Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Mar 22.
Published in final edited form as: Lang Learn Dev. 2017 Jan 31;13(2):171–190. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2016.1263573

Does the approximate number system serve as a foundation for symbolic mathematics?

Emily Szkudlarek 1, Elizabeth M Brannon 1
PMCID: PMC5362122  NIHMSID: NIHMS851593  PMID: 28344520

Introduction

Although we are not always explicitly aware of our deep dependence on number, mathematics is an integral part of our daily lives. Every time we tell time, determine how many quarters to put in the parking meter, pay our bills, grade a student’s exam, or determine the materials needed for a kitchen renovation we are relying on our numerical abilities. Many of these tasks rely on skills honed through years of school-based mathematics education. The concept of natural number, the set of all whole, non-negative numbers, is fundamental to symbolic mathematical thought. A central question in cognitive science is how and from where do natural number concepts arise? To resolve this question, we must come to understand the preverbal cognitive capacities that mathematical education builds upon, discover the evolutionary foundations that support mathematical thinking, and ultimately identify how natural number concepts emerge from these foundations.

While non-human animals and human infants will never prove an algebraic theorem, calculate the tip at a restaurant, or double the proportions to implement a recipe, there is nevertheless extensive evidence that many animals and even the youngest of babies have an intuitive number sense termed here the approximate number system (ANS; Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004). The ANS has two behavioral hallmarks, the distance and size effects. The distance effect refers to the fact that it is easier to discriminate numbers that are further apart in numerical distance (2 vs. 7 is easier than 2 vs. 4), while the size effect refers to the observation that it is easier to discriminate smaller numbers compared to larger numbers at the same distance (2 vs 4 is easier than 22 vs 24). Thus, ANS representations of number follow Weber’s law such that the difficulty in discriminating any two numbers is dependent on the ratio between them, rather than their absolute difference. In this way the representations supported by the ANS are fundamentally distinct from the exact representations made possible by Arabic numerals. Arabic numerals, for example, allow us to appreciate that the difference between 18 and 19 is exactly the same as the difference between 1,834 and 1,835. The ability to precisely represent number is a necessary prerequisite for humans to perform complex calculations and a myriad of other mathematical endeavors. In contrast, the ANS only supports fuzzy magnitude representations that are ratio dependent. Despite these profound differences, there is growing evidence of a link between the ANS and symbolic mathematical ability. In this review, we first summarize properties of the ANS by examining the number sense of infants and non-human primates. We then review the evidence linking symbolic math skill to the ANS, and discuss the potential causes of conflicting findings in the literature. Finally, we discuss implications of this relationship both for the genesis of natural number concepts and educational applications.

Properties of the ANS – Evidence from Infants and Non-Human Primates

Numerical competence has been observed throughout the animal kingdom, from insects to primates. Numerical representations allow North Island robins to determine successful caching strategies, mosquito and stickleback fish to determine the more numerous social group, and elephants to choose the larger amount of food (Agrillo, Dadda, Serena, & Bisazza, 2008; Garland, Low, & Burns, 2012; Mehlis, Thünken, Bakker, & Frommen, 2015; Perdue, Talbot, Stone, & Beran, 2012). This widespread use of quantity discrimination throughout the animal kingdom is evidence of either the emergence of numerical abilities in a very early common ancestor, or of convergent evolution. Regardless of the evolutionary origin of numerical ability, the ubiquity of these competencies suggests that being able to discriminate the larger of two sets is highly adaptive.

Similarly, infants just moments after birth are sensitive to the numerical attributes of the world around them (Izard, Sann, Spelke, & Streri, 2009). Multiple experimental methods that make use of infants’ gaze direction, gaze duration, and brain waves all provide converging evidence that infants rely on the ANS to make numerical discriminations (Hyde & Spelke, 2011; Libertus & Brannon, 2009, 2010; Lipton & Spelke, 2003; Xu & Spelke, 2000). For example, when 6-month-old infants are shown two image streams displaying dot arrays that frequently change in the spacing, location, and size of the dots, infants preferentially attend to the stream that is changing numerically over the stream that is numerically constant (Libertus & Brannon, 2010). Whereas newborns require a 1:3 ratio to detect differences in numerosity (Izard et al., 2009), by 6 months of age babies can handle a 1:2 ratio, and by 9-months a 2:3 ratio (Libertus & Brannon, 2010; Lipton & Spelke, 2003; Xu & Spelke, 2000). This increase in acuity continues throughout childhood (Halberda & Feigenson, 2008), and in a large-scale online study was seen to continue to improve until around 30 years of age (Halberda, Ly, Wilmer, Naiman, & Germine, 2012).

A contentious question in both the comparative and developmental literatures has been whether the putative ability to represent approximate number can be better explained as a sensitivity to continuous variables such as surface area or contours (Clearfield & Mix, 1999; Clearfield & Mix, 2001; Cordes & Brannon, 2009; Davis & Pérusse, 1988). For example, Mix and colleagues argued that poor stimulus controls in many infant studies likely resulted in effects that were driven by infants’ attending to perimeter or surface area rather than numerosity (Mix, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2002). A recent series of studies with human infants came to a contrary conclusion using the change detection procedure (Libertus, Starr, & Brannon, 2014; Starr & Brannon, 2015). In one study infants were presented with a screen in which images changed by a 1:3 ratio in area and were constant in numerosity, and a second screen in which the stimuli changed by a 1:3 ratio in numerosity and remained constant in area. Infants overwhelmingly preferred the screen that changed in numerosity. Only when the change in area was increased to a 1:10 ratio did infants look equally long at the two streams. These findings provide strong evidence that when young infants are presented with arrays of discrete items, numerosity is more salient than the cumulative continuous variables of the sets. Cordes and colleagues conducted another series of studies with the visual habituation procedure and came to similar conclusion (Cordes & Brannon, 2008). In those studies, 6-month old infants required a 1:2 ratio change in numerosity to show a reliable novelty effect when tested in the visual habituation paradigm. However, when cumulative surface area of the dot arrays was manipulated, Cordes and colleagues found that 6-month-old infants required a 4-fold change in the cumulative surface area of an array to show a novelty effect. These studies suggest that infants represent both number and continuous variables, but show greater sensitivity to numerical attributes of discrete arrays.

Studies of animal numerical cognition have yielded similar controversies over the relative salience of numerosity and continuous variables of dot arrays. For example, Davis & Pérusse (1988) argued that animals only attend to the numerical attribute of the stimuli when they have had extensive artificial laboratory training to do so, and thus as a “last resort strategy.” In contrast, Gallistel (1990) proposed that number, along with time and space, are critical attributes of the world that all organisms represent. Cantlon and colleagues addressed the question of the relative salience of numerosity and other non-numerical attributes in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Monkeys were trained in a match-to-sample task where the sample and the correct match were the same both in number and in cumulative surface area. Monkeys were then given non-differentially reinforced probe trials where one choice matched the sample in number and not total area, and the other matched in total area and not number. Rhesus monkeys were more likely to match arrays based on numerosity than surface area when the two cues were in conflict (Cantlon & Brannon, 2007). Collectively these studies suggest that although it may seem intuitive that number is more cognitively challenging to represent than continuous variables, this intuition is misleading. Instead, when babies and monkeys are presented with sets of discrete items they are more likely to detect numerosity than they are to form summary statistics of continuous variables.

A powerful aspect of the ANS is that despite its imprecision, it supports arithmetic calculations. For example, rhesus monkeys are capable of performing addition and subtraction operations using ANS representations (Cantlon & Brannon, 2007; Cantlon, Merritt, & Brannon, 2015). In these studies, monkeys were shown addition operations where two sets disappeared behind an occluder, or subtraction operations where after an initial set was occluded a subset of the first array emerged from behind the occluder and left the screen. Monkeys were then given a choice between an array that numerically summed the two addends (or represented the difference in the subtraction problem) and a distractor array. Monkeys were then tested on probe trials with novel numerical values, where they performed above chance expectation indicating acquisition of the arithmetic rules. Training and probe trial performance was dependent on the ratio between the correct answer and the distractor, following Weber’s law.

The violation of expectancy paradigm suggests that infants also keep track of objects over addition and subtraction events. Karen Wynn pioneered the method demonstrating that infants form expectations about the number of objects that should be behind an occluder when they watch addition and subtraction events (Wynn, 1992). However, in these early experiments it was unclear whether infants relied on the ANS or instead relied on an object-file system that is limited to representing small sets. If infants were relying on the object-file system they would detect incorrect arithmetic solutions by tracking each object, holding these objects in memory, and then noticing the difference between the number of objects stored in memory and the number presented after the arithmetic event. However, McCrink and Wynn (2004, 2009) showed nine-month-old infants animations of arithmetic problems with sets too large to be represented by an object tracking system. When shown events such as 5 + 5 = 5 or 5 + 5 = 10 infants looked significantly longer at the impossible outcomes, presumably because they found them surprising. In other studies, Spelke and colleagues have shown that young children make approximate arithmetic calculations that show systematic ratio dependence (Barth, La Mont, Lipton, & Spelke, 2005; Gilmore, McCarthy, & Spelke, 2010). Moreover, animals and human infants are both adept at approximate ordinal numerical comparisons. Monkeys show ratio dependent number discrimination when trained to choose the larger of two numerical arrays and transfer to novel numerical values (e.g., Brannon & Terrace, 1998) and human infants habituated to a sequence of images that increased (or decreased) in numerosity dishabituated to novel sequences that displayed the reverse ordinal relationship (e.g., Brannon, 2002).

The ANS also supports the capacity to reason about second order numerical relationships. Drucker, Rossa, & Brannon (2015) presented rhesus monkeys with two arrays that each contained one or more positive items that were associated with reward or negative items that were associated with the absence of reward (see Figure 1A). The monkeys were reinforced for choosing the array that had the more favorable ratio of positive to negative items while ignoring the absolute number of positive or negative items in each array. The monkeys were then tested with novel ratio comparisons. During these unreinforced probe trials the moneys continued to choose the array with a higher ratio of positive to negative items. The monkeys’ performance improved as the ratio between ratios increased, following Weber’s law. This finding demonstrates that monkeys are able to represent the ratio of good to bad items in an array, and compare these two ratios to choose the array that is more favorable.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A) Example stimuli presented to rhesus monkeys in Drucker, Rossa, & Brannon (2015). In this example the black circle is the positive stimulus and the white diamond is the negative stimulus. The correct answer is on the left. Figure modified from Drucker, Rossa, & Brannon (2015). B) Example stimuli presented to 6 month old infants in McCrink & Wynn (2007). The example habituation ratio shown here is 1:4. Figure modified from McCrink & Wynn (2007).

Human infants also represent the ratio between sets of discrete quantities (McCrink & Wynn, 2007). When infants were habituated to arrays that showed a constant proportion of blue to yellow shapes they selectively dishabituated to new arrays that featured a different proportion of yellow to blue shapes (See Figure 1B). Thus the ANS not only allows for the approximate representation of numerical values, it supports mental transformations across those representations. These transformations include arithmetic operations, ordinal relationships, and proportional reasoning in human infants and non-human primates.

Is the ANS foundational for symbolic mathematics?

The previous section established the existence of a preverbal number sense present in nonhuman animals and human infants. In this section we explore five lines of evidence that this primitive system is meaningfully related to our uniquely human mathematical mind. The first line of evidence comes from studies that demonstrate a positive correlation between ANS acuity and symbolic math ability. In the first study to show this relationship, Halberda and colleagues demonstrated that Weber fraction measured at 14 years of age retroactively predicted standardized math scores at age 5, even after controlling for verbal IQ (Halberda, Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008). This positive correlation has since been found in adults (Agrillo, Piffer, & Adriano, 2013; DeWind & Brannon, 2012; Halberda, Ly, Wilmer, Naiman, & Germine, 2012; Libertus, Odic, & Halberda, 2012; Lourenco, Bonny, Fernandez, & Rao, 2012); in school-aged children (Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Rouder, 2015; Pinheiro-Chagas et al., 2014); in children just beginning formal math education (Gilmore et al., 2010; Keller & Libertus, 2015; Mundy & Gilmore, 2009); and in preschool aged children before they begin formal math education (Chu, vanMarle, & Geary, 2015; Libertus, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011, 2013; Mazzocco, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011b; Soto-Calvo, Simmons, Willis, & Adams, 2015; Starr, Libertus, & Brannon, 2013; van Marle, Chu, Li, & Geary, 2014). Three meta-analyses have concluded there is a significant correlation between ANS and math ability (Chen & Li, 2014; Fazio, Bailey, Thompson, & Siegler, 2014; Schneider et al., 2016). A correlation between ANS acuity and math has, however, failed to emerge in other studies (Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Iuculano, Tang, Hall, & Butterworth, 2008; Lyons, Price, Vaessen, Blomert, & Ansari, 2014; Nosworthy, Bugden, Archibald, Evans, & Ansari, 2013; Sasanguie, Defever, Maertens, & Reynvoet, 2014). We return to these discrepant results and their implications for understanding the role the ANS plays in symbolic math knowledge later in this review.

A second line of evidence comes from longitudinal studies investigating the relationship between numerical acuity and later symbolic math performance. In one study, numerical change detection scores at 6 months of age predicted some of the variance in both ANS acuity and the Test of Early Mathematical Achievement, but not verbal IQ, in 3.5 year-olds. (Starr, Libertus & Brannon, 2013). Recent longitudinal studies that follow children in the preschool years have revealed a nuanced relationship between the ANS and early symbolic math skills (Purpura & Logan, 2015; Soto-Calvo et al., 2015). Soto-Calvo and colleagues measured preschoolers’ ANS ability, a variety of symbolic math skills, phonological awareness, and visual-spatial short term memory at age 4 and then again 14 months later (Soto-Calvo et al., 2015). ANS ability, along with visual-spatial short term memory and phonological awareness, predicted children’s accuracy in solving addition and subtraction word problems, but did not predict early counting proficiency. Purpura & Logan (2015) assessed children aged 3 to 5 at the beginning and end of one year of preschool on a battery of cognitive skills including ANS acuity, math language ability, and early symbolic math skills. ANS acuity predicted symbolic math ability only for children in the 25th percentile of the distribution of math scores, while math language ability predicted math scores of the 50th–75% percentile of this distribution. These non-linear relationships highlight the fact that different math skills may be important for the acquisition of complex math abilities at different stages of math development. As symbolic math ability develops the connection to ANS acuity may become more complex, but these findings provide evidence for an ongoing link in early childhood.

Evidence that the ANS acuity of young children predicts aspects of math performance suggests a particular directional effect whereby ANS acuity facilitates math performance. However, there is also evidence that the ANS is refined through practice with the symbolic number system (Matejko & Ansari, 2016; for review, Mussolin et al., 2015). Work with the Mundurukú, an indigenous group in Brazil who do not have words for numbers greater than 5, supports this position. Members of this community who have had some formal schooling in math displayed significantly better ANS acuity than members of the community with less formal schooling (Piazza, Pica, Izard, Spelke, & Dehaene, 2013). Mussolin, Nys, Content, & Leybaert (2014) twice assessed the ANS acuity and symbolic math ability of preschool children 7 months apart. Children’s performance at time 1 on the full battery of symbolic math assessments and a number word knowledge test predicted accuracy at time 2 on the non-symbolic comparison task. The reverse predictive relationship was not significant. Taken together, the directionality of a potential causal relationship between the ANS and symbolic math is unclear. The possibility of an ongoing bidirectional relationship across development is plausible, and warrants further investigation.

A third and related line of evidence supporting the proposal that the ANS is meaningfully related to symbolic math is that ANS acuity is impaired in at least a subset of children with math specific learning disabilities (Wilson & Dehaene, 2007; Butterworth, Varma, & Laurillard, 2011; Desoete, Ceulemans, De Weerdt, & Pieters, 2012; Mazzocco, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011a; Piazza et al., 2010; Pinheiro-Chagas et al., 2014). Developmental dyscalculia is a math specific learning disorder diagnosed in children struggling to learn about numbers and arithmetic, but whose performance on tests of vocabulary, IQ, and working memory are within the range of typically developing children (Butterworth et al., 2011; Landerl, Bevan, & Butterworth, 2004). Researchers have investigated low ANS acuity as one cause of this domain-specific impairment. Piazza et al. (2010) found that school aged dyscalculics (mean age 10.69) have roughly the same ANS acuity of an average typically developing 5-year-old. Mazzocco et al. (2011a) demonstrated that slightly older dyscalculics (mean age 14.83) had significantly lower ANS acuity as compared to low achieving, typically achieving, and high achieving peers. This result persisted after controlling for other domain general abilities that contribute to math ability, such as working memory for spatial locations and lexical retrieval of color and number words. Further, school aged children with math specific difficulties perform poorly on both non-symbolic comparison and non-symbolic addition, two different tasks that both utilize the ANS (Pinheiro-Chagas et al., 2014). It is important to note that developmental dyscalculia is a heterogeneous disorder with evidence supporting multiple causal factors contributing to severe mathematical difficulties (Bugden & Ansari, 2015; Defever, De Smedt, & Reynvoet, 2013; Defever, Reynvoet, & Gebuis, 2013; Fias, Menon, & Szucs, 2013; Geary, 2010). For example, Noël & Rousselle (2007; 2011) find little evidence of ANS deficits in very young dyscalculic children, and argue that this may reflect the fact that symbolic math sharpens the acuity of ANS representations rather than vice versa. There are likely multiple subtypes of dyscalculia, and one of these subtypes may comprise children with specific impairment in the ANS (Bartelet, Ansari, Vaessen, & Blomert, 2014; Skagerlund & Träff, 2016).

A fourth line of evidence that the ANS is meaningfully related to symbolic math is that overlapping brain structures are recruited when people engage in non-symbolic and symbolic numerical tasks (e.g., Butterworth & Walsh, 2011; Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003). Using an fMRI adaptation design, Piazza and colleagues showed participants dot patterns and Arabic digits in a sequence. The same numerical magnitude was repeatedly presented as dots or as a digit to create a decrease in BOLD signal. Recovery of the BOLD signal occurred when a new numerical magnitude (i.e., switch from 8 to 16) was presented, but not when a new stimulus format (i.e., switch from dots to digits) was presented. This cross notation adaptation and recovery occurred in the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus (hIPS), suggesting that the hIPS codes numerical quantities and number symbols in the same way (Piazza, Pinel, Le Bihan, & Dehaene, 2007). In an fMRI study that systematically looked at the conjunction in neural activation when completing a symbolic or non-symbolic magnitude comparison task, the right inferior parietal lobule emerged as a region significantly activated by both task formats (Holloway, Price, & Ansari, 2010; see also Libertus, Woldorff, & Brannon, 2007). Similarly, Lussier and Cantlon (2016) had participants perform within format comparisons across dot arrays, number words, and object sizes. The authors found that the right IPS in children and bilateral IPS in adults showed a distance effect for both dot and number word comparisons, but not when comparing object sizes.

In contrast, other functional imagining studies have found evidence against format independent representation of numerical quantities in the IPS using both adaptation designs and comparison tasks (Bulthé, De Smedt, & Beeck, 2014; Bulthé, De Smedt, & Beeck, 2015; Cohen-Kadosh et al., 2011; Lyons, Ansari, & Beilock, 2015). Using an adaptation paradigm, Cohen-Kadosh and colleagues (2011) presented numerals or dots in a sequence where either the magnitude, the format, or the color of the presented stimulus changed as the sequence progressed. Results indicated a recovery of the BOLD signal during a format change in the absence of a change in magnitude of the stimulus, establishing at least some degree of format dependent representation. Damarla and Just (2013) found common neural patterns for different pictorial representations of a given numerosity (i.e. 3 tomatoes and 3 cars), but different neural patterns for pictorial and symbolic representations (e.g., 3 trees and the Arabic numeral 3). However, in a later study Damarla, Cherkassky, and Just (2016) found evidence of shared representation across visual and auditory modalities for small numerical values. The way in which number is defined in the brain is currently far from being fully characterized, and even evidence for co-activation during symbolic and non-symbolic number processing is not necessarily evidence of a shared representation. Future fMRI studies are needed to elucidate the implications of overlapping brain activation, and clarify how approximate and symbolic number are represented in the brain.

The final source of evidence we will review that supports the idea that there is a meaningful relationship between the ANS and math comes from recent studies that have employed training designs in an effort to move beyond correlation and address causality (Park & Brannon, 2013, 2014; Hyde, Khanum, & Spelke, 2014). In the first of these studies, Park & Brannon (2013) trained adults to solve approximate arithmetic problems and asked how this affected their ability to perform simple symbolic calculations. On addition trials, participants observed two arrays of dots each disappear behind a centrally located occluder. On subtraction trials, participants observed a single array move behind an occluder and then a subset of items float out from behind the occluder and leave the screen. The participant’s task was to estimate the total number of dots behind the occluder to solve the addition or subtraction problem. On some trials, the participant was required to match their mental sum (or difference) to one of two presented arrays. On other trials the participant was shown a single target array and asked to compare their mental sum (or difference) to this new target array to make a greater or less than judgment. Before and after approximate arithmetic training, symbolic math performance was measured by how many two and three-digit addition and subtraction problems a participant could solve within two 5-minute blocks. As a control task, a vocabulary test was also administered. For comparison, an age-matched no-contact control group was given the pre and post-tests, but not the ANS training sessions. Results showed that the participants who trained on the approximate arithmetic task improved in symbolic arithmetic performance, but not vocabulary performance. As expected, the no-contact control group did not improve on either measure.

In a second experiment, Park & Brannon (2013) examined the efficacy of approximate arithmetic training in comparison to two different active control groups. One group was trained daily with general world knowledge trivia questions and a second control group was required to make rapid judgments about the order of Arabic numerals. The numerical order judgment condition is particularly interesting in light of evidence suggesting that the ability to access the ordinal relation of numbers could mediate the relationship between the ANS and symbolic math (Lyons & Beilock, 2011). In the numerical order judgment training participants were presented with triads of single digit numbers that moved across the screen rapidly in both horizontal directions. The participant’s task was to mouse-click on the triad of digits until all triads moving to the right were in ascending order and all triads moving to the left were in descending order. Although there was clear evidence that the numeral ordering group improved on a post-test assessment of numeral ordering, the group showed no improvement on the timed symbolic arithmetic task. Participants in the approximate arithmetic condition replicated the result from Experiment 1 by demonstrating significant improvement on the symbolic arithmetic test. As expected, the participants in the knowledge training condition did not improve on either measure. The lack of a transfer effect in the symbol-ordering condition argues against the idea that the approximate arithmetic group improved due to a placebo effect (e.g., Dillon, Pires, Hyde, & Spelke, 2015). If participants expected any numerical task to improve their math skills they should have benefited as much or more from numerical ordering.

In a subsequent set of studies Park & Brannon (2014) sought to isolate the components of the approximate arithmetic task that improved math performance. At minimum, approximate arithmetic involves forming representations of approximate numerical magnitudes, holding magnitudes in short-term memory, and combining approximate magnitudes. Would training on only one of these components of approximate arithmetic be sufficient to induce an improvement in symbolic arithmetic? In this study a new cohort of participants trained on approximate arithmetic, a traditional non-symbolic comparison task, a visuo-spatial short-term memory task, or a numeral symbol-ordering task. Results showed that participants who trained in the approximate arithmetic condition showed significantly greater improvement on the symbolic arithmetic test compared to participants in all other training conditions. Training on visuo-spatial short term memory alone was not enough to induce improvement in symbolic arithmetic. Collectively, these training studies suggest that there is a causal relationship between non-symbolic approximate arithmetic and symbolic arithmetic. More research is needed to fully understand the nature of this transfer effect.

Hyde, Khanum, & Spelke (2014) asked a similar question in children. They gave 6-year-old children a single short session of practice on approximate arithmetic, approximate line length addition, non-symbolic numerical comparison, or approximate brightness comparison, and then tested the same children on a timed symbolic arithmetic test part way through training and immediately after training. Children in both the approximate arithmetic and numerical comparison conditions were faster at completing the symbolic arithmetic test than children in the other two conditions, suggesting increased arithmetic fluency after completing tasks that activated the ANS. Although generally consistent with the Park and Brannon findings, children in the numerosity comparison condition appeared to benefit in symbolic arithmetic, while the same condition had little effect in adults. An interesting possibility for future research is to explore whether this is a true developmental or skill-based difference. In adults with strong symbolic math skills, ANS training might induce improvement in symbolic math ability only when there is a manipulation of mental magnitudes, rather than a simple comparison. It is possible that manipulating arrays of dots for adults is simply a more challenging ANS task that leads to greater motivation to train, whereas both non-symbolic comparison and arithmetic were engaging for young children. Approximate arithmetic training likely utilizes other cognitive abilities to a greater degree than approximate comparison, though as described in Park & Brannon (2014), visuo-spatial short term memory training alone was not sufficient to induce improvements in symbolic arithmetic. Future work will need to uncover a mechanistic explanation for the approximate arithmetic training effect that includes possible developmental differences in the efficacy of approximate comparison training. We return to possible mechanisms of the relationship between the ANS and arithmetic later in this review.

To further test if approximate arithmetic training improves the early math skills of preschoolers using a pre/posttest design, Park and colleagues trained preschool aged children on an approximate arithmetic or a memory iPad game, and tested their early symbolic math abilities (Park, Bermudez, Roberts, & Brannon, 2016). Children in the approximate arithmetic condition showed significantly greater improvements on the Test of Early Mathematical Achievement (TEMA-3) compared to children in the memory game condition. Future work is exploring how approximate arithmetic training stacks up against commercial applications aimed at teaching numerical symbols (Szkudlarek & Brannon, in prep).

Challenges in describing the relationship between the ANS and symbolic math

As reviewed above, while many studies have reported a correlation between ANS acuity and math performance, a substantial number of studies have failed to find a relationship (e.g., Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Iuculano et al., 2008; Lyons et al., 2014; Nosworthy et al., 2013; Sasanguie et al., 2014). Three meta-analyses found support for a modest but significant positive relation between ANS and math ability (r = .2 to .4; Chen & Li, 2014; Fazio et al., 2014; Schneider et al., 2016). Fazio et al. (2014) analyzed 19 studies, and found that the relationship between the ANS and math decreased in strength after the onset of formal schooling, though it was still maintained. Chen & Li (2014) analyzed 36 cross sectional studies and found that a positive relationship held even after controlling for general cognitive abilities. ANS acuity also predicted later symbolic math performance, and ANS acuity at a later age was correlated with early symbolic math performance. Schneider et al. (2016) included 45 papers in their analysis, and found evidence of a link between ANS acuity and a wide range of math competencies across all ages. Given the modest effect size described in these meta-analyses, a potentially widespread reason for conflicting findings in the literature is that many studies examining the link between the ANS and math ability are underpowered (Chen & Li, 2014). Nevertheless, these three meta-analyses together lend support for the association between symbolic math and the ANS.

Beyond issues of sample size and power there are additional problematic factors that likely contribute to discrepant findings. One possibility is that the correlation between the ANS and symbolic math is an artifact of the stimulus control protocols employed in ANS tasks. To control for non-numerical stimulus attributes, researchers commonly structure stimuli such that surface area (or sometimes perimeter) is incongruent with numerosity on half of the trials. For example, on a given ordinal comparison trial a stimulus with 10 dots with a total surface area of 100 cm2 might be presented with another stimulus with 20 dots and a total surface area of 50 cm2. It has been suggested that inhibitory control demands created by the nature of these incongruent trials drives the observed relationship between ANS acuity and symbolic math (Fuhs & McNeil, 2013; Gilmore et al., 2013; Negen & Sarnecka, 2015; Soltész, Szucs, & Szucs, 2010). To accurately judge numerosity on incongruent trials, participants may need to inhibit a response based on surface area or another stimulus feature typically confounded with numerosity, and focus on the numerosity of the stimulus despite the incongruity. Thus under this proposal the relationship between inhibitory processes and math is masked as a correlation between ANS and math. While this is an interesting possibility, both congruent and incongruent trials have been found to correlate with symbolic math in a number of data sets (e.g., Keller & Libertus, 2015; Libertus et al., 2013; Sotp-Calvo et al., 2015).

Many researchers are coming to appreciate that it is not possible to perfectly control for non-numerical attributes of a stimulus array (e.g., DeWind et al., 2015; Leibovich & Ansari, 2016). However, the response to this conundrum has differed. Leibovich & Ansari (2016) suggest that the impossibility of controlling for all stimulus attributes at once is problematic for any attempt to measure ANS acuity. They argue that any observed relationship between ANS acuity and symbolic math cannot be dissociated from potential relationships between other magnitudes and symbolic math abilities. In contrast, DeWind and colleagues (2015) suggest that mathematical modeling can disentangle the contributions of different stimulus attributes (see Figure 2). In their model, all features of the stimulus, both numerical and non-numerical (i.e. total surface area, total perimeter, density…etc.), are expressed as linear combinations of the spacing, size, and numerosity of a dot stimulus. Accuracy on a standard numerical comparison task is analyzed as a function of not only the numerical ratio between the dot arrays that are compared, but also as a function of the size ratio and the spacing ratio. Using this model Starr, DeWind, and Brannon (in prep) found that young children’s numerical acuity, but not their size or spacing acuity, was correlated with symbolic math measures. It will be important to apply this modeling technique to data with animals and human infants.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

A stimulus space created by plotting the log of size, numerosity, and spacing of dot stimuli along the x, y, and z axes. Any dot stimulus can be represented in this space as a linear combination of the values along these cardinal axes. Figure from Park, DeWind, Woldorff, & Brannon, 2015.

Another problem for assessing the relationship between ANS acuity and symbolic math is the low reliability of ANS measures. Low ANS reliability has in fact led some researchers to challenge the enterprise of correlating ANS acuity with math ability (Clayton, Gilmore, & Inglis, 2015; Smets, Sasanguie, Szücs, & Reynvoet, 2015). When participants were given the standard numerosity comparison task used to measure ANS acuity with two different stimulus sets that control for non-numerical attributes in different ways, performance was not significantly correlated for the two stimulus sets (Clayton et al., 2015; Smets et al., 2015). However, DeWind & Brannon (2016) re-analyzed the data collected by Clayton et al. (2015) with the model described above, and found that ANS acuity was actually correlated for the two stimulus sets, although the correlation coefficients were not very high. These findings collectively call for a deeper investigation of how stimulus variables (e.g., range of numerosities, range of surface area, and density) influence ANS acuity estimates, and how the choice of stimulus controls influences correlations between the ANS and symbolic math. A gold standard for the assessment of the non-numerical features of a dot stimulus will lead to greater consistency in the literature, and may help resolve whether ANS acuity and symbolic math are fundamentally linked.

Another possible source of conflicting findings is the possibility that the relationship may change over development, or with level of symbolic math skill (Castronovo & Göbel, 2012; Halberda et al., 2012). Indeed, in a number of studies the relationship between ANS acuity and symbolic math is much stronger in children than adults (Fazio et al., 2014; Inglis, Attridge, Batchelor, & Gilmore, 2011). It is also possible that ANS acuity change maps onto periods of rapid symbolic math improvement, such as during late infancy or during the preschool years. For example, Shusterman et al. (2016) found that change in ANS acuity coincided with improved number word knowledge. A greater understanding of the trajectory of ANS acuity change over the course of development could clarify this point. There is also evidence that the relation between ANS acuity and symbolic math may differ as a function of math skill during early schooling. In a longitudinal study, ANS acuity predicted symbolic math ability only for children in the 25th percentile of the distribution of math scores, whereas math language ability predicted math scores of the 50th–75% percentile of this distribution (Purpura & Logan, 2015). Consistent with that finding, Bonny & Lourenco (2013) tested 3–5 year olds on a dot comparison task and the TEMA-3, a standardized symbolic math test. The authors found that the correlation between performance on these two measures varied with level of symbolic math ability. In children with low math scores there was a stronger correlation between their performance on the dot comparison task and the TEMA-3 than for children who had a high TEMA-3 score. Interactions between the level of math knowledge a child brings to the experiment and ANS acuity could also be a cause of the some of the differential findings in the literature.

Discrepant findings may also result from the diversity in measurements used to operationally define symbolic math. It is unlikely that all types of symbolic math skills are correlated with ANS acuity. When researchers refer to math, they can be referring to any number of math problem types, and usually these types are presented in combination on a standardized test. On the Test of Early Mathematics -3, a standardized symbolic math test used with young children, ANS acuity is correlated with informal, but not formal mathematics questions (Libertus et al., 2013). Formal math questions involve reading and writing numerals, while informal questions included counting the number of objects on a page, or symbolic number comparison. Using a longitudinal design, Toll, Van Viersen, Kroesbergen, & Van Luit (2015) found that growth in non-symbolic comparison ability was a predictor for math fluency of arithmetic facts in first grade, suggesting ANS ability is foundational for early arithmetic. However, the authors found symbolic comparison skills to be the best predictor of other basic math skills, such as math word problems. These findings highlight the complexity of the interaction between symbolic and non-symbolic math skills during the beginning of formal math training.

To complicate matters further, a correlation between complex math skills and ANS acuity in adults points to the possibility that the ANS continues to support formal math skills as a child progresses through math education. This would indicate that the specific math skills linked to the ANS change over time. In college students ANS acuity is correlated with the mathematical section of the SAT, a college entrance exam that includes many formal math abilities. This result holds even when controlling for performance on the verbal portion of the SAT (Halberda et al., 2012; Libertus et al., 2012). Geary and colleagues (2015) found that ANS acuity is positively correlated with specific algebra skills in 9th grade students. ANS acuity was a significant predictor for accuracy of placing a point in the coordinate plane and for accuracy in evaluating simple algebraic expressions, but not for remembering algebra equations. Similarly to the findings with algebra, Lourenco et al. (2012) examined how approximate magnitude representations could be related to specific forms of more advanced math, namely complex calculation and geometry. The authors found that ANS precision was correlated with complex calculation, while performance discriminating the total area of an array of dots was correlated with geometry scores. This result implicates a more complex and longstanding relationship between discrete and continuous magnitude representations and math into adulthood. Often, math skills as a general construct can be too broad of a measure to determine the nature of the relationship between the ANS and symbolic math abilities. By dissecting math skills into different components researchers may be able to ascertain which skills are influenced by ANS acuity.

Mechanisms of the ANS and Symbolic Math Relationship

As the field determines the extent of the relationship between the ANS and symbolic math, we must simultaneously move beyond establishing a correlation to understanding the reason why such a correlation might exist. How is the ability to make fuzzy, inexact numerical judgments related to our ability to calculate the orbital trajectories of planets, or more simply, to learn the meaning of “2”? Does ANS acuity influence only specific aspects of symbolic math performance, such as the understanding of ordinality? Does ANS acuity facilitate the acquisition of early math only as a child, or have input throughout the lifespan? Does ANS acuity influence more complex aspects of symbolic math beyond addition and subtraction?

One prominent mechanistic hypothesis is that sharper ANS acuity facilitates the acquisition of early number word representations in young childhood through understanding of the cardinal principle. The cardinal principle is a major milestone in children’s acquisition of the verbal counting system, and specifies that the number of objects in a set refers to the last number produced when counting the objects in that set. In two recent studies with preschool children the relationship between ANS acuity and early symbolic math achievement was fully mediated by a child’s understanding of the cardinal principle (Chu et al., 2015; van Marle et al., 2014). Relatedly, a longitudinal study by Shusterman et al. (2016) found that improved ANS acuity coincided with the understanding of the cardinal principle. Thus increased ANS acuity may facilitate acquisition of the cardinal principle.

The idea that ANS acuity impacts learning of the cardinal principal is at odds with the proposal that children only come to map approximate number representations onto number words after learning the cardinal principle (Carey, 2009; Le Corre & Carey, 2007). However, there is growing evidence that number words are mapped onto ANS representations before children have fully grasped the cardinal principle (Huang, Spelke, & Snedeker, 2010; Odic, Le Corre, & Halberda, 2015; Pinhas, Donohue, Woldorff, & Brannon, 2014; Wagner & Johnson, 2011). Pinhas and colleagues recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) as 3- to 5-year-old children heard the words one, two, three or six and simultaneously looked at pictures of 1, 2, 3 or 6 objects (See Figure 3). On half the trials the auditory number word was incongruent with the number of visual objects, and congruent on the other half. Children with the least number-word knowledge did not show any ERP incongruency effects, whereas those with intermediate and high number-word knowledge showed an enhanced, negative-polarity incongruency response (Ninc) over centro-parietal sites from 200–500 ms after the number-word onset. This negativity was followed by an enhanced, positive- polarity incongruency effect (Pinc) that emerged bilaterally over parietal sites at about 700 ms. Moreover, children with the most number-word knowledge showed a numerical distance effect in the Pinc (larger for greater compared to smaller numerical mismatches). Thus they showed a larger Pinc when the numerical disparity between the auditory number word and the visual object array was greater. Critically, a similar modulation of the Pinc from 700–800 ms was found in children with intermediate number-word knowledge. These results suggest that children map number words onto ANS representations before they fully master the verbal count list.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

A) Example stimuli from Pinhas et al. (2014). B) Differential effects on the Ninc by knower level. C) Differential effects on the Pinc by knower level. Figure modified from Pinhas et al. (2014).

The ERP findings are consistent with behavioral studies that suggest children map number words to ANS magnitude representations before learning the cardinal principle (e.g., Odic et al., 2015). Thus ANS representations may play some role in children’s acquisition of the verbal counting system. However, if facilitating number word acquisition were the only function ANS acuity played in symbolic math ability then we might expect the relationship to completely dissipate by late childhood once number words are automatized. While the relationship may get weaker over time, a positive correlation between the ANS and symbolic math skills has been demonstrated in multiple adult samples (e.g., Halberda et al., 2012; Dewind & Brannon 2012).

Another proposed mechanistic explanation for the relationship between ANS acuity and complex calculation is that the ANS could function as an online form of error detection (Feigenson, Libertus, & Halberda, 2013; Lourenco et al., 2012). As adults or children perform calculations underlying magnitude representations of number could provide an estimate of the correct solution to the problem, allowing the correct rejection of grossly inaccurate results. Adults and children with a more accurate ANS would be able to more adeptly reject incorrect solutions, and therefore would have higher accuracy when solving calculation problems. Similarly, online error detection should be tested as a potential explanation for the mechanism by which approximate arithmetic training enhances symbolic arithmetic in training studies (Park and Brannon, 2013; 2014). This proposal merits further research to establish experimental support.

While there is building evidence that approximate arithmetic training enhances symbolic arithmetic skill, it is less clear whether numerosity comparison training has a similar influence. Park and Brannon (2014) found no evidence for improved symbolic arithmetic after numerosity comparison training with adults, however, Hyde and colleagues (2013) did find evidence that brief exposure to numerosity comparison enhanced arithmetic fluency in young children. If in fact approximate arithmetic training is superior to numerosity comparison for enhancing symbolic arithmetic, a possible mechanism for the transfer is the functional isomorphism between approximate non-symbolic arithmetic and symbolic arithmetic. Young children are capable of solving non-symbolic approximate addition, subtraction, multiplication, division, and even addend-unknown algebra problems well before they learn how to perform these operations with Arabic numerals (Barth et al., 2006; Barth, Baron, Spelke, & Carey, 2009; Barth et al., 2005; McCrink & Spelke, 2016; McCrink & Wynn, 2004; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015). When a mathematical operation is performed using only non-symbolic quantities, and without symbols, the meaning of the operation may become more explicit. For example, when solving a non-symbolic approximate arithmetic problem, two arrays are hidden in the same location. Repeated enactment of the operation may thus facilitate a conceptual understanding of addition as the combining of two quantities. Supporting this idea, Pinheiro-Chagas et al. (2014) found that performance on an approximate arithmetic task fully mediated the effects of non-symbolic comparison on a test of symbolic addition, subtraction and multiplication. Thus training approximate arithmetic may allow children, and even adults, to anchor arithmetic and algebra operations conceptually.

There are a host of alternative domain general explanations for both the correlation between ANS acuity and symbolic mathematics and the approximate arithmetic training effect. As Park and Brannon (2013, 2014) discuss, there are notable working memory demands in the approximate arithmetic training task. Although they attempted to control for the possibility that approximate arithmetic benefits symbolic arithmetic by including a working memory control training task and a short-term memory pre-post-test, this possibility remains important given that working memory is known to be an important factor in math performance (e.g. Geary & Brown, 1991). Thus, future work should continue to explore the possibility that approximate arithmetic facilitates symbolic mathematics via domain general mechanisms such as working memory.

Finally, brain-imaging techniques may provide another avenue into understanding the mechanisms that drive the relationship between ANS acuity and symbolic math. Neuroimaging data analysis techniques such as Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis (MVPA) or Representational Similarity Analysis allow for a nuanced look at how the brain represents number, both symbolically and non-symbolically. If ANS and symbolic number tasks recruit overlapping brain regions, then training on one may result in neural changes that benefit both. Future studies should test this hypothesis by combining cognitive training paradigms with functional brain imaging methods to uncover how the brain changes in response to ANS training (Bugden, DeWind, & Brannon, 2016).

Conclusion and Future Directions

In this paper, we first reviewed properties of the ANS through research that examined the numerical abilities of human infants and non-human primates. We then discussed five lines of evidence that suggest there is a meaningful relation between ANS acuity and symbolic math abilities, and highlighted potential reasons for conflicting findings on the nature of this relationship. Finally, we explored mechanistic explanations for the relation between ANS ability and symbolic math skills. As a field we are only beginning to develop clear testable hypotheses about the mechanisms by which ANS representations could facilitate symbolic numerical operations, but some early ideas have emerged including ANS acuity influencing the acquisition of number words, ANS acuity grounding symbolic arithmetic by facilitating error detection during arithmetic operations, approximate arithmetic working to prime a conceptual understanding of arithmetic, and the possibility that shared neural resources allow training on non-symbolic arithmetic to enhance symbolic arithmetic performance. Many outstanding questions remain.

An important caveat is that even if ANS acuity accounts for unique variance in symbolic math, it is likely that other variables account for equal or greater variance. Parental input (Berkowitz et al., 2015; Casey, Dearing, Dulaney, Heyman, & Springer, 2014; Maloney, Ramirez, Gunderson, Levine, & Beilock, 2015; Ramani, Rowe, Eason, & Leech, 2015) teacher quality (Beilock, Gunderson, Ramirez, & Levine, 2010; Klibanoff, Levine, Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, & Hedges, 2006), and quantity of math-talk (Gunderson & Levine, 2011; Levine, Suriyakham, Rowe, Huttenlocher, & Gunderson, 2010) have all been shown to influence math readiness in early school years and likely account for a larger percentage of the variance in math performance compared to ANS acuity. Additionally, early symbolic math skills are important predictors of later math success. For example, in a large scale cross sectional study (N = 1391) elementary school students in grades 1–6 were tested on a variety of symbolic and non-symbolic math tasks, and performance on these tasks was used to predict arithmetic skills. The best predictor of arithmetic skills changed by grade level, with numeral ordering increasing in predictive power as children aged, while performance on a number line estimation task predicted more variance in grades one and two. Performance on an ANS dot comparison task did not predict unique arithmetic skill variance at any grade level (Lyons et al., 2014). Thus we do not argue that the ANS is the sole or most important contributor to variability in math, but instead we seek to determine whether ANS acuity accounts for any of the variance in symbolic math ability and to define the mechanisms of this relationship.

Finally, there are important educational implications for this work. If the ANS is indeed foundational for math skills, then finding ways to improve ANS acuity could contribute to a suite of tools to improve math skills. This idea is not new and many early math interventions incorporate number sense training into a larger set of tools (Clements & Sarama, 2011; Kuhn & Holling, 2014). As we learn more about how abstract cognitive skills build upon evolutionarily ancient precursor abilities, we hope that ultimately we can harness these foundational skills to benefit mathematical education (Obersteiner, Reiss, & Ufer, 2013; Räsänen, Salminen, Wilson, Aunio, & Dehaene, 2009; Wilson, Revkin, Cohen, Cohen, & Dehaene, 2006). While efforts to increase ANS acuity alone will be insufficient, this foundational skill could be an important component in building a multifaceted approach to improving math readiness in children at risk for poor math outcomes.

Works Cited

  1. Agrillo C, Dadda M, Serena G, Bisazza A. Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition. 2008;11(3):495–503. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0140-9. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0140-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrillo C, Piffer L, Adriano A. Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2013;9(1):26. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-9-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bartelet D, Ansari D, Vaessen A, Blomert L. Cognitive subtypes of mathematics learning difficulties in primary education. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2014;35(3):657–670. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.12.010. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barth H, Baron A, Spelke E, Carey S. Children’s multiplicative transformations of discrete and continuous quantities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103(4):441–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.014. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barth H, La Mont K, Lipton J, Dehaene S, Kanwisher N, Spelke E. Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children. Cognition. 2006;98(3):199–222. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.011. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barth H, La Mont K, Lipton J, Spelke ES. Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(39):14116–14121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505512102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beilock SL, Gunderson EA, Ramirez G, Levine SC. Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(5):1860–1863. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910967107. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910967107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berkowitz T, Schaeffer MW, Maloney EA, Peterson L, Gregor C, Levine SC, Beilock SL. Math at home adds up to achievement in school. Science. 2015;350(6257):196–198. doi: 10.1126/science.aac7427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonny JW, Lourenco SF. The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: Evidence from the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;114(3):375–388. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.015. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brannon EM. The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy. Cognition. 2002;53(3):223–240. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00005-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brannon EM, Terrace HS. Ordering of the Numerosities 1 to 9 by Monkeys. Science. 1998;252(5389):746–749. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5389.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bugden S, Ansari D. Probing the nature of deficits in the “Approximate Number System” in children with persistent Developmental Dyscalculia. Developmental Science. 2015:n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/desc.12324. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12324. [DOI] [PubMed]
  13. Bugden S, DeWind NK, Brannon EM. Using cognitive training studies to unravel the mechanisms by which the approximate number system supports symbolic math ability. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.002. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  14. Bulthé J, De Smedt B, Op de Beeck HP. Format-dependent representations of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses. NeuroImage. 2014;87:311–322. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.049. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D. Dyscalculia: From Brain to Education. Science. 2011;332(6033):1049–1053. doi: 10.1126/science.1201536. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1201536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Butterworth B, Walsh V. Nueral basis of mathematical cognition. Current Biology. 2011;21(16):R617–R618. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cantlon JF, Brannon EM. How much does number matter to a monkey (Macaca mulatta)? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2007;33(1):32–41. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.33.1.32. http://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.33.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cantlon JF, Merritt DJ, Brannon EM. Monkeys display classic signatures of human symbolic arithmetic. Animal Cognition. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0942-5. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0942-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  19. Casey BM, Dearing E, Dulaney A, Heyman M, Springer R. Young girls’ spatial and arithmetic performance: The mediating role of maternal supportive interactions during joint spatial problem solving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2014;29(4):636–648. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.07.005. [Google Scholar]
  20. Castronovo J, Göbel SM. Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(4):e33832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033832. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chen Q, Li J. Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica. 2014;148:163–172. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chu FW, vanMarle K, Geary DC. Early numerical foundations of young children’s mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.006. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed]
  23. Clayton S, Gilmore C, Inglis M. Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: The effect of different visual controls on ANS measurement. Acta Psychologica. 2015;161:177. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Clearfield MW, Mix KS. Number versus contour length in infants’ discrimination of small visual sets. Psychological Science. 1999;10(5):408–411. [Google Scholar]
  25. Clearfield MW, Mix KS. Amount Versus Number: Infants’ Use of Area and Contour Length to Discriminate Small Sets. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2001;2(3):243–260. http://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0203_1. [Google Scholar]
  26. Clements DH, Sarama J. Early childhood mathematics intervention. Science. 2011;333(6045):968–970. doi: 10.1126/science.1204537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cohen-Kadosh R, Bahrami B, Walsh V, Butterworth B, Popescu T, Price CJ. Specialization in the Human Brain: The Case of Numbers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;5 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00062. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cordes S, Brannon EM. The difficulties of representing continuous extent in infancy: Using number is just easier. Child Development. 2008;79(2):476–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01137.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cordes S, Brannon EM. The relative salience of discrete and continuous quantity in young infants. Developmental Science. 2009;12(3):453–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00781.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00781.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Damarla SR, Cherkassky VL, Just MA. Modality-independent representations of small quantities based on brain activation patterns: Modality-Independent Representations of Numbers. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37(4):1296–1307. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23102. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Damarla SR, Just MA. Decoding the representation of numerical values from brain activation patterns: Decoding the Representation of Numerical Values from Brain Activation Patterns. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(10):2624–2634. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22087. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Davis H, Pérusse R. Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1988;11(04):561–579. [Google Scholar]
  33. Defever E, De Smedt B, Reynvoet B. Numerical matching judgments in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2013;34(10):3182–3189. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.018. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Defever E, Reynvoet B, Gebuis T. Task-and age-dependent effects of visual stimulus properties on children’s explicit numerosity judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;116(2):216–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.006. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2003;20(3–6):487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239. http://doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Desoete A, Ceulemans A, De Weerdt F, Pieters S. Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study: Mathematical learning disabilities in kindergarten. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012;82(1):64–81. doi: 10.1348/2044-8279.002002. http://doi.org/10.1348/2044-8279.002002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. DeWind NK, Adams GK, Platt ML, Brannon EM. Modeling the approximate number system to quantify the contribution of visual stimulus features. Cognition. 2015;142:247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.016. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. DeWind NK, Brannon EM. Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00068. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. DeWind NK, Brannon EM. Significant Inter-Test Reliability across Approximate Number System Assessments. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00310. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dillon MR, Pires AC, Hyde DC, Spelke ES. Children’s expectations about training the approximate number system. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2015:n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12118. http://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12118. [DOI] [PubMed]
  41. Drucker CB, Rossa MA, Brannon EM. Comparison of discrete ratios by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) Animal Cognition. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0914-9. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0914-9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  42. Fazio LK, Bailey DH, Thompson CA, Siegler RS. Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2014;123:53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E. Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8(7):307–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Feigenson L, Libertus ME, Halberda J. Links Between the Intuitive Sense of Number and Formal Mathematics Ability. Child Development Perspectives. 2013;7(2):74–79. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12019. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fias W, Menon V, Szucs D. Multiple components of developmental dyscalculia. Trends in Neuroscience and Education. 2013;2(2):43–47. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.006. [Google Scholar]
  46. Fuhs MW, McNeil NM. ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science. 2013;16(1):136–148. doi: 10.1111/desc.12013. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gallistel CR. The organization of learning. The MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  48. Garland A, Low J, Burns KC. Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes) Animal Cognition. 2012;15(6):1129–1140. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0537-3. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0537-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Geary DC. Mathematical disabilities: Reflections on cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Learning and Individual Differences. 2010;20(2):130–133. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.008. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Geary DC, Brown SC. Cognitive addition: Strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in gifted, normal, and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27(3):398. [Google Scholar]
  51. Geary DC, Hoard MK, Nugent L, Rouder JN. Individual differences in algebraic cognition: Relation to the approximate number and semantic memory systems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;140:211–227. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.010. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gilmore C, Attridge N, Clayton S, Cragg L, Johnson S, Marlow N, Inglis M. Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(6):e67374. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067374. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gilmore CK, McCarthy SE, Spelke ES. Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling. Cognition. 2010;115(3):394–406. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gunderson EA, Levine SC. Some types of parent number talk count more than others: relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge: Types of parent number talk. Developmental Science. 2011;14(5):1021–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Halberda J, Feigenson L. Developmental change in the acuity of the “number sense”: The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(5):1457–1465. doi: 10.1037/a0012682. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0012682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Halberda J, Ly R, Wilmer JB, Naiman DQ, Germine L. Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(28):11116–11120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200196109. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200196109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Halberda J, Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L. Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature Letters. 2008;455(2) doi: 10.1038/nature07246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Holloway ID, Ansari D. Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103(1):17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Holloway ID, Price GR, Ansari D. Common and segregated neural pathways for the processing of symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude: An fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2010;49(1):1006–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.071. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Huang YT, Spelke E, Snedeker J. When Is Four Far More Than Three?: Children’s Generalization of Newly Acquired Number Words. Psychological Science. 2010;21(4):600–606. doi: 10.1177/0956797610363552. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hyde DC, Khanum S, Spelke ES. Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children. Cognition. 2014;131(1):92–107. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.007. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hyde DC, Spelke ES. Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition: Neural signatures of number in infants. Developmental Science. 2011;14(2):360–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00987.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00987.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Inglis M, Attridge N, Batchelor S, Gilmore C. Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2011;18(6):1222–1229. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Iuculano T, Tang J, Hall CWB, Butterworth B. Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. Developmental Science. 2008;11(5):669–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00716.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Izard V, Sann C, Spelke ES, Streri A. Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(25):10382–10385. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812142106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Keller L, Libertus M. Inhibitory control may not explain the link between approximation and math abilities in kindergarteners from middle class families. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00685. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Klibanoff RS, Levine SC, Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Hedges LV. Preschool children’s mathematical knowledge: The effect of teacher “math talk”. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(1):59–69. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.59. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kuhn JT, Holling H. Number sense or working memory? The effect of two computer-based trainings on mathematical skills in elementary school. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2014;10(2):59–67. doi: 10.5709/acp-0157-2. http://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0157-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Landerl K, Bevan A, Butterworth B. Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students. Cognition. 2004;93(2):99–125. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.004. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Le Corre M, Carey S. One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition. 2007;105(2):395–438. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Leibovich T, Ansari D. The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence, and outstanding questions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale. 2016;70(1):12–23. doi: 10.1037/cep0000070. http://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Levine SC, Suriyakham LW, Rowe ML, Huttenlocher J, Gunderson EA. What counts in the development of young children’s number knowledge? Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(5):1309–1319. doi: 10.1037/a0019671. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Behavioral and Neural Basis of Number Sense in Infancy: Number Sense in Infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18(6):346–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01665.x. http://doi.Org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01665.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy: Number discrimination in infants. Developmental Science. 2010;13(6):900–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00948.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00948.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Libertus ME, Feigenson L, Halberda J. Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability: Approximate number system and math abilities. Developmental Science. 2011;14(6):1292–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Libertus ME, Feigenson L, Halberda J. Numerical approximation abilities correlate with and predict informal but not formal mathematics abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;116(4):829–838. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Libertus ME, Odic D, Halberda J. Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. Acta Psychologica. 2012;141(3):373–379. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.009. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Libertus ME, Starr A, Brannon EM. Number trumps area for 7-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50(1):108–112. doi: 10.1037/a0032986. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0032986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lipton JS, Spelke ES. Origins of number sense large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science. 2003;14(5):396–401. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lourenco SF, Bonny JW, Fernandez EP, Rao S. Non-symbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(46):18737–18742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207212109. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207212109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lussier CA, Cantlon JF. Developmental bias for number words in the intraparietal sulcus. Developmental Science. 2016:n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/desc.12385. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  82. Lyons IM, Beilock SL. Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence. Cognition. 2011;121(2):256–261. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lyons IM, Price GR, Vaessen A, Blomert L, Ansari D. Numerical predictors of arithmetic success in grades 1–6. Developmental Science. 2014;17(5):714–726. doi: 10.1111/desc.12152. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Maloney EA, Ramirez G, Gunderson EA, Levine SC, Beilock SL. Intergenerational Effects of Parents’ Math Anxiety on Children’s Math Achievement and Anxiety. Psychological Science. 2015 doi: 10.1177/0956797615592630. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615592630. [DOI] [PubMed]
  85. Matejko AA, Ansari D. Trajectories of Symbolic and Non-symbolic Magnitude Processing in the First Year of Formal Schooling. PLOS ONE. 2016;11(3):e0149863. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149863. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L, Halberda J. Impaired Acuity of the Approximate Number System Underlies Mathematical Learning Disability (Dyscalculia): Impaired Numerical Acuity Contributes to MLD. Child Development. 2011a;82(4):1224–1237. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L, Halberda J. Preschoolers’ Precision of the Approximate Number System Predicts Later School Mathematics Performance. PLoS ONE. 2011b;6(9):e23749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023749. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. McCrink K, Spelke ES. Non-symbolic division in childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2016;142:66–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.015. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. McCrink K, Wynn K. Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 2004;15(11):776–781. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. McCrink K, Wynn K. Ratio abstraction by 6-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 2007;18(8):740–745. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01969.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. McCrink K, Wynn K. Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103(4):400–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.013. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Mehlis M, Thünken T, Bakker TCM, Frommen JG. Quantification acuity in spontaneous shoaling decisions of three-spined sticklebacks. Animal Cognition. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0884-y. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0884-y. [DOI] [PubMed]
  93. Mix KS, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: Is number one of them? Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(2):278–294. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.278. http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.2.278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mundy E, Gilmore CK. Children’s mapping between symbolic and non-symbolic representations of number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103(4):490–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Mussolin C, Nys J, Content A, Leybaert J. Symbolic Number Abilities Predict Later Approximate Number System Acuity in Preschool Children. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(3):e91839. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091839. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mussolin C, Nys J, Leybaert J, Content A. How approximate and exact number skills are related to each other across development: A review☆. Developmental Review. 2015 http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.11.001.
  97. Negen J, Sarnecka BW. Is there really a link between exact-number knowledge and approximate number system acuity in young children? British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2015;33(1):92–105. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12071. http://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Noël M-P, Rousselle L. Developmental Changes in the Profiles of Dyscalculia: An Explanation Based on a Double Exact-and-Approximate Number Representation Model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;5 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00165. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Nosworthy N, Bugden S, Archibald L, Evans B, Ansari D. A Two-Minute Paper-and-Pencil Test of Symbolic and Non-symbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Explains Variability in Primary School Children’s Arithmetic Competence. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(7):e67918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067918. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Obersteiner A, Reiss K, Ufer S. How training on exact or approximate mental representations of number can enhance first-grade students’ basic number processing and arithmetic skills. Learning and Instruction. 2013;23:125–135. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.08.004. [Google Scholar]
  101. Odic D, Le Corre M, Halberda J. Children’s mappings between number words and the approximate number system. Cognition. 2015;138:102–121. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.008. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Park J, Bermudez V, Roberts RC, Brannon EM. Non-symbolic approximate arithmetic training improves math performance in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2016;152:278–293. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.011. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Park J, Brannon EM. Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency. Psychological Science. 2013;24(10):2013–2019. doi: 10.1177/0956797613482944. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613482944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Park J, Brannon EM. Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: An investigation of underlying mechanism. Cognition. 2014;133(1):188–200. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.011. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Park J, DeWind NK, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM. Rapid and Direct Encoding of Numerosity in the Visual Stream. Cerebral Cortex. 2015:bhv017. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv017. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  106. Perdue BM, Talbot CF, Stone AM, Beran MJ. Putting the elephant back in the herd: elephant relative quantity judgments match those of other species. Animal Cognition. 2012;15(5):955–961. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0521-y. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0521-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Piazza M, Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Berteletti I, Conte S, Lucangeli D, Zorzi M. Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia. Cognition. 2010;116(1):33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.012. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Piazza M, Pica P, Izard V, Spelke ES, Dehaene S. Education Enhances the Acuity of the Nonverbal Approximate Number System. Psychological Science. 2013;24(6):1037–1043. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464057. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Piazza M, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex. Neuron. 2007;53(2):293–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Pinhas M, Donohue SE, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM. Electrophysiological Evidence for the Involvement of the Approximate Number System in Preschoolers’ Processing of Spoken Number Words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;26(9):1891–1904. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00631. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Pinheiro-Chagas P, Wood G, Knops A, Krinzinger H, Lonnemann J, Starling-Alves I, Haase VG. In How Many Ways is the Approximate Number System Associated with Exact Calculation? PLoS ONE. 2014;9(11):e111155. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111155. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Purpura DJ, Logan JAR. The nonlinear relations of the approximate number system and mathematical language to early mathematics development. Developmental Psychology. 2015;57(12):1717–1724. doi: 10.1037/dev0000055. http://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Ramani GB, Rowe ML, Eason SH, Leech KA. Math talk during informal learning activities in Head Start families. Cognitive Development. 2015;35:15–33. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.11.002. [Google Scholar]
  114. Räsänen P, Salminen J, Wilson AJ, Aunio P, Dehaene S. Computer-assisted intervention for children with low numeracy skills. Cognitive Development. 2009;24(4):450–472. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.09.003. [Google Scholar]
  115. Rousselle L, Noël MP. Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition. 2007;102(3):361–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Sasanguie D, Defever E, Maertens B, Reynvoet B. The approximate number system is not predictive for symbolic number processing in kindergarteners. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2014;67(2):271–280. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.803581. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.803581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Schneider M, Beeres K, Coban L, Merz S, Susan Schmidt S, Stricker J, De Smedt B. Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Developmental Science. 2016:n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/desc.12372. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12372. [DOI] [PubMed]
  118. Shusterman A, Slusser E, Halberda J, Odic D. Acquisition of the Cardinal Principle Coincides with Improvement in Approximate Number System Acuity in Preschoolers. PLOS ONE. 2016;11(4):e0153072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153072. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Skagerlund K, Träff U. Processing of space, time, and number contributes to mathematical abilities above and beyond domain-general cognitive abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2016;143:85–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.016. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Smets K, Sasanguie D, Szücs D, Reynvoet B. The effect of different methods to construct non-symbolic stimuli in numerosity estimation and comparison. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2015;27(3):310–325. http://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.996568. [Google Scholar]
  121. Soltész F, Szucs D, Szucs L. Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4-to 7-year-old children: A developmental study. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2010;6(13):1–14. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Soto-Calvo E, Simmons FR, Willis C, Adams AM. Identifying the cognitive predictors of early counting and calculation skills: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;140:16–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.011. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Starr A, Brannon EM. Evidence against continuous variables driving numerical discrimination in infancy. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00923. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Starr A, DeWind NK, Brannon EM. The role of non-numerical stimulus features in the development of the number sense. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.07.004. (in prep) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Starr A, Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(45):18116–18120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302751110. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1302751110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Toll SW, Van Viersen S, Kroesbergen EH, Van Luit JE. The development of (non-) symbolic comparison skills throughout kindergarten and their relations with basic mathematical skills. Learning and Individual Differences. 2015 Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1041608015000023.
  127. van Marle K, Chu FW, Li Y, Geary DC. Acuity of the approximate number system and preschoolers’ quantitative development. Developmental Science. 2014;17(4):492–505. doi: 10.1111/desc.12143. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Wagner JB, Johnson SC. An association between understanding cardinality and analog magnitude representations in preschoolers. Cognition. 2011;119(1):10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.014. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Wilson AJ, Revkin SK, Cohen D, Cohen L, Dehaene S. An open trial assessment of “The Number Race”, an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2006;2(20) doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Wynn K. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature. 1992;358:749–750. doi: 10.1038/358749a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Xu F, Spelke ES. Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition. 2000;74(1):B1–B11. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00066-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES