Skip to main content
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health logoLink to International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
. 2017 Mar 9;14(3):286. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030286

Perceived Stigma of Sudden Bereavement as a Risk Factor for Suicidal Thoughts and Suicide Attempt: Analysis of British Cross-Sectional Survey Data on 3387 Young Bereaved Adults

Alexandra Pitman 1,2,*, Khadija Rantell 3, Louise Marston 4, Michael King 1,2, David Osborn 1,2
Editors: Rory O’Connor, Gwendolyn Portzky
PMCID: PMC5369122  PMID: 28282958

Abstract

The sudden death of a friend or relative, particularly by suicide, is a risk factor for suicide. People who experience sudden bereavement report feeling highly stigmatised by the loss, potentially influencing access to support. We assessed whether perceived stigma following sudden bereavement is associated with suicidal thoughts and suicide attempt. We analysed cross-sectional survey data on 3387 young adults bereaved by the sudden death of a close contact. We tested the association of high versus low perceived stigma (on the stigma sub-scale of the Grief Experience Questionnaire) with post-bereavement suicidal ideation and suicide attempt, using random effects logistic regression, adjusting for socio-demographic factors, pre-bereavement psychopathology, and mode of sudden bereavement (natural causes/unnatural causes/suicide). Subjects with high perceived stigma scores were significantly more likely to report post-bereavement suicidal thoughts (adjusted odds ratio (AOR) = 2.74; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.93–3.89) and suicide attempt (AOR = 2.73; 95% CI = 2.33–3.18) than those with low stigma scores. People who feel highly stigmatised by a sudden bereavement are at increased risk of suicidal thoughts and suicide attempt, even taking into account prior suicidal behaviour. General practitioners, bereavement counsellors, and others who support people bereaved suddenly, should consider inquiring about perceived stigma, mental wellbeing, and suicidal thoughts, and directing them to appropriate sources of support.

Keywords: suicide, self-harm, bereavement, stigma, depression, support, risk factor

1. Introduction

The search for modifiable risk factors for suicide underpins the suicide prevention research agenda. Sudden bereavement, particularly by suicide [1], is now recognised as a robust risk factor for suicide [2], but explanations for this are unclear. Studies controlling for mental illness indicate that neither heritability [2,3] nor assortative mating [4] completely account for the observed association. An alternative explanation is perceived stigma; the subjective awareness of others’ negative attitudes [5]. This is a common feature of sudden or violent bereavements and may influence access to support [1]. Stigma is also potentially modifiable [6]. Studies comparing grief reactions after different causes of death reveal that experiences of stigma, shame, and concealing the cause are reported after all modes of bereavement, but particularly after violent deaths [1,7] and specifically suicide [8]. Accounting for high levels of perceived stigma has been found to attenuate the association of suicide bereavement with suicide attempt [9], suggesting its role as a mediator of suicide risk. The implication is that anti-stigma interventions might reduce the risk of suicide attempt in people who experience sudden bereavement, perhaps by reducing distress and/or optimising support.

The means by which stigma creates barriers to help-seeking have been well-described in relation to mental illness [10], but less well in relation to sudden bereavement [7,11,12,13]. In people with mental illness, stigma is hypothesised to contribute to suicidality through factors such as social isolation, hopelessness, and a perception of being a burden [14]. The same might be theorised after sudden bereavement, when avoidance might arise due to embarrassment, or fear of appearing socially incompetent [15]. Feeling stigmatised by a death contributes to a sense of thwarted belongingness and poor social support; both of which may engender suicidal thoughts [16]. Our objective was to investigate whether high levels of perceived stigma after sudden bereavement are associated with suicidal behaviour. To do this, we analysed British cross-sectional survey data on adults who had experienced sudden bereavement. Our hypothesis was that high stigma scores are associated with post-bereavement suicidal behaviour and depression. To build our understanding of mechanisms, we also hypothesised that high stigma scores would be negatively associated with social support, and receipt of formal or informal support. To understand what differentiates those who attempt suicide from those who consider suicide after sudden bereavement, we hypothesised that high stigma scores are associated with suicide attempt in the sub-group of those with suicidal thoughts following bereavement [17]. Finally, we hypothesised that the effect of high stigma scores on primary outcomes would be modified by gender and by mode of bereavement, such that it would be more pronounced in women and in people bereaved by non-suicide causes.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

We analysed data from the UCL Bereavement Study [8,9]. This was a UK-wide cross-sectional survey of young adults aged 18–40 working and/or studying at UK higher education institutions (HEIs) who had experienced the sudden bereavement of a close friend or relative. This study had focused on young adults due to concerns about their risk of suicide [18] and the difficulties of engaging young suicidal men with services [19]. Full details of sampling for this closed online survey have been described elsewhere, including the survey instrument (see Supplementary Materials) [8,9]. Sampling via institution-wide email lists (to all staff and students) avoided the biases associated with recruiting a help-seeking sample, and was felt to be the most efficient, comprehensive and pragmatic means of recruiting a hard-to-reach population of young adults [20]. Of 5085 respondents to the survey, we included those who consented to participate, completed a stigma score, and specified their mode of bereavement (n = 3387).

The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee in 2010 (ref: 1975/002). All participants provided online informed consent.

2.2. Measures

Our exposure measure was high perceived stigma of the bereavement, defined using the 10-item stigma subscale of the Grief Experience Questionnaire (GEQ) [21]. The GEQ is a standardised, self-administered instrument for the assessment of the phenomenology of grief. It was originally developed in the U.S. using qualitative data from individuals bereaved by natural causes, accidental death, and suicide [22], and subsequently validated [21]. The stigma sub-scale includes items describing perceptions of others’ avoidance and lack of concern (see Box 1), capturing perceived rather than personal stigma. Responses to items in each subscale are rated using a 5-point Likert-style frequency scale, generating subscale scores of 5 to 25 (at 0.5 intervals). The majority of studies measuring GEQ scores use GEQ subscales rather than overall GEQ scores, allowing them to delineate specific components of grief [8,23,24,25]. Based on precedent [23] and the normal distribution of stigma scores in this sample, we used the mean to dichotomise stigma scores, classifying them as low (5 to 12) or high (12.5 to 25) to aid clinical interpretation.

Box 1. GEQ stigma subscale items.

Stem: Since the death how often did you….

  1. feel like a social outcast?

  2. feel like no-one cared to listen to you?

  3. feel that neighbours and friends did not offer enough concern?

  4. feel avoided by friends?

  5. think people were gossiping about you or the person?

  6. think that others didn’t want you to talk about the death?

  7. feel somehow stigmatised by the death?

  8. feel like people were probably wondering about what kind of personal problems you and the person had experienced?

  9. think that people were uncomfortable offering their condolences to you?

  10. feel like the death somehow reflected negatively on you or your family?

Our primary outcomes were self-reported suicidal ideation (“Have you ever thought of taking your life, even though you would not actually do it?”) [26] and self-reported suicide attempt (“Have you ever made an attempt to take your life, by taking an overdose of tablets or in some other way?”) [27] post-bereavement. These standardised, validated measures were derived from the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS) [28], a regular national population survey in England, qualified by whether each was before or after the sudden bereavement, or both, to derive an incident measure.

Our three secondary mental health outcomes were post-bereavement non-suicidal self-harm (self-poisoning and self-injury without suicidal intent) using the standardised, validated APMS measure [27] (adapted as above); post-bereavement suicidal and non-suicidal self-harm (aggregating the suicide attempt and non-suicidal self-harm measures, to correspond to that used in a major longitudinal study of self-harm in England [29]); and post-bereavement depression, using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) screen for lifetime depression [30], also validated for use in an online questionnaire [31] (adapted for incident cases as above).

Our three self-reported support measures were level of current social support (using a standardised ordinal measure from the APMS [28]); receipt of any formal bereavement support (using a binary measure developed for this study); and receipt of any informal bereavement support (using a binary measure developed for this study). Classification of formal and informal bereavement support was derived from similar British [32] and international [33] studies of service use . Self-help was excluded due to problematic categorisation in relation to formal versus informal bereavement support [34]. Thus, formal support was defined as that received from healthcare or social services staff; psychological therapists or counsellors; voluntary sector helplines or counsellors; police officers; funeral directors; coroners’ officers; teaching staff; school or HEI counselling services; line managers, or employer counselling services. Informal support was defined as that received from friends; family; spiritual/religious advisors, or complementary and alternative medicine practitioners.

We selected nine confounding variables on the basis of existing literature and clinical judgement: age; gender; socio-economic status (using the UK Office for National Statistics Standard Occupational Classification [35]); mode of sudden bereavement; kinship to the deceased; family history of suicide (excluding an index bereavement by suicide); pre-loss depression; pre-loss suicidal and non-suicidal self-harm; and years since sudden bereavement. Mode of bereavement was classified via self-report as bereavement by suicide, bereavement by sudden natural causes (e.g., cardiac arrest), and bereavement by sudden unnatural causes (e.g., accidental death). In the case of exposures to more than one mode of sudden bereavement, all those bereaved by suicide were classified as such, regardless of other exposures. Those bereaved by non-suicide death were asked to relate their responses to whichever person they had felt closest to, with exposure status classified accordingly.

Missing data for model covariates and outcomes were less than 7%.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

We investigated simple associations between the outcome variables and exposure using χ2 tests or one-way analysis of variance, as appropriate.

We investigated the relationship between outcomes and high stigma scores using multilevel regression models with HEI as random effect, to take into account the clustering effect at the HEI level. We used ordinal logistic regression to investigate the relationship between social support and high levels of perceived stigma scores. All multivariable models included the nine pre-specified confounding variables described above. Models were fitted using complete case analysis. We used the Bonferroni correction to set a significance threshold of p = 0.006 for multiple testing.

To test whether the effect of high stigma scores on primary outcomes varied by gender and by mode of bereavement, we added interaction terms to these models, using a less stringent p-value threshold (p = 0.1) to reflect the limited statistical power of interaction tests.

To test an additional research question about whether high perceived stigma helps differentiate those who attempt suicide after bereavement from those with suicidal ideation after bereavement, we ran our multivariable model for suicide attempt in the sub-sample of those who reported suicidal thoughts or attempts post-bereavement (n = 1510).

We ran a series of a priori defined sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of our main findings when taking into account biases introduced by <7% missing data and by our sampling strategy. In the first and second analyses, we used best-case and worst-case scenarios to impute missing values by recoding all missing values on outcomes/covariates as positive (e.g., no suicidal ideation/attempt) or as negative (e.g., suicidal ideation/attempt) respectively [36]. In the third and fourth, we used more stringent inclusion criteria: dropping the 10 HEIs that modified the stipulated recruitment method, and the 18 HEIs with participant numbers below the median cluster size. Finally, we conducted linear regression to test whether there was a linear association between stigma scores and outcomes.

All analyses were conducted using Stata version 12 (Stata Corp. 2011. Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. College Station, TX, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Participant Characteristics

The majority of the sample were female (81%), of white ethnicity (90%), bereaved by sudden natural causes (61%), and reported the death of a relative (71%). The mean time elapsed since bereavement was 5 years (standard deviation (SD) = 5.3 years; range = 1 day to 30 years), with no group differences (Table 1). The age of the deceased varied from 0 (for miscarriage or stillbirth) to 100 years, and median age was significantly younger for those reporting high (median age = 45; inter-quartile range (IQR) = 22–58) versus low stigma scores (median = 50; IQR = 23–70). The group reporting high stigma scores were more likely to be women, students, those in higher social classes, and those educated to a higher level than the group with low stigma scores. They were also significantly more likely to have been bereaved by suicide, and to have had a history of suicidal or non-suicidal self-harm and of depression prior to the loss.

Table 1.

Characteristics of participants by high versus low perceived stigma scores.

GEQ Stigma Sub-Scale Score Low Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1764) High Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1623) Total (n = 3387) p-Value b
Socio-demographic characteristics
Gender
 Female n (%) 1388 (79) 1360 (84) 2748 (81) <0.001
 missing n (%) 0 (0) 1 (<1) 1 (<1)
Age of participant
 mean (SD) 25.0 (6.2) 25.1 (6.4) 25.0 (6.3) 1.000
Self-defined ethnicity
 white n (%) 1598 (91) 1449 (89) 3047 (90) 0.224
 non-white n (%) 165 (9) 172 (11) 337 (10)
 missing n (%) 1 (<1) 2 (<1) 3 (<1)
Socio-economic status c
 social classes 1.1 and 1.2 n (%) 551 (31) 440 (27) 991 (29) <0.001
 social class 2 n (%) 586 (33) 526 (32) 1112 (33)
 social class 3 n (%) 209 (12) 185 (11) 394 (12)
 social class 4 n (%) 91 (5) 63 (4) 154 (5)
 social classes 5–7 and 9 n (%) 273 (16) 359 (22) 632 (19)
 missing n (%) 54 (3) 50 (3) 104 (3)
Educational status
 attained up to A level equivalent leaving  qualification n (%) 734 (42) 750 (46) 1484 (44) 0.008
 attained undergraduate degree or above n (%) 1025 (58) 871 (54) 1896 (56)
 missing n (%) 5 (<1) 2 (<1) 7 (<1)
Student status
 student n (%) 1472 (83) 1428 (88) 2900 (86) 0.001
 staff n (%) 238 (13) 156 (10) 394 (12)
 both n (%) 54 (3) 38 (2) 92 (3)
 missing n (%) 1 (0) 1 (<1) 1 (<1)
Characteristics of index bereavement
Mode of death
 sudden natural causes n (%) 1184 (67) 892 (55) 2076 (61) <0.001
 sudden unnatural causes n (%) 368 (21) 336 (21) 704 (21)
 suicide n (%) 212 (12) 395 (24) 607 (18)
 missing n (%) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Kinship to the deceased
 blood relative n (%) 1224 (69) 1176 (73) 2400 (71) 0.050
 unrelated n (%) 533 (30) 441 (27) 974 (29)
 missing n (%) 7 (<1) 6 (<1) 13 (<1)
Age of the deceased
 median (IQR) 50 (23–70) 45 (22–58) 47 (23–64) <0.001
Gender of the deceased
 Female n (%) 666 (38) 583 (36) 1249 (37) 0.216
 missing n (%) 54 (3) 41 (3) 95 (3)
Time since bereavement
 mean (SD) 4.6 (5) 5.4 (6) 5 (5.3) 1.000
Clinical characteristics
Family history of suicide (excluding index suicide bereavement)
 Yes n (%) 109 (6) 104 (6) 213 (6) 0.829
 missing n (%) 128 (7) 106 (7) 234 (7)
Family history of psychiatric problems
 Yes n (%) 1008 (57) 1059 (65) 2067 (61) <0.001
 missing n (%) 124 (7) 102 (6) 226 (7)
Personality disorder screen positive d
 Yes n (%) 464 (26) 712 (44) 1176 (35) <0.001
 missing n (%) 105 (6) 84 (5) 189 (6)
Pre-loss depression
 Yes n (%) 280 (16) 353 (22) 633 (19) <0.001
 missing n (%) 74 (4) 54 (3) 128 (4)
Pre-loss non-suicidal self-harm and suicide attempt
 Yes n (%) 325 (18) 380 (23) 705 (21) <0.001
 missing n (%) 127 (7) 104 (6) 231 (7)
Help sought after suicide attempt post-bereavement
 Yes n (%) 15 (1) 54 (3) 69 (2) <0.001
 No n (%) 36 (2) 101 (6) 137 (4)
 No suicide attempt post-bereavement n (%) 1713 (97) 1468 (91) 3181 (94)

SD = standard deviation; IQR = inter-quartile range; a using Grief Experience Questionnaire (GEQ) stigma sub-scale score dichotomised at mean into low (5 to 12) and high (12.5 to 25). † pre-specified covariate entered into adjusted models. b significance threshold of p = 0.05; not adjusted for multiple testing. c socio-economic status using the five categories from UK Office for National Statistics. d SAPAS-SR screen for personality disorder [37].

Amongst subjects who had made a suicide attempt since the bereavement, those who reported high stigma scores were significantly less likely to have sought help for it than those reporting low stigma scores.

Overall, 32% of the sample had received no informal support after the bereavement (Table 2).

Table 2.

Summary of outcomes by low versus high perceived stigma scores.

GEQ Stigma sub-Scale Score Low Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1764) High Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1623) Total (n = 3387) p-Value b
Primary outcomes
Post-loss suicidal thoughts
 Yes n (%) 582 (33) 929 (57) 1511 (45) <0.001
 missing n (%) 121 (7) 100 (6) 221 (7)
Post-loss suicide attempt
 Yes n (%) 51 (3) 155 (10) 206 (6) <0.001
 missing n (%) 126 (7) 100 (6) 226 (7)
Secondary outcomes
Secondary mental health outcomes
Post-loss non-suicidal self-harm
 Yes n (%) 260 (15) 473 (29) 733 (22) <0.001
 missing n (%) 125 (15) 102 (6) 227 (7)
Post-loss non-suicidal self-harm and suicide attempt
 Yes n (%) 280 (16) 519 (32) 799 (24) <0.001
 missing n (%) 121 (7) 98 (6) 219 (7)
Post-loss depression
 Yes n (%) 361 (21) 699 (43) 1060 (4) <0.001
 missing n (%) 74 (4) 54 (3) 128 (4)
Support measures
Measure of social support c
 no lack of perceived social support n (%) 1228 (70) 739 (46) 1967 (58) <0.001
 moderate lack of perceived social support n (%) 406 (23) 499 (31) 905 (27)
 severe lack of perceived social support n (%) 130 (7) 384 (24) 514 (15)
 missing n (%) 0 (0) 1 (<1) 1 (<1)
Receipt of formal support after index bereavement
 Yes n (%) 553 (31) 704 (43) 1257 (37) <0.001
 No n (%) 1135 (64) 861 (53) 1996 (59)
 missing n (%) 76 (4) 58 (4) 134 (4)
Receipt of informal support after index bereavement
 Yes n (%) 1277 (72) 907 (56) 2184 (65) <0.001
 No n (%) 411 (23) 658 (41) 1069 (32)
 missing n (%) 76 (4) 58 (4) 134 (4)

GEQ = Grief Experience Questionnaire, a using GEQ stigma sub-scale score. a using Grief Experience Questionnaire stigma sub-scale score dichotomised at mean into low (5 to 12) and high (12.5 to 25). b significance threshold of p = 0.05; not adjusted for multiple testing. c measure of social support from Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey [28].

3.2. Association between High Stigma Scores and Outcomes

In an adjusted analysis (Table 3), high stigma scores were associated with a significantly higher probability of post-bereavement suicidal thoughts (AOR = 2.74; 95% CI = 1.93–3.89), suicide attempt (AOR = 2.73; 95% CI = 2.33–3.18), non-suicidal self-harm (2.16; 95% CI = 1.76–2.64), any self-harm (AOR = 2.25; 95% CI = 1.85–2.74), depression (AOR = 3.84; 95% CI = 3.21–4.59).

Table 3.

Estimates of the association between high stigma scores and outcomes.

GEQ Stigma Sub-Scale Score Low Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1764) High Perceived Stigma Score a (n = 1623)
Primary Outcomes Odds Ratio (reference) Unadjusted Odds Ratio (95% CI) p Value b Adjusted c Odds Ratio (95% CI) p Value b
post-bereavement suicidal ideation 1 3.45 (2.47–4.81) <0.001 2.74 (1.93–3.89) <0.001
post-bereavement suicide attempt 1 2.84 (2.45–3.89) <0.001 2.73 (2.33–3.18) <0.001
Secondary Measures
post-bereavement non-suicidal self-harm 1 2.40 (2.01–2.86) <0.001 2.16 (1.76–2.64) <0.001
post-bereavement suicidal and non-suicidal self-harm 1 2.50 (2.11–2.96) <0.001 2.25 (1.85–2.74) <0.001
post-bereavement depression 1 2.91 (2.48–3.41) <0.001 3.84 (3.21–4.59) <0.001
low perceived social support 1 2.84 (2.45–3.28) <0.001 2.86 (2.44–3.34) <0.001
use of formal bereavement support 1 1.76 (1.52–2.04) <0.001 1.87 (1.60–2.19) <0.001
use of informal bereavement support 1 0.44 (0.37–0.51) <0.001 0.48 (0.41–0.57) <0.001

GEQ = Grief Experience Questionnaire. a using Grief Experience Questionnaire stigma sub-scale score dichotomised at mean into low (5 to 12) and high (12.5 to 25). b using corrected significance threshold of p = 0.006. c adjusted for nine pre-specified confounding variables: age; gender; socio-economic status; mode of sudden bereavement; kinship to the deceased; family history of suicide (excluding index bereavement); pre-loss depression; pre-loss suicidal and non-suicidal self-harm; and years since index bereavement.

High stigma scores were positively associated with poor social support (AOR = 2.86; 95% CI = 2.44–3.34), and use of formal bereavement support (AOR = 1.87; 95% CI = 1.60–2.19), but negatively associated with use of informal bereavement support (AOR = 0.48; 95% CI = 0.41–0.57).

In the sub-sample of n = 1510 individuals who reported suicidal thoughts or attempts post-bereavement, we found a significant association between high stigma scores and post-bereavement suicide attempt (AOR = 1.90; 95% CI = 1.32–2.72; p = 0.001).

3.3. Interactions

Gender did not modify the associations between stigma and primary outcomes, but there was an interaction with mode of bereavement, such that the magnitude of the association between high stigma and suicidal ideation was higher for those bereaved by sudden natural death (AOR = 3.08; 95% CI = 2.52–3.76) or sudden unnatural death (AOR = 3.02; 95% CI = 2.13–4.28) than for those bereaved by suicide (AOR = 1.61; 95% CI = 1.09–2.39).

3.4. Sensitivity Analysis

The magnitude and direction of adjusted odds ratios for primary outcomes were unchanged in four sensitivity analyses simulating potential biases introduced by missing data and by our sampling strategy. Conducting the analysis using linear regression showed that stigma scores were significantly associated with suicidal ideation (adjusted coefficient = 0.033; 95% CI = 0.029–0.037; p ≤ 0.001) and suicide attempt (adjusted coefficient = 0.009; 95% CI = 0.006–0.107; p ≤ 0.001). On all other measures, it also showed directions of associations consistent with those in our main analysis (p ≤ 0.001 in all cases).

4. Discussion

4.1. Main Findings

The findings of this analysis of British cross-sectional data support our hypothesis that people who feel highly stigmatised by the sudden death of a friend or relative are at increased risk of suicidal thoughts, suicide attempt, non-suicidal self-harm, and depression. The cross-sectional nature of the data limits interpretation of the chronology of the pathways between high stigma scores and outcomes. However, associations with support measures suggested a buffering effect of social support on the negative effects of perceived stigma: those with low perceived stigma scores were more likely to report use of informal support, whereas those with high stigma scores perceived poor social support. Contrary to our hypothesis, use of formal support was more likely in people who felt highly stigmatised, perhaps because they could not rely on friends and family. It is possible that informal support plays an important role in preventing and/or redressing perceived stigma and in mitigating the effects of stigma on suicidality and depression. The results of our interaction tests are interpretable in the context of previous findings from this dataset, namely the increased risk of suicide attempt in people bereaved by suicide [9]. Amongst those bereaved by non-suicide causes, it was those perceiving high levels of stigma who were much more likely to report suicidal thoughts.

4.2. Results in the Context of Other Studies

Previous studies measuring the stigma of sudden bereavement have compared groups defined by cause of death [1,9], but none have explored whether stigma scores per se are associated with adverse outcomes. Instead, qualitative approaches have been used to describe the nature of the stigma experienced by people bereaved traumatically, and how the “death taboo” influences their and others’ avoidance of the topic [11]. Qualitative studies of the stigma perceived by people with mental illness identify the anticipation of negative consequences as a key theme in relation to help-seeking behaviour [10]. The same might apply after bereavement, particularly where the bereaved anticipate social awkwardness [15]. There is some evidence that the stigma attached to help-seeking for mental health problems [10] also applies to bereavement support groups [38], even despite social expectations for the bereaved to engage with support [39] so they can quickly “move on” with their grief [40,41,42].

4.3. Strengths and Limitations

We analysed data from a large, UK-wide sample of 3387 bereaved adults using a validated stigma measure. We tested specific hypotheses formulated on the basis of theory and clinical experience, and our models were adjusted for pre-selected potential confounders. Results were robust to sensitivity analysis simulating potential biases. We acknowledge the potential for male non-response bias, and selection bias of highly educated adults from HEIs. This, and the restricted 18–40 age range, suggest that this study’s findings may only be generalizable to highly-educated young women in the UK. All measures were potentially subject to recall bias. Although validated, our measure of lifetime depression was derived from a brief screening tool [30], and may have over- or under-estimated past depression where used as a potential confounder in multivariable models. Our measures of formal and informal support use were subjective, and represent both preferences and availability. Although the GEQ stigma subscale captures stigmatising aspects of the death specifically, perceived stigma may have been compounded by stigmatising depression or suicidal behaviour. As this was a cross-sectional study, it was not possible to ascertain the temporal sequence of outcomes, including whether suicidal behaviour following bereavement had preceded the awareness of stigma, or of lack of support. However, quantitative [43] and qualitative [44] studies identify perceptions of a lack of support immediately after a sudden death.

4.4. Clinical and Policy Implications

This study has identified perceived stigma after sudden bereavement as a potentially useful marker for suicidality, depression, and for poor social support. Indeed, among those who had felt suicidal following sudden bereavement, perceptions of high stigma helped differentiate attempters from ideators. Identification of people who feel highly stigmatised after bereavement creates an opportunity to intervene and prevent suicide attempt. General practitioners and bereavement counsellors who encounter bereaved people might consider inquiring about perceived stigma as a way of building a rapport before probing, where appropriate, for low mood and thoughts of self-harm and suicide. Anyone in contact with someone bereaved traumatically has a role in providing information on sources of voluntary sector bereavement support [45,46,47]. Given our findings, this is particularly important for those who feel most stigmatised. Specific resources are available for people bereaved by suicide [46,48], recognising their elevated risk of suicide [2,4] and efforts to target this group in suicide prevention strategies [49,50].

4.5. Future Research

The role of stigma as a putative mediator in the association between sudden bereavement and suicide-related outcomes, and the role of informal support as a moderator of this effect, would need formal testing using longitudinal approaches. If stigma is confirmed as mediating risk of suicidality, the next step would be to develop and trial individual-level or community-level anti-stigma interventions. These might address the barriers to seeking or receiving support, and potentially reduce suicide rates. Cultural dimensions of grief [51] suggest that the development of anti-stigma interventions will need to be based on the findings of qualitative studies. These should explore why bereaved people in different communities feel stigmatised, and how this influences help-seeking behaviour and mental health, but also to understand how informal networks perceive their role and what prevents them from offering adequate support.

5. Conclusions

The results of this study suggest that people who feel the most stigmatised by a sudden bereavement are at greater risk of suicidal behaviour and depression, and are more likely to feel inadequately socially supported by friends and family. High perceived stigma helps differentiate those who attempt suicide after sudden bereavement from those who consider it. Clinicians who inquire about perceived stigma are in a position to identify suicidal distress and address support needs. All those who have contact with people bereaved traumatically should direct them to appropriate sources of support, to overcome any barriers to help-seeking and the effects of perceived stigma.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Medical Research Council Population Health Scientist Fellowship to AP (G0802441). The Medical Research Council, as with other UK Research Councils, provides UCL with funds to cover open access for research papers it has funded. We would like to thank all the HEIs from England, Wales, Northern Ireland, and Scotland that consented to participate in the UCL Bereavement Study, listed below, and all the bereaved individuals who took time to respond to the online survey. Participating HEIs: Bishop Grosseteste University College Lincoln; Bournemouth University; Central School of Speech and Drama; City University; Cranfield University; Courtauld Institute; De Montfort University; University of Greenwich; King’s College London; Liverpool Institute for Performing Arts; Liverpool John Moores University; London Metropolitan University; Norwich University College of the Arts; Royal Veterinary College; School of Oriental and African Studies; St George’s, University of London; Staffordshire University; Trinity Laban Conservatoire of Music and Dance; UCL; University Campus Suffolk; University of Bedfordshire; University of Chester; University of Cumbria; University of Leeds; University of Liverpool; University of Oxford; University of Southampton; University of Worcester; University of Westminster; Queen Margaret University; Heriot-Watt University; Scottish Agricultural College; University of Dundee; Cardiff University; Cardiff Metropolitan University (formerly University of Wales Institute Cardiff); Queen’s University Belfast; University of Ulster.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/14/3/286/s1.

Author Contributions

Alexandra Pitman conceived the study with Michael King and David Osborn, and designed the analysis plan in collaboration with Khadija Rantell, Louise Marston, Michael King and David Osborn. Alexandra Pitman conducted the data analysis. All authors interpreted data. Alexandra Pitman wrote the paper, with input from Khadija Rantell, Louise Marston, Michael King and David Osborn. Alexandra Pitman had full access to all the data in the study, takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis, and is the guarantor.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The Medical Research Council had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision to publish the results.

References

  • 1.Pitman A., Osborn D.P.J., King M.B., Erlangsen A. Effects of suicide bereavement on mental health and suicide risk. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1:86–94. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70224-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Qin P., Mortensen P.B. The impact of parental status on the risk of completed suicide. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:797–802. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bolton J.M., Au W., Leslie W.D., Martens P.J., Enns M.W., Roos L.L., Katz L.Y., Wilcox H.C., Erlangsen A., Chateau D., et al. Parents bereaved by offspring suicide: A population-based longitudinal case-control study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:158–167. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Agerbo E. Midlife suicide risk, partner’s psychiatric illness, spouse and child bereavement by suicide or other modes of death: A gender specific study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2005;59:407–412. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.024950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.King M., Dinos S., Shaw J., Watson R., Stevens S., Passetti F., Weich S., Serfaty M. The Stigma Scale: Development of a standardised measure of the stigma of mental illness. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2007;190:248–254. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.024638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mehta N., Clement S., Marcus E., Stona A.C., Bezborodovs N., Evans-Lacko S., Palacios J., Docherty M., Barley E., Rose D., et al. Evidence for effective interventions to reduce mental health-related stigma and discrimination in the medium and long term: Systematic review. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2015;207:377. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.151944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sveen C.-A., Walby F.A. Suicide survivors’ mental health and grief reactions: A systematic review of controlled studies. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 2008;38:13–29. doi: 10.1521/suli.2008.38.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pitman A.L., Osborn D.P.J., Rantell K., King M.B. The stigma perceived by people bereaved by suicide and other sudden deaths: A cross-sectional UK study of 3432 bereaved adults. J. Psychosom. Res. 2016;87:22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pitman A.L., Osborn D.P.J., Rantell K., King M.B. Bereavement by suicide as a risk factor for suicide attempt: A cross-sectional national UK-wide study of 3432 young bereaved adults. BMJ Open. 2016;6:e009948. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Clement S., Schauman O., Graham T., Maggioni F., Evans-Lacko S., Bezborodovs N., Morgan C., Rusch N., Brown J.S.L., Thornicroft G. What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychol. Med. 2014;45:11–27. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chapple A., Ziebland S., Hawton K. Taboo and the different death? Perceptions of those bereaved by suicide or other traumatic death. Sociol. Health Illn. 2015;37:610–625. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sudak H., Maxim K., Carpenter M. Suicide and Stigma: A Review of the Literature and Personal Reflections. Acad. Psychiatry. 2008;32:136–142. doi: 10.1176/appi.ap.32.2.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cvinar J.G. Do Suicide Survivors Suffer Social Stigma: A Review of the Literature. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care. 2005;41:14–21. doi: 10.1111/j.0031-5990.2005.00004.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rusch N., Zlati A., Black G., Thornicroft G. Does the stigma of mental illness contribute to suicidality? Br. J. Psychiatry. 2014;205:257–259. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.145755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Knieper A.J. The suicide survivor’s grief and recovery. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 1999;29:353–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.O’Connor R.C., Nock M.K. The psychology of suicidal behaviour. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1:73–85. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70222-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stack S. Differentiating Suicide Ideators from Attempters: Violence-A Research Note. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 2013;44:46–57. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pitman A., Krysinska K., Osborn D., King M. Suicide in young men. Lancet. 2012;379:2383–2392. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60731-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.McQueen C., Henwood K. Young men in ‘crisis’: Attending to the language of teenage boys’ distress. Soc. Sci. Med. 2002;55:1493–1509. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00186-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pitman A.L., Osborn D.P.J., King M.B. The use of internet-mediated cross-sectional studies in mental health research. BJPsych Adv. 2015;21:175–184. doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.114.012948. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bailley S.E., Dunham K., Kral M.J. Factor structure of The Grief Experience Questionnaire (GEQ) Death Stud. 2000;24:721–738. doi: 10.1080/074811800750036596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Barrett T., Scott T. Development of the Grief Experience Questionnaire. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 1989;19:201–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.1989.tb01033.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Seguin M., Lesage A., Kiely M.C. Parental bereavement after suicide and accident: A comparative study. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 1995;25:489–492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kovac S.H., Range L.M. Writing Projects: Lessening Undergraduates’ Unique Suicidal Bereavement. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 2000;30:50–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Harwood D., Hawton K., Hope T., Jacoby R. The grief experiences and needs of bereaved relatives and friends of older people dying through suicide: A descriptive and case-control study. J. Affect. Disord. 2002;72:185–194. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00462-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Spiers N., Bebbington P.E., Dennis M.S., Brugha T.S., McManus S., Jenkins R., Meltzer H. Trends in suicidal ideation in England: The National Psychiatric Morbidity Surveys of 2000 and 2007. Psychol. Med. 2014;44:175–183. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bebbington P.E., Minot S., Cooper C., Dennis M., Meltzer H., Jenkins R., Brugha T. Suicidal ideation, self-harm and attempted suicide: Results from the British psychiatric morbidity survey 2000. Eur. Psychiatry. 2010;25:427–431. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McManus S., Meltzer H., Brugha T., Bebbington P., Jenkins R. Adult Psychiatric Morbidity in England, 2007. Results of a Household Survey. National Centre for Social Research; Leeds, UK: 2009. The Health and Social Care Information Centre. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Haw C., Bergen H., Casey D., Hawton K. Repetition of Deliberate Self-Harm: A Study of the Characteristics and Subsequent Deaths in Patients Presenting to a General Hospital According to Extent of Repetition. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 2007;37:379–396. doi: 10.1521/suli.2007.37.4.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Robins L.N., Wing J.K., Wittchen H.-U., Helzer J.E., Babor T., Burke J., Framer A.E., Jablensky A., Pickens R., Regier D. The Composite International Diagnostic Interview. An epidemiologic instrument for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1988;45:1069–1077. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800360017003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Peters L., Clark D., Carroll F. Are computerized interviews equivalent to human interviewers? CIDI-Auto versus CIDI in anxiety and depressive disorders. Psychol. Med. 1998;28:893–901. doi: 10.1017/S0033291798006655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brown J., Evans-Lacko S., Aschan L., Henderson M., Hatch S., Hotopf M. Seeking informal and formal help for mental health problems in the community: A secondary analysis from a psychiatric morbidity survey in South London. BMC Psychiatry. 2014;14:1–15. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0275-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bruffaerts R., Demyttenaere K., Hwang I., Chiu W., Sampson N., Kessler R.C., Alonso J., Borges G., de Girolamo G., de Graaf R., et al. Treatment of suicidal people around the world. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2011;199:64–70. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.084129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Stevenson F.A., Britten N., Barry C.A., Bradley C.P., Barber N. Self-treatment and its discussion in medical consultations: How is medical pluralism managed in practice? Soc. Sci. Med. 2003;57:513–527. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00377-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.ONS (Office for National Statistics) Standard Occupational Classification 2010 Volume 3, The National Statistics Socio-Economic Classification: (Rebased on the SOC2010) User Manual. HMSO; London, UK: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Haukoos J.S., Newgard C.D. Advanced Statistics: Missing Data in Clinical Research-Part 1: An Introduction and Conceptual Framework. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2007;14:662–668. doi: 10.1197/j.aem.2006.11.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Germans S., Van Heck G.L., Moran P., Hodiamont P.P.G. The Self-report Standardized Assessment of Personality-abbreviated Scale: Preliminary results of a brief screening test for personality disorders. Personality. 2008;2:70–76. doi: 10.1002/pmh.34. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Levy L.H., Derby J.F. Bereavement support groups: Who joins; who does not; and why. Am. J. Community Psychol. 1992;20:649–662. doi: 10.1007/BF00941776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Morgan G. The relative’s response. In: Jenkins R., Griffiths S., Wylie I., Hawton K., Morgan G., Tylee A., editors. The Prevention of Suicide. Department of Health (HMSO); London, UK: 1994. pp. 123–124. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Walter T. On Bereavement: The Culture of Grief. Open University Press; Buckingham, UK: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Feigelman W., Gorman B.S., Jordan J.R. Stigmatization and suicide bereavement. Death Stud. 2009;33:591–608. doi: 10.1080/07481180902979973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wagner K.G., Calhoun L.G. Perceptions of Social Support by Suicide Survivors and Their Social Networks. Omega. 1991;24:61–73. doi: 10.2190/3748-G16Y-YEBF-QD10. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pitman A.L., Rantell K., Moran P., Sireling L., Marston L., King M.B., Osborn D.P.J. Support received after bereavement by suicide and other sudden deaths: A cross-sectional UK study of 3432 bereaved adults. BMJ Open. 2017 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014487. under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pitman A.L., Putri A.K., Kennedy N., De Souza T., King M., Osborn D. Priorities for the development and evaluation of support after suicide bereavement in the UK. Bereave. Care. 2016;35:109–116. doi: 10.1080/02682621.2016.1254457. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.INQUEST . Inquest Handbook. INQUEST; London, UK: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Public Health England. National Suicide Prevention Alliance . Help Is at Hand: Support after Someone May Have Died by Suicide. HMSO; London, UK: 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cruse Bereavement Care . Changing Lives: Annual Review 2015/2016. Cruse Bereavement Care; Richmond, UK: 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Carson JSpencer Foundation. Crisis Care Network. National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention. American Association of Suicidology . A Manager’s Guide to Suicide Postvention in the Workplace: 10 Action Steps for Dealing with the Aftermath of Suicide. Carson JSpencer Foundation; Denver, CO, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Department of Health . Preventing Suicide in England: A Cross-Government Outcomes Strategy to Save Lives. Mental Health and Disability Division, Deaprtment of Health; London, UK: 2012. p. 17680. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS), Office of the Surgeon General and National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention . National Strategy for Suicide Prevention: Goals and Objectives for Action. HHS; Washington, DC, USA: 2012. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Walter T. Modern Death: Taboo or not Taboo? Sociology. 1991;125:293–310. doi: 10.1177/0038038591025002009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES