1):47–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Krebs G, Waszczuk MA, Zavos HMS, Bolton D, Eley T. Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive behaviour across development: a longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine. 2014:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kuntsi J, Rijsdijk FV, Ronald A, Asherson PJ, Plomin R. Genetic influences on the stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms from early to middle childhood. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:647–654. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lacourse E, Boivin M, Brendgen M, Petitclerc A, Girard A, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE. A longitudinal twin study of physical aggression during early childhood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44(12):2617–2627. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713003218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lahey BB, Van Hulle Ca, Rathouz PJ, Rodgers JL, D'Onofrio B, Waldman ID. Are oppositional-defiant and hyperactive-inattentive symptoms developmental precursors to conduct problems in late childhood?: Genetic and environmental links. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(1):45–58. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9257-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lamb DJ, Middeldorp CM, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Bartels M, van der Aa N, Polderman TJC, Boomsma DI. Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49(3):248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Larsson B, Frisk M. Social competence and emotional/behaviour problems in 6- 16 year-old Swedish school children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999;8(1):24–33. doi: 10.1007/s007870050080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, Larsson J-O. Genetic contributions to the development of ADHD subtypes from childhood to adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(8):973–981. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000222787.57100.d8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Larsson J-O, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: a longitudinal twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43(10):1267–1275. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000135622.05219.bf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lau JYF, Eley T. Changes in genetic and environmental influences on depressive symptoms across adolescence and young adulthood. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;189:422–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.018721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lau JYF, Eley T. The genetics of mood disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010;6:313–37. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lester KJ, Eley T. Therapygenetics: Using genetic markers to predict response to psychological treatment for mood and anxiety disorders. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders. 2013;3(1):4. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lewis GJ, Plomin R. Heritable influences on behavioural problems from early childhood to mid-adolescence: evidence for genetic stability and innovation. Psychological Medicine. 2015:1–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Loehlin JC. The Cholesky approach: A cautionary note. Behavior Genetics. 1996;26(1):65–69. doi: 10.1007/BF02361160. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. LoParo D, Waldman ID. Twins' Rearing Environment Similarity and Childhood Externalizing Disorders: A Test of the Equal Environments Assumption. Behavior Genetics. 2014;44(6):606–613. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9685-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lopez B, Turner RJ, Saavedra LM. Anxiety and risk for substance dependence among late adolescents/young adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2005;19(3):275–294. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Luo YLL, Haworth CMa, Plomin R. A novel approach to genetic and environmental analysis of cross-lagged associations over time: the cross-lagged relationship between self-perceived abilities and school achievement is mediated by genes as well as the environment. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2010;13(5):426–436. doi: 10.1375/twin.13.5.426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Malone SM, Taylor J, Marmorstein NR, McGue M, Iacono WG. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior and alcohol dependence from adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16(4):943–966. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404040088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. McLeod JD, Kaiser K. Childhood Emotional and Behavioral Problems and Educational Attainment. American Sociological Review. 2004 doi: 10.1177/000312240406900502. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Merikangas KR, He J-P, Brody D, Fisher PW, Bourdon K, Koretz DS. Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders among US children in the 2001-2004 NHANES. Pediatrics. 2010;125(1):75–81. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Merikangas KR, He J-P, Burstein M, Swanson Sa, Avenevoli S, Cui L, Swendsen J. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication--Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A) Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49(10):980–989. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Mesman J, Koot HM. Early preschool predictors of preadolescent internalizing and externalizing DSM-IV diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(9):1029–1036. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200109000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Middeldorp CM, Cath DC, Van Dyck R, Boomsma DI. The co-morbidity of anxiety and depression in the perspective of genetic epidemiology. A review of twin and family studies. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35(5):611–624. doi: 10.1017/S003329170400412X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100(4):674–701. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13(2):355–375. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401002097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Nadder TS, Rutter M, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Eaves LJ. Genetic effects on the variation and covariation of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional-defiant disorder/conduct disorder (Odd/CD) symptomatologies across informant and occasion of measurement. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(1):39–53. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702229530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington E. Genetically informative designs for distinguishing developmental pathways during adolescence: Responsible and antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8(04):779. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007422. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Nes RB, Røysamb E, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Harris JR, Tambs K. Symptoms of anxiety and depression in young adults: genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10(3):450–61. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.3.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Niv S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA. Aggression and rule-breaking: Heritability and stability of antisocial behavior problems in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2013;41(5):285–291. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Niv S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Wang P, Baker LA. Heritability and longitudinal stability of impulsivity in adolescence. Behavior Genetics. 2012;42:378–392. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9518-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Nivard MG, Dolan CV, Kendler KS, Kan K-J, Willemsen G, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI. Stability in symptoms of anxiety and depression as a function of genotype and environment: a longitudinal twin study from ages 3 to 63 years. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(05):1039–1049. doi: 10.1017/S003329171400213X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. O'Connor TG, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington E, Plomin R. Genetic contributions to continuity, change, and co-occurrence of antisocial and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39(3):323–336. doi: 10.1017/S0021963097002126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Ollendick TH, King NJ. Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment of internalizing problems in children: the role of longitudinal data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62(5):918–27. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.62.5.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9(12):947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Plomin R. Commentary: Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2011 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM. Behavioral Genetics. 6th ed. New York: Worth Publishers; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  103. Plomin R, Emde RN, Braungart J, Campos J, Corley R, Fulker D, Zahn-Waxler C. Genetic change and continuity from fourteen to twenty months: the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Development. 1993;64(5):1354–1376. doi: 10.2307/1131539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Plomin R, Willerman L, Loehlin JC. Resemblance in appearance and the equal environments assumption in twin studies of personality traits. Behavior Genetics. 1976;6(1):43–52. doi: 10.1007/BF01065677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Polderman TJC, Huizink AC, Verhulst FC, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI, Bartels M. A genetic study on attention problems and academic skills: Results of a longitudinal study in twins. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;20(1):22–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Price TS, Simonoff E, Asherson PJ, Curran S, Kuntsi J, Waldman ID, Plomin R. Continuity and change in preschool ADHD symptoms: Longitudinal genetic analysis with contrast effects. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35(2):121–132. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-1013-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rapee RM, Schniering Ca, Hudson JL. Anxiety disorders during childhood and adolescence: origins and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2009;5:311–41. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(3):490–529. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rice F, Harold GT, Thapar A. The genetic aetiology of childhood depression: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2002 Jan; doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rietveld MJH, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI. Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results froma studyof twins, age 3 to12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(3):577–588. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rijsdijk FV, Sham PC. Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics. 2002;3(2):119–33. doi: 10.1093/bib/3.2.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Roberson-Nay R, Eaves LJ, Hettema JM, Kendler KS, Silberg JL. Childhood separation anxiety disorder and adult onset panic attacks share a common genetic diathesis. Depression and Anxiety. 2012;29(4):320–327. doi: 10.1002/da.21931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Roza SJ, Hofstra MB, Van Der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Stable prediction of mood and anxiety disorders based on behavioral and emotional problems in childhood: A 14-year follow-up during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003 Dec;160:2116–2121. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rutter M. Pathways of genetic influences on psychopathology. European Review. 2004;12(01):19–33. doi: 10.1017/S1062798704000031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006;47:276–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Rutter M, Silberg JL. Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:463–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Rutter M, Silberg JL, O'Connor TG, Simonoff E. Genetics and child psychiatry: II Empirical research findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1999;40(1):19–55. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Scarr S, McCartney K. How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Development. 1983;54(2):424–435. doi: 10.2307/1129703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Schmitz S, Fulker D, Mrazek Da. Problem behavior in early and middle childhood: An initial behavior genetic analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1995;36(8):1443–1458. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01674.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Scourfield J, Rice F, Thapar A, Harold GT, Martin N, McGuffin P. Depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: changing aetiological influences with development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44(7):968–76. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Silberg JL, Rutter M, Eaves LJ. Genetic and environmental influences on the temporal association between earlier anxiety and later depression in girls. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49(12):1040–1049. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01161-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Silberg J, Pickles A, Rutter M, Hewitt J, Simonoff E, Maes H, Eaves L. The influence of genetic factors and life stress on depression among adolescent girls. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56(3):225–232. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.3.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sourander A, Helstelä L. Childhood predictors of externalizing and internalizing problems in adolescence: A prospective follow-up study from age 8 to 16. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;14(8):415–423. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0475-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Takahashi Y, Yamagata S, Kijima N, Shigemasu K, Ono Y, Ando J. Continuity and change in behavioral inhibition and activation systems: A longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43(6):1616–1625. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Taylor MJ, Charman T, Robinson EB, Plomin R, Happé F, Asherson PJ, Ronald A. Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43:1735–46. doi: 10.1017/S003329171200253X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK. Depression in adolescence. Lancet. 2012;379(9820):1056–67. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60871-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Tomarken AJ, Waller NG. Structural equation modeling: strengths, limitations, and misconceptions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005;1:31–65. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Traylor M, Markus H, Lewis CM. Homogeneous case subgroups increase power in genetic association studies. European Journal of Human Genetics. 2014 Apr;:1–7. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2014.194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tremblay RE, Nagin DS, Séguin JR, Zoccolillo M, Zelazo PD, Boivin M, Japel C, et al. Physical aggression during early childhood: trajectories and predictors. The Canadian Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Review = La Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie de L'enfant et de L'adolescent. 2005;14(1):3–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.114.1.e43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Trzaskowski M, Davis OSP, DeFries JC, Yang J, Visscher PM, Plomin R. DNA evidence for strong genome-wide pleiotropy of cognitive and learning abilities. Behavior Genetics. 2013;43(4):267–273. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9594-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Trzaskowski M, Zavos HMS, Haworth CMa, Plomin R, Eley T. Stable genetic influence on anxiety-related behaviours across middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(1):85–94. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9545-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Tully EC, Iacono WG, McGue M. Changes in genetic and environmental influences on the development of nicotine dependence and major depressive disorder from middle adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22(2010):831–848. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Turkheimer EN. Three Laws of Behavior Genetics and What They Mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9(5):160–164. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00084. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  134. Turkheimer EN, Waldron M. Nonshared environment: a theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(1):78–108. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.1261.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Tuvblad C, Eley T, Lichtenstein P. The development of antisocial behavior from childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal twin study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;14:216–225. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0458-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Tuvblad C, Narusyte J, Grann M, Sarnecki J, Lichtenstein P. The genetic and environmental etiology of antisocial behavior from childhood to emerging adulthood. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41(5):629–40. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9463-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Tuvblad C, Raine A, Zheng M, Baker LA. Genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 2009;35:437–452. doi: 10.1002/ab.20319. July 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Van Beijsterveldt CEM, Bartels M, Hudziak JJ, Boomsma DI. Causes of stability of aggression from early childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal genetic analysis in Dutch twins. Behavior Genetics. 2003;33(5):591–605. doi: 10.1023/A:1025735002864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Van Den Berg SM, Willemsen G, De Geus EJC, Boomsma DI. Genetic etiology of stability of attention problems in young adulthood. American Journal of Medical Genetics - Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2006;141 B(1):55–60. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Van den Oord EJCG, Rowe DC. Continuity and change in children's social maladjustment: a developmental behavior genetic study. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(2):319–332. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Van der Valk J, van den Oord EJCG, Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change in children's internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42(10):1212–1220. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200310000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Van der Valk J, Verhulst FC, Neale MC, Boomsma DI. Longitudinal genetic analysis of problem behaviors in biologically related and unrelated adoptees. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28(5):365–380. doi: 10.1023/A:1021621719059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Van Grootheest DS, Bartels M, Cath DC, Beekman AT, Hudziak JJ, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental contributions underlying stability in childhood obsessive-compulsive behavior. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61(3):308–15. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Van Grootheest DS, Cath DC, Hottenga JJ, Beekman AT, Boomsma DI. Genetic factors underlie stability of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12(5):411–419. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.5.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Van Hulle Ca, Waldman ID, D'Onofrio B, Rodgers JL, Rathouz PJ, Lahey BB. Developmental structure of genetic influences on antisocial behavior across childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118(4):711–721. doi: 10.1037/a0016761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Verhulst FC, van der Ende J. The eight-year stability of problem behavior in an epidemiologic sample. Pediatric Research. 1995;38(4):612–617. doi: 10.1203/00006450-199510000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Vierikko E, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Rose RJ. Genetic and environmental sources of continuity and change in teacher-rated aggression during early adolescence. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:308–320. doi: 10.1002/ab.20117. (April 2004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  148. Wang Z, Deater-Deckard K, Petrill SA, Thompson LA. Externalizing problems, attention regulation, and household chaos: A longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Development and Psychopathology. 2012 Aug; doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Waszczuk MA, Zavos HMS, Eley T. Genetic and environmental influences on relationship between anxiety sensitivity and anxiety subscales in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2013;27(5):475–84. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Waszczuk MA, Zavos HMS, Gregory AM, Eley T. The continuity and change of aetiological influences on depression, anxiety disorder symptoms and their co-occurrence across adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Medicine. 2016 doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Waszczuk MA, Zavos HMS, Gregory AM, Eley T. The Phenotypic and Genetic Structure of Depression and Anxiety Disorder Symptoms in Childhood, Adolescence, and Young Adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(8):905–916. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Weems CF. Developmental trajectories of childhood anxiety: Identifying continuity and change in anxious emotion. Developmental Review. 2008;28(4):488–502. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2008.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  153. Weissman MM, Wolk S, Wickramaratne P, Goldstein RB, Adams P, Greenwald S, Steinberg D, et al. Children with prepubertal-onset major depressive disorder and anxiety grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56(9):794–801. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Wertz J, Zavos H, Matthews T, Harvey K, Hunt A, Pariante CM, Arseneault L. Why some children with externalising problems develop internalizing symptoms: testing two pathways in a genetically sensitive cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;56(7):738–746. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wichers M, Gardner CO, Maes HH, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H, Kendler KS. Genetic innovation and stability in externalizing problem behavior across development: a multi-informant twin study. Behavior Genetics. 2013;43(3):191–201. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9586-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Zavos HMS, Gregory AM, Eley T. Longitudinal genetic analysis of anxiety sensitivity. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48(1):204–12. doi: 10.1037/a0024996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES