Skip to main content
The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity logoLink to The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity
. 2017 Mar 27;14:40. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0495-x

Does social distinction contribute to socioeconomic inequalities in diet: the case of ‘superfoods’ consumption

Joost Oude Groeniger 1,, Frank J van Lenthe 1, Mariëlle A Beenackers 1, Carlijn B M Kamphuis 2
PMCID: PMC5369222  PMID: 28347301

Abstract

Background

The key mechanisms underlying socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intake are still poorly understood, hampering the development of interventions. An important, but sparsely mentioned mechanism is that of ‘social distinction’, whereby those in a higher socioeconomic position adopt dietary patterns by which they can distinguish themselves from lower socioeconomic groups. We investigated the importance of distinction as a mechanism, by testing the socioeconomic gradient in the consumption of so-called ‘superfoods’ and the contribution of a well-established indicator of distinction, cultural participation.

Methods

Data from participants (25–75 years) of the 2014 survey of the Dutch population-based GLOBE study were used (N = 2812). Multivariable regression models were used to analyse the association between education, income and cultural participation (e.g. visits to museums, opera, theatre, concerts) and the consumption of superfoods (spelt, quinoa and goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass).

Results

The consumption of superfoods is far more prevalent among higher socioeconomic groups. Adjusting for cultural participation strongly attenuated the educational and income gradient in superfoods consumption, whereas cultural participation remained strongly associated with superfoods consumption. Those in the highest quintile of cultural participation reported the highest consumption of spelt products (OR = 2.97, 95% CI = 2.10;4.18), quinoa (OR = 3.50, 95% CI = 2.12;5.79) and goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass (OR = 2.69, 95% CI = 1.73;4.17).

Conclusions

The associations between socioeconomic position and the consumption of ‘superfoods’ seem to be partially driven by a process of social distinction. These findings suggest that distinction may be an important, but currently neglected mechanism in generating socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intake. It deserves a more prominent role in interventions to reduce these inequalities.

Keywords: Socioeconomic inequalities, Dietary patterns, Superfoods, Distinction, Cultural participation

Background

Dietary intake is greatly socioeconomically patterned, with those in higher socioeconomic groups consuming more healthy products (e.g. fruit and vegetables) and less unhealthy products (e.g. sugars and fats) [14]. Common explanations for these inequalities are differences in monetary resources [5], unequal access to healthy foods [6], or knowledge [7]. Indeed, these factors contribute to the explanation of inequalities in dietary intakes, but cannot fully explain the gradient in healthy intakes [8]. An additional, but far less mentioned mechanism is that of ‘social distinction’, whereby those in a higher socioeconomic position adopt dietary patterns by which they can distinguish themselves from lower socioeconomic groups [912]. Social distinction – most famously described by the French sociologist Pierre Bourdieu [9] as part of his notion of ‘cultural capital’ – may have become increasingly important as a determinant for health-related lifestyles, now that opportunities for distinction on the basis of material possessions have declined [13]. Due to increasing economic prosperity and wealth, more people are able to afford luxury consumer goods (e.g. smartphone ownership, branded clothing) and material conditions have become less important as a determinant of lifestyles [14]. Except for some theoretical contributions on this notion [12, 15], empirical studies focussing on the importance of social distinction for socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intake are scarce [10]. The rapid popularity of so-called ‘superfoods’ offers an interesting opportunity to do so.

Although no official guidelines exist to what constitutes a ‘superfood’, it usually applies to food products that contain high amounts of particular nutrients (e.g. anti-oxidants, vitamins, minerals), which were only recently marketed to a wider public in Western countries (e.g. spelt products, quinoa, goji berries). Given that the scientific evidence is inconclusive – there is no evidence that superfoods are healthier than equivalent alternatives – is must be merely the perception of health benefits that may have triggered the popularity. At the same time, high prices may have strengthened the exclusivity of the perceived super-healthy products. As such, superfoods consumption may be particularly prone to social distinction.

Building upon the theory of cultural capital, cultural participation (e.g. visiting museums, theatre) is arguably the best available indicator of distinction [9, 16, 17]. The knowledge and preferences that are required for the appreciation of cultural practices, such as paintings or ballet, are highly socially patterned and transmitted across generations [18, 19], making it a suitable method for distinction and exclusion [9]. Importantly, cultural participation is unlikely to be associated with the consumption of superfoods in a causal way (i.e. the mere act of a cultural visit is not likely to cause an increase in superfoods consumption). We therefore examined whether superfoods consumption is patterned by socioeconomic position and positively associated with this well-established indicator of social distinction. Specifically, we investigated whether the consumption of five popular and commonly available superfoods (i.e. spelt products, quinoa, goji berries, chia seeds and wheatgrass) is 1) more prevalent in high than low socioeconomic groups, and 2) associated with cultural participation, once adjusted for education, income, and potential confounders.

Methods

Data were collected from participants of the Dutch population-based GLOBE study - a cohort study on socioeconomic inequalities in health in the Netherlands. The GLOBE study was initiated in 1991 when data was collected among a random sample of non-institutionalized persons (ages 15–74) from the city of Eindhoven and surrounding municipalities. More detailed information on the objectives, study design, and data collection of the Dutch GLOBE study can be found elsewhere [20, 21]. The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943).

For the purposes of this study, cross-sectional data from the fifth wave (2014) of the GLOBE study were used. Data were collected by means of a large-scale postal survey which was sent out to 10,668 persons, comprising 4,886 participants of the existing GLOBE cohort, supplemented with a random sample of 5782 newly selected persons from the municipality register of the city of Eindhoven. Of the respondents of the 2014 data collection (N = 4,851, response = 45.5%), those between 25 and 75 years old and living in the city of Eindhoven were selected to represent the target population. This resulted in a total sample of 2812 participants eligible for the study, among which 1114 participants from the original GLOBE cohort.

Explanatory variables

Cultural participation was measured by asking respondents how often they visited seven cultural practices: art museums, historical museums, opera or ballet, classical concerts, theatre, architecture, and popular concerts or festivals (0 – never, 1 – once per year and 2 – more than once per year). These were summed and subsequently divided into quintiles, which resulted into categories that corresponded with the interpretation of one higher quintile equals 2 additional visits.

Highest attained educational level was classified according to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED): 1 – primary education (ISCED 0–1), 2 – lower secondary education (ISCED 2), 3 – upper secondary education (ISCED 3–4), 4 – tertiary education (ISCED 5–7).

Household equivalent income was measured as the level of household income per month divided by the square root of the number of people living from this income and divided into 5 categories: 1–< €1000/month, 2–€1000–€1500/month, 3–€1500–€2000/month, 4–€2000–2500/month and 5–> €2500/month.

Potential confounders included were sex (male, female), age (in 10-year age groups), living together with a partner (yes, no), country of birth (Netherlands, other), having children living at home (yes, no), employment status (full-time employed, part-time employed, unemployed, retired, homemaker, other), and weekly fruit and vegetable consumption (to adjust for ‘regular’ healthy food choices). Fruit and vegetable consumption was measured with a food frequency questionnaire [22]. Participants were asked how many times, on a weekly basis, they had consumed fruit and vegetables in the previous month. Subsequently they were asked to indicate how many portions they ate on a typical occasion. Consumption of fruit and vegetables was calculated by adding up how much fruit and vegetables (per 100 grams) participants consumed in total in a typical week, where 100 grams was used as the equivalent of one piece of fruit.

Outcome variables

Participants were asked how many times per week on average they consumed five superfoods over the last month: spelt products, quinoa, goji berries, chia seeds and wheatgrass (selected because these are commonly available superfoods in The Netherlands). Because of the low prevalence of superfoods consumption, the variables were dichotomised (≥1/week, versus <1/week). The prevalence of goji berries, chia seeds and wheatgrass consumption were especially low. Since these products are usually consumed as added substances (e.g. to a salad or a smoothie), and may be consumed interchangeably (diluting the effect of the separate products), the three variables were summed in one variable that indicated consuming goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass at least once per week.

Statistical analysis

First, descriptive statistics for the study sample were calculated. Second, age- and sex-standardized prevalence of superfoods consumption were calculated for the full sample and for all education, income and cultural participation groups. Third, multivariable logistic regression models were used to estimate the associations between each of the three explanatory variables separately (cultural participation, education and income) and the consumption of superfoods, adjusted for potential confounders. Fourth, a multivariable logistic regression model was used to estimate the associations between the explanatory variables and the consumption of superfoods adjusted for each other and potential confounders. Missing data were present for questions about living together with a partner (1.1%), country of birth (.5%), employment status (1.9%), fruit and vegetable consumption (3.3%), cultural participation (5.3%), educational level (.9%) and household equivalent income (12.7%) and were handled via multiple imputation (m = 5). Respondents with missing data on an outcome variable were excluded concerning that outcome variable, resulting in different samples for the three outcome variables: 1.9% missing on spelt products, 2.7% missing on quinoa and 2.3% missing on goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass. The analyses were weighted by respondent-level sample weights to account for the sampling strategy within the GLOBE study. All analyses were performed using Stata, version 14.

Results

The mean age of the sample was 48.9 (SD 15.6) and 55.2% was female (Table 1). Prevalence rates of superfoods consumption ranged from 40.5% for spelt, 21.7% for quinoa, to 18.6% for goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass. Those with higher levels of education, income or cultural participation had higher prevalence rates of all superfoods (Table 2).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of GLOBE 2014 participantsa

Variables Percentage or mean (SD)
Gender
 Men 44.8
 Women 55.2
Age groups
 25–34 years 25.6
 35–44 years 16.9
 45–54 years 17.6
 55–64 years 19.3
 65–75 years 20.7
Living together with a partner
 Yes 74.0
 No 26.0
Country of birth
 Netherlands 88.5
 Else 11.5
Children living at home
 No 64.3
 Yes 35.7
Employment status
 Full-time employed 38.8
 Part-time employed 24.9
 Unemployed 8.0
 Retired 20.4
 Homemaker 4.5
 Other 3.4
Weekly intake of fruit & vegetables (per 100 grams)b 19.7 (10.0)
Cultural participation
 1 lowest (0–1 visits per year) 24.8
 2 (at least 2 visits per year) 20.9
 3 (at least 4 visits per year) 20.3
 4 (at least 6 visits per year) 17.7
 5 highest (at least 8 visits per year) 16.3
Educational level
 Primary 4.9
 Lower secondary 20.9
 Upper secondary 25.1
 Tertiary 49.1
Household equivalent income
  < €1000/month 13.8
 €1000–€1500/month 20.1
 €1500–€2000/month 24.0
 €2000–€2500/month 29.5
  > €2500/month 12.7
Superfoods consumption (at least once per week)
 Spelt products 41.0
 Quinoa 21.5
 Goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass 18.6

a Descriptive statistics calculated on non-imputed and weighted data

b Weekly intake of fruit and vegetables is expressed as mean (standard deviation)

Table 2.

Age- and sex-standardized prevalence of superfoods consumption by education, income and cultural participation

Spelt products Quinoa Goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass
At least once per week (%) At least once per week (%) At least once per week (%)
Total 40.5 21.7 18.6
Cultural participation
 1 lowest (0–1 visits per year) 23.5 7.8 11.3
 2 (at least 2 visits per year) 34.4 14.8 16.3
 3 (at least 4 visits per year) 41.3 22.0 17.2
 4 (at least 6 visits per year) 53.2 31.4 23.0
 5 highest (at least 8 visits per year) 59.8 38.1 30.8
Educational level
 Primary 19.1 7.7 12.9
 Lower secondary 30.5 8.6 10.1
 Upper secondary 35.0 15.4 16.9
 Tertiary 50.3 30.9 21.9
Household equivalent income
  < €1000/month 29.9 15.3 18.5
 €1000–€1500/month 36.9 15.8 16.4
 €1500–€2000/month 40.7 20.8 18.1
 €2000–€2500/month 48.0 27.7 20.1
  > €2500/month 53.9 35.6 23.6

Cultural participation was strongly associated with the consumption of all superfoods, also when taking education and income into account (Table 3). Compared to participants in the lowest quintile of cultural participation, those in the highest quintile reported more consumption of spelt products (OR = 2.97, 95% CI = 2.10;4.18), quinoa (OR = 3.50, 95% CI = 2.12;5.79) and goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass (OR = 2.69, 95% CI = 1.73;4.17). The associations between education and income and superfoods consumption were strongly attenuated when adjusting for all variables (Table 3). Educational level remained associated with superfood consumption, although less strongly than cultural participation. Income was not independently associated with any of the outcomes. Associations of the potential confounders with superfood consumption are presented in Appendix 1.

Table 3.

Multivariable logistic regression models of the association between cultural participation, educational level and income on superfoods consumption

Spelt products (N = 2759) Quinoa (N = 2737) Goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass (N = 2746)
Crude modela Adjusted modelb Crude modela Adjusted modelb Crude modela Adjusted modelb
Variables Categories OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Cultural participation (quintiles) 1 lowest 1 1 1 1 1 1
2 1.54 (1.16, 2.05) 1.39 (1.04, 1.87) 1.64 (1.06, 2.55) 1.40 (.89, 2.22) 1.53 (1.03, 2.29) 1.49 (.98, 2.25)
3 1.99 (1.48, 2.67) 1.72 (1.25, 2.35) 2.68 (1.72, 4.16) 2.08 (1.29, 3.35) 1.59 (1.07, 2.36) 1.55 (1.01, 2.36)
4 3.06 (2.26, 4.15) 2.54 (1.83, 3.52) 4.09 (2.64, 6.34) 2.94 (1.83, 4.72) 2.04 (1.37, 3.04) 1.98 (1.28, 3.05)
5 highest 3.72 (2.72, 5.08) 2.97 (2.10, 4.18) 5.37 (3.41, 8.47) 3.50 (2.12, 5.79) 2.83 (1.89, 4.22) 2.69 (1.73, 4.17)
Educational level Primary 1 1 1 1 1 1
Lower secondary 2.15 (1.23, 3.76) 1.78 (1.00, 3.16) 1.13 (.46, 2.81) .91 (.36, 2.33) 1.19 (.56, 2.53) 1.10 (.51, 2.40)
Upper secondary 2.58 (1.46, 4.54) 1.87 (1.04, 3.37) 2.12 (.88, 5.11) 1.45 (.58, 3.63) 1.79 (.85, 3.76) 1.50 (.69, 3.27)
Tertiary 4.36 (2.49, 7.63) 2.55 (1.41, 4.62) 4.70 (1.97, 11.25) 2.37 (.93, 6.02) 2.08 (1.00, 4.32) 1.50 (.68, 3.32)
Household equivalent income <€1000 1 1 1 1 1 1
€1000–€1500 1.20 (.85, 1.69) 1.02 (.71, 1.45) .84 (.53, 1.33) .71 (.44, 1.15) .82 (.51, 1.31) .73 (.44, 1.20)
€1500–€2000 1.34 (.93, 1.93) .97 (.67, 1.41) 1.15 (.72, 1.84) .81 (.50, 1.31) .87 (.56, 1.34) .69 (.44, 1.09)
€2000–€2500 1.64 (1.16, 2.31) .97 (.66, 1.42) 1.58 (1.01, 2.46) .87 (.55, 1.40) .94 (.61, 1.45) .68 (.42, 1.07)
>€2500 1.93 (1.28, 2.91) 1.01 (.65, 1.58) 2.48 (1.53, 4.01) 1.18 (.71, 1.95) 1.07 (.65, 1.77) .71 (.41, 1.22)

aAll models were adjusted for confounders: sex, age, living together with a partner, country of birth, children living at home and employment status and fruit and vegetable consumption

bAll models included cultural participation, educational level, household equivalent income, sex, age, living together with a partner, country of birth, children living at home, employment status and fruit and vegetable consumption

Discussion

This is the first study to show that the consumption of five popular ‘superfoods’ was highly patterned by socioeconomic position. Superfoods consumption was also strongly correlated with cultural participation – a classical indicator of social distinction – independent of education, income, and a range of potential confounders. Moreover, the associations between education and income and superfoods consumption were strongly attenuated after inclusion of indicators of cultural participation.

To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have tried to link a well–established indicator of social distinction to the consumption of superfoods in a large population-based cohort. Yet, our study suffers from some limitations. First, cultural participation and superfoods consumption were self-reported. Although these questions were only a small part of a broader questionnaire, it may be that higher socioeconomic groups are more likely to report higher levels of cultural participation and superfoods consumption due to social desirability (as a result of the hypothesised distinction mechanism). Second, we only studied five types of ‘superfoods’. Even though these superfoods are among the most readily available in The Netherlands, they constitute a limited number of potential superfood products. Third, we also included a limited number of cultural activities, but there are likely more social distinction instruments. For instance, a previous study examined whether smoking can be seen as a form of distinction by testing the relationship between musical tastes and smoking [23]. In our study we chose cultural participation as a more suitable indicator of distinction, since a recent review found that this indicator was most often used to measure ‘embodied cultural capital’ [16]. In line with the work of Pierre Bourdieu, it is embodied cultural capital (internalized dispositions, such as skills, tastes and attitudes that can be used for distinction and exclusion) [9, 24, 25] that is most relevant for distinctive lifestyle patterns [15]. Fourth, we tried to adjust for several important confounders. For instance, availability of time to participate in cultural activities was controlled for by including employment status and having children living at home [26]. Also, sensitivity analyses showed that controlling for social participation (frequency of participating in several associations and organizations, e.g. neighbourhood association, political organization, sport club, volunteer organization) did not change the findings. However, we cannot rule out that there are other determinants that are related to both cultural participation and the consumption of superfood for which we did not control. For instance, certain personality traits (e.g. openness to experience) could make it more likely that someone visits cultural activities and consumes superfoods [16].

Our findings are in line with previous findings that higher socioeconomic groups make more health-conscious food choices than lower socioeconomic groups [27]. The early adoption of innovative products in higher socioeconomic groups is also seen for other products [28]. The clear association with cultural participation however, adds to this that social distinction may be an important underlying mechanism. Because superfoods are more expensive and exclusive, they may be attractive as a means to distinguish oneself from other social groups [10, 29]. Yet, the strong attenuation of the observed association between income and superfoods consumption also suggests that income in itself is not an important determinant of superfoods intake. Future research should examine whether distinction is indeed also relevant for more common dietary products with established health benefits and relative high prices, and whether the socioeconomic gradient in healthy dietary intake is to some extent reflective of this process. For these purposes, it could be worthwhile to use methods that are able to measure the mechanism of distinction (both implicit and explicit) more directly (e.g. Implicit Association Tests [30] or prototype perceptions [31]).

An important implication from our study is that interventions aimed at improving dietary choices in lower socioeconomic groups particularly, should be aware of the mechanism of social distinction. Distinction reflects a lifelong socialization process that is influenced by the norms, habits and preferences within socio-cultural environments [9, 12, 15, 32]. Dietary patterns are part of a distinct lifestyle pattern and some low socioeconomic subgroups may be more inclined to consume foods that are cheap and high in fat, and may be less concerned with healthiness than their advantaged counterparts [9, 27, 29, 33]. Therefore, it can be particularly challenging to improve the dietary behaviors of socioeconomically disadvantaged people [34]. Interventions focussing on young children and their parents may therefore be most successful, as incorporating healthy food habits may then become part of the socialisation process. On the other hand, interventions may also aim to reduce the exclusiveness of healthy products, and thus change the socio-cultural meaning and ‘distinctive value’ of healthy food products.

Conclusions

The consumption of ‘superfoods’ seems to be a contemporary expression of social distinction in higher socioeconomic groups. This distinction mechanism may be an important determinant of inequalities in dietary intake. It deserves a more prominent role in interventions to reduce these inequalities.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The study was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (grant number 200500005).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to privacy regulations but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Authors’ contributions

JOG analyzed the data and wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to interpreting the results and writing the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943).

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Table 4.

Multivariable logistic regression models of the association between the explanatory variables and the consumption of superfoods

Spelt products Quinoa Goji berries, chia seeds or wheatgrass
Variables Categories OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Sex Men 1 1 1
Women 1.89 1.54 2.31 2.06 1.62 2.62 2.14 1.67 2.74
Age groups 25–34 1 1 1
35–44 1.06 .80 1.41 .71 .51 .99 .74 .53 1.04
45–54 1.25 .93 1.69 .90 .63 1.28 .57 .39 .82
55–64 .97 .72 1.31 .65 .45 .93 .57 .40 .82
65–75 .77 .44 1.32 .46 .21 1.00 .30 .13 .68
Living together with a partner Yes 1 1 1
No .98 .77 1.25 .98 .73 1.31 1.01 .76 1.36
Birth country The Netherlands 1 1 1
Else .73 .54 1.00 .83 .57 1.20 1.65 1.17 2.34
Children living at home No 1 1 1
Yes .83 .65 1.06 .89 .67 1.19 .97 .72 1.32
Employment status Full-time employed 1 1 1
Part-time employed 1.07 .83 1.38 .96 .72 1.28 .98 .73 1.32
Unemployed .52 .34 .79 .60 .36 .99 .79 .48 1.31
Retired 1.28 .74 2.21 .61 .28 1.31 .84 .37 1.90
Non-employed .94 .56 1.57 .57 .27 1.16 .65 .33 1.25
Other 1.00 .62 1.59 1.08 .61 1.90 1.21 .68 2.16
Weekly fruit & vegetable consumption (per 100 grams) 1.03 1.02 1.04 1.03 1.02 1.05 1.05 1.04 1.06
Cultural participation (quintiles) 1 lowest 1 1 1
2 1.39 1.04 1.87 1.40 .89 2.22 1.49 .98 2.25
3 1.72 1.25 2.35 2.08 1.29 3.35 1.55 1.01 2.36
4 2.54 1.83 3.52 2.94 1.83 4.72 1.98 1.28 3.05
5 highest 2.97 2.10 4.18 3.50 2.12 5.79 2.69 1.73 4.17
Educational level Primary 1 1 1
Lower secondary 1.78 1.00 3.16 .91 .36 2.33 1.10 .51 2.40
Upper secondary 1.87 1.04 3.37 1.45 .58 3.63 1.50 .69 3.27
Tertiary 2.55 1.41 4.62 2.37 .93 6.02 1.50 .68 3.32
Household equivalent income <€1000 1 1 1
€1000–€1500 1.02 .71 1.45 .71 .44 1.15 .73 .44 1.20
€1500–€2000 .97 .67 1.41 .81 .50 1.31 .69 .44 1.09
€2000–€2500 .97 .66 1.42 .87 .55 1.40 .68 .42 1.07
>€2500 1.01 .65 1.58 1.18 .71 1.95 .71 .41 1.22

All models included cultural participation, educational level, household equivalent income, sex, age, living together with a partner, country of birth, children living at home, employment status and fruit and vegetable consumption

Contributor Information

Joost Oude Groeniger, Phone: +31 10 703 84 58, Email: j.oudegroeniger@erasmusmc.nl.

Frank J. van Lenthe, Email: f.vanlenthe@erasmusmc.nl

Mariëlle A. Beenackers, Email: m.beenackers@erasmusmc.nl

Carlijn B. M. Kamphuis, Email: C.B.M.Kamphuis@uu.nl

References

  • 1.Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Does social class predict diet quality? Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87:1107–17. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/87.5.1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Giskes K, Avendano M, Brug J, et al. A systematic review of studies on socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intakes associated with weight gain and overweight/obesity conducted among European adults. Obes Rev. 2010;11:413–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2009.00658.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Novaković R, Cavelaars A, Geelen A, et al. Socio-economic determinants of micronutrient intake and status in Europe: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17:1031–45. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013001341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.De Irala-Estevez J, Groth M, Johansson L, et al. A systematic review of socio-economic differences in food habits in Europe: consumption of fruit and vegetables. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54:706–14. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Contribution of food prices and diet cost to socioeconomic disparities in diet quality and health: a systematic review and analysis. Nutr Rev. 2015;73:643–60. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Story M, Kaphingst KM, Robinson-O'Brien R, et al. Creating healthy food and eating environments: policy and environmental approaches. Annu Rev Public Health. 2008;29:253–72. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Turrell G, Kavanagh AM. Socio-economic pathways to diet: modelling the association between socio-economic position and food purchasing behaviour. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9:375–83. doi: 10.1079/PHN2006850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ball K, Crawford D, Mishra G. Socio-economic inequalities in women's fruit and vegetable intakes: a multilevel study of individual, social and environmental mediators. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9:623–30. doi: 10.1079/PHN2005897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bourdieu P. Distinction: A social critique of the judgement of taste. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1984.
  • 10.Pampel FC, Krueger PM, Denney JT. Socioeconomic Disparities in Health Behaviors. Annu Rev Sociol. 2010;36:349–70. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cockerham WC. Health lifestyle theory and the convergence of agency and structure. J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46:51–67. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Williams SJ. Theorising class, health and lifestyles: can Bourdieu help us? Sociol Health Illn. 1995;17:577–604. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.ep10932093. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mackenbach JP. The persistence of health inequalities in modern welfare states: the explanation of a paradox. Soc Sci Med. 2012;75:761–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.02.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Van Eijck K, Bargeman B. The changing impact of social background on lifestyle:“culturalization” instead of individualization? Poetics. 2004;32:447–69. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2004.09.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Abel T. Cultural capital and social inequality in health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62:e13. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.066159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kamphuis CB, Jansen T, Mackenbach JP, et al. Bourdieu's Cultural Capital in Relation to Food Choices: A Systematic Review of Cultural Capital Indicators and an Empirical Proof of Concept. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0130695. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nagel I, Ganzeboom HBG. Art and Socialisation. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. Second. Oxford: Elsevier; 2015. pp. 7–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kraaykamp G, van Eijck K. The Intergenerational Reproduction of Cultural Capital A Threefold Perspective. Soc Forces. 2010;89:209–31. doi: 10.1353/sof.2010.0087. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dimaggio P. Cultural Capital and School Success - the Impact of Status Culture Participation on the Grades of United-States High-School-Students. Am Sociol Rev. 1982;47:189–201. doi: 10.2307/2094962. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CB, Beenackers MA, et al. Cohort Profile: Understanding socioeconomic inequalities in health and health behaviours: the GLOBE study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:721–30. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mackenbach JP, van de Mheen H, Stronks K. A prospective cohort study investigating the explanation of socio-economic inequalities in health in The Netherlands. Soc Sci Med. 1994;38:299–308. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90399-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bogers RP, Van Assema P, Kester AD, et al. Reproducibility, validity, and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for measuring fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159:900–9. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pampel FC. Socioeconomic Distinction, Cultural Tastes, and Cigarette Smoking. Soc Sci Q. 2006;87:19–35. doi: 10.1111/j.0038-4941.2006.00366.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson JG, editor. Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education. Westport: Greenwood Press; 1986. pp. 241–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lamont M, Lareau A. Cultural capital: Allusions, gaps and glissandos in recent theoretical developments. Sociol Theory. 1988;6:153–68. doi: 10.2307/202113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kraaykamp G, van Gils W, Ultee W. Cultural participation and time restrictions - Explaining the frequency of individual and joint cultural visits. Poetics. 2008;36:316–U3. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2008.04.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kamphuis CBM, de Bekker-Grob EW, van Lenthe FJ. Factors affecting food choices of older adults from high and low socioeconomic groups: a discrete choice experiment. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101:768–74. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.096776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rogers EM. Diffusion of innovations. New York: Free Press; 1962.
  • 29.Kraaykamp G. Cumulative advantages and inequality in lifestyle: A Dutch description of distinction in taste. Neth J Soc Sci. 2002;38:121–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:1464–80. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gibbons FX, Gerrard M. Predicting Young-Adults Health Risk Behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69:505–17. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Singh-Manoux A, Marmot M. Role of socialization in explaining social inequalities in health. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:2129–33. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Christensen VT, Carpiano RM. Social class differences in BMI among Danish women: applying Cockerham's health lifestyles approach and Bourdieu's theory of lifestyle. Soc Sci Med. 2014;112:12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Delormier T, Frohlich KL, Potvin L. Food and eating as social practice - understanding eating patterns as social phenomena and implications for public health. Sociol Health Illn. 2009;31:215–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2008.01128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to privacy regulations but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES