Abstract
Circadian rhythms optimize physiology and behavior to the varying demands of the 24 h day. The master circadian clock is located in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus and it regulates circadian oscillators in tissues throughout the body to prevent internal desynchrony. Here, we demonstrate for the first time that, under standard 12 h:12 h light/dark (LD) cycles, object, visuospatial, and olfactory recognition performance in C57BL/6J mice is consistently better at midday relative to midnight. However, under repeated exposure to constant light (rLL), recognition performance becomes desynchronized, with object and visuospatial performance better at subjective midday and olfactory performance better at subjective midnight. This desynchrony in behavioral performance is mirrored by changes in expression of the canonical clock genes Period1 and Period2 (Per1 and Per2), as well as the immediate-early gene Fos in the SCN, dorsal hippocampus, and olfactory bulb. Under rLL, rhythmic Per1 and Fos expression is attenuated in the SCN. In contrast, hippocampal gene expression remains rhythmic, mirroring object and visuospatial performance. Strikingly, Per1 and Fos expression in the olfactory bulb is reversed, mirroring the inverted olfactory performance. Temporal desynchrony among these regions does not result in arrhythmicity because core body temperature and exploratory activity rhythms persist under rLL. Our data provide the first demonstration that abnormal lighting conditions can give rise to temporal desynchrony between autonomous circadian oscillators in different regions, with different consequences for performance across different sensory domains. Such a dispersed network of dissociable circadian oscillators may provide greater flexibility when faced with conflicting environmental signals.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT A master circadian clock in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus regulates physiology and behavior across the 24 h day by synchronizing peripheral clocks throughout the brain and body. Without the SCN, these peripheral clocks rapidly become desynchronized. Here, we provide a unique demonstration that, under lighting conditions in which the central clock in the SCN is dampened, peripheral oscillators in the hippocampus and olfactory bulb become desynchronized, along with the behavioral processes mediated by these clocks. Multiple clocks that adopt different phase relationships may enable processes occurring in different brain regions to be optimized to specific phases of the 24 h day. Moreover, such a dispersed network of dissociable circadian clocks may provide greater flexibility when faced with conflicting environmental signals (e.g., seasonal changes in photoperiod).
Keywords: circadian, clock genes, hippocampus, internal desynchrony, olfactory bulb, suprachiasmatic nuclei
Introduction
Virtually all organisms possess an internal circadian clock that adapts physiology and behavior to the varying demands of the 24 h day. In mammals, the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the anterior hypothalamus are the site of the master circadian pacemaker and lesions of the SCN abolish daily rhythms in behavior and endocrine function (Moore and Eichler, 1972; Stephan and Zucker, 1972). The SCN clock is based upon an intracellular transcriptional–translational feedback loop involving a number of canonical clock genes (Reppert and Weaver, 2002). This includes Period1 (Per1) and Period2 (Per2), which are directly modulated by light to enable the SCN clock to be entrained to the external light/dark (LD) cycle (Albrecht et al., 1997; Bae et al., 2001). Rhythmic clock gene expression is also found in many different regions of the brain outside of the SCN (Guilding and Piggins, 2007; Herzog, 2007; Kyriacou and Hastings, 2010). These peripheral clocks are thought to regulate local rhythms in tissue-specific physiology, but are dependent upon the master pacemaker in the SCN to prevent internal desynchrony and maintain an appropriate phase relationship with the external LD cycle (Herzog and Tosini, 2001; Dibner et al., 2010). In isolation from the SCN, most peripheral clocks rapidly dampen as individual cellular oscillators become desynchronized (Yoo et al., 2004).
Like many biological and cognitive processes, learning and memory are under the influence of the circadian system and circadian variation in performance has been described in many nonmammalian species, with performance better during the active phase (Fernandez et al., 2003; Lyons et al., 2005). In contrast, findings from nocturnal rodents are less conclusive. Some studies reported better performance during the active (i.e., dark) phase (Valentinuzzi et al., 2001; Gritton et al., 2009, 2012), whereas others found better performance during the light phase (Chaudhury and Colwell, 2002; Rawashdeh et al., 2014). These contradictory findings could be due to the nature of the behavioral tasks used (typically appetitive or aversive) or the nature of stimuli used and have been a major limitation in further understanding the mechanisms by which circadian rhythms regulate learning and memory performance.
Here, we use the spontaneous object recognition task (Ennaceur and Delacour, 1988; Dix and Aggleton, 1999; Bevins and Besheer, 2006) to investigate circadian variation in memory performance in C57BL/6J mice. Different variants of the task are used to investigate memory for objects, places, and odors to determine whether there is any consistent day/night variation in memory for different types of stimulus. We show that, under standard 12 h:12 h LD cycles, memory performance for objects, places, and odors is consistently better during the light phase. We then repeated these experiments under constant conditions, using 48 h of constant light (LL) interspersed with 48 h of LD to prevent free running. Under these repeated LL (rLL) conditions, in which environmental time cues are eliminated at the time of recognition testing, we demonstrate a decoupling of object and visuospatial performance (which remains optimal during the subjective light phase) versus olfactory performance (which now becomes optimal during the subjective dark phase). Moreover, we show that circadian changes in mRNA levels of Per1 and the immediate-early gene Fos are dampened in the SCN. Strikingly, in accordance with the behavioral data, rLL alters the circadian phase of Per1 and Fos expression in the olfactory bulb, but not in the dorsal hippocampus, a region that (together with adjacent and interconnected cortical areas) contributes to object and visuospatial recognition memory processes (Clark et al., 2000; Broadbent et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2010; Aggleton et al., 2012; Cohen et al., 2013). Collectively, our data provide a unique demonstration that, under abnormal lighting conditions in which SCN rhythms are dampened, decoupling of olfactory versus object and visuospatial recognition performance can occur. These dissociable circadian rhythms in behavioral performance may provide greater flexibility when faced with conflicting environmental signals (e.g., seasonal changes in photoperiod).
Materials and Methods
Animals
C57BL/6JOlaHsd male mice (2.5–5 months of age; Envigo RRID:IMSR_JAX:000664) were used in all experiments. Upon arrival at the colony, mice were put in pairs in plastic cages (length × width × height = 46 cm × 29 cm × 23 cm) with ad libitum access to food and water and housed under LD; the room temperature of the colony was maintained at 22 ± 2°C. Cages were placed inside a light-tight ventilated chamber (LTC) to eliminate exposure to extraneous photic cues (Fisher et al., 2012). The LTC was equipped with multiple white LEDs, which provided a light level of 250–280 lux (measured at the center of the cage floor) in the light phase. All mice were housed under LD for at least 7 d before they were allocated to different lighting conditions (described below). All procedures were performed in accordance with the United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 under Project Licenses 30/2812 and 30/3371 and Personal License I869292DB.
Lighting conditions
Condition 1: standard 12 h:12 h LD cycles
For mice in the LD condition, behavioral testing was conducted at midday [Zeitgeber time (ZT) 6 h ± 30 min] and at midnight (ZT 18 h ± 30 min) across multiple days, as shown in Figure 1A, left. ZT 0 h indicates the onset of the light phase and ZT 12 h indicates the onset of the dark phase.
Condition 2: rLL
For mice in the rLL condition, the lighting condition was alternated between standard LD and 48 h of constant light, as shown in Figure 1A, right. Behavioral testing was conducted at the animal's subjective midday [Circadian time (CT) 6 h ± 30 min] and subjective midnight (CT 18 h ± 30 min) under multiple 48 h periods of constant light (starting from the second cycle of LL); the circadian time points are defined by the LD transition before the onset of LL. The repeated alternation between standard LD and LL enables animals to reentrain to the original LD cycle after 2 d of LL (Thompson et al., 2008), preventing animals from adopting different phases at the time of testing due to free running under prolonged LL (which will result in an internal period of longer than 24 h; Aschoff, 1960). Moreover, LL was used rather than constant dark (DD) because visuospatial recognition requires visual input (Tam et al., 2016) and could not be assessed in DD.
Control LL
A one-off period of LL was used as a control condition to investigate whether the effects observed under rLL were due to repeated alternation between LD and LL or just the prolonged exposure to light. Mice in this condition were exposed to a single 48 h period of LL, as shown in Figure 2A and behavioral testing was conducted at their subjective midday (CT 6 h ± 30 min) and subjective midnight (CT 18 h ± 30 min), defined by the LD transition before the onset of LL.
Apparatus and stimuli in recognition tasks
Arena
A 20 cm × 20 cm × 20 cm open-top arena made of transparent acrylic was used for recognition testing. To facilitate discrimination of the four corners, two distinct 20 cm × 20 cm wallpapers were attached to the outside of the arena (Fig. 1B). One of the wallpapers was a checkerboard pattern with alternating 4 cm × 4 cm black and white squares. Each square subtended a visual angle of 23° vertically and horizontally (assuming the mouse's eye at the center of the arena). The other wallpaper had a white symmetrical five-point star on a black background. The star shape was drawn within a notational circle with a diameter of 14 cm, which subtended a visual angle of 70°. Small pieces of hook-and-loop stickers (Rip 'n' Grip) were adhered to the floor of the arena and to the bottom of all objects so that objects could be affixed to the arena during behavioral testing. A web camera was positioned 37 cm above the center of the arena. The arena was placed inside a LTC equipped with white LEDs, which provided a light level of 120–150 lux at the center of the arena for all recognition tasks.
Objects
For object and visuospatial recognition trials, we used everyday objects (e.g., bottles, light bulbs, and paperweights; Fig. 1B, top row) that had a base area smaller than 7.0 cm × 7.0 cm and a height <9 cm. The objects to be discriminated on each object recognition trial differed in multiple sensory dimensions such as color, size, shape, and texture so they could be discriminated on the basis of visual cues, nonvisual cues, or both. A detailed description of some of the objects used in the current study can be found in Tam et al. (2016). There were at least four replicates of each object so that different replicates of the same object could be presented in sample and test phases, eliminating the possibility that, at test, the mouse simply recognized and ignored its own odor traces left on the object at preexposure.
Odors
For odor recognition trials, various essential oils, including lemon, vanilla, and peppermint extracts (Dr. Oetker), orange, chocolate, and rose extracts (Nielsen-Massey), as well as banana and jasmine essence (Double Seahorse), were used as olfactory stimuli. Shortly before recognition testing, 1 ml of each stimulus was delivered with a syringe into a clear shot glass (Fig. 1B, bottom row) with a base diameter of 4.6 cm and a height of 6.4 cm. There were eight identical shot glasses so that different shot glasses could be presented in the sample and test phases.
Behavioral procedure
Before recognition testing, all mice were kept under LD for at least 7 d and given 6 habituation trials in the empty arena over a 3 d period: 3 trials at ZT 6 and 3 trials at ZT 18. The aim of these trials was to minimize the effects of arousal associated with handling and novelty of the arena, which could otherwise interfere with object exploration and disrupt test performance. At ZT 6 and 18, the mouse was removed from its home cage and put into the arena. The time required to transfer the mouse from its home cage into the arena was 10–20 s only because the apparatus was set up in a separate LTC located right below the LTC where animals were housed; therefore, mice received relatively little handling before each trial. Mice were allowed to explore the empty arena for 10 min on each habituation trial. After the habituation phase, each mouse in LD or rLL was tested repeatedly in different variants of the spontaneous recognition task across diurnal or circadian cycles in different sequences (Table 1). In the control condition, in which there was only a single 48 h period of LL, each mouse was given the object, visuospatial, or odor task at CT 6 and 18.
Table 1.
Lighting condition | Cohort | Recognition task sequence |
---|---|---|
Condition 1: LD | 1A (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object × 4 → (2) object displacement × 2 → (3) object-in-place × 2 |
1B (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object × 4 → (2) object displacement × 2 → (3) object-in-place × 2 → (4) odor × 4 | |
1Ca (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object × 4 → (2) object displacement × 2 → (3) object-in-place × 2 | |
Condition 2: rLL | 2Ab (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object displacement × 2 → (2) object-in-place × 2 → (3) odor × 4 |
2Bc (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) odor × 4 → (2) object displacement × 2 → (3) object-in-place × 2 → (4) object × 4 | |
2Cb (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object displacement × 2 → (2) odor × 2 | |
Control LL | 3A (n = 16) | H × 6 → (1) object × 2 |
3B (n = 8) | H × 6 → (1) object-in-place × 2 | |
3C (n = 16) | H × 6 → (1) object × 2 (LD) → (2) odor × 2 (LL) |
All mice received 6 habituation trials (H) in the empty arena (3 trials at ZT 6 and 3 trials at ZT 18 under LD) before recognition testing. In each cohort, half of the recognition trials were given at ZT/CT 6 and the remaining trials were given at ZT/CT 18.
aSample–test delay was 24 h for cohort 1C; for all other cohorts, the sample–test delay was 5 min.
bCohorts 2A and 2C experienced one cycle of LD–LL alternation before recognition testing.
cCohort 2B experienced four cycles of LD–LL alternation before recognition testing.
Object recognition
The object recognition task consisted of a sample phase and a test phase separated in time by either a 5 min or 24 h delay for mice under LD. For mice under rLL and control LL, the sample–test delay was always 5 min. At ZT/CT 6 or 18, a mouse was removed from its home cage and put into the arena. Two identical replicates of an object, A1 and A2, were placed at two corners of the arena (top left and bottom left) and the animal was allowed to explore the objects and arena freely for 10 min. After the required delay, the animal was tested for 1 min in the same arena (which was cleaned with 50% ethanol). One replicate of A was replaced by a novel object, B, whereas the other replicate of A was replaced by a third replicate, A3, which had never been presented to the animal. The time of day at which the object recognition task was given was counterbalanced under each lighting condition. For example, half of the mice under LD were given the object recognition task at ZT 6, ZT 18, ZT 18, and ZT 6 (Subgroup 1, red stars in Fig. 1A, left), whereas for the remaining mice, the task was given at ZT 18, ZT 6, ZT 6, and ZT 18 (Subgroup 2, blue stars in Fig. 1A, left). The same counterbalancing procedure was applied in rLL and control LL. A different pair of objects was used on each of the four object recognition trials.
As each mouse was tested at both (subjective) midday and midnight, the identities of the novel and familiar objects were counterbalanced across the two different time points. For example, for animals given the object recognition task first at ZT 6 and then at ZT 18 (Subgroup 1, red stars in Fig. 1A, left), a wood block and a glass candle holder were used at ZT 6 and a plastic bottle and a spherical ornament were used at ZT 18. This arrangement was reversed for animals given the object recognition task first at ZT 18 and then at ZT 6 (Subgroup 2, blue stars in Fig. 1A, left), the wood block and glass candle holder at ZT 18 and the plastic bottle and spherical ornament at ZT 6. In addition, within each subgroup at a particular time of day, the identities of novel and familiar objects and their positions at test were counterbalanced to take into account any potential bias toward a particular object or a certain part of the arena. For example, for Subgroup 1, which were first tested at ZT 6 (or Subgroup 2, which were first tested at ZT 18), the wood block was assigned as the familiar stimulus at test and the glass candle holder as the novel stimulus for half of the mice; this arrangement was reversed for the remaining half of animals such that the candle holder was the familiar object and the wood block was the novel object at test. Furthermore, for half of the animals within each of these subgroups, the novel object was located at the top left corner of the arena at test, whereas for the remaining animals, it was located at the bottom left corner. The same counterbalancing procedure was applied in rLL and control LL.
Visuospatial recognition
Spatial task 1.
The object-in-place recognition task was similar to the object recognition task, except that two different objects were placed at two corners of the arena (top left and bottom right) during the sample phase and the mouse was allowed to explore the objects freely for 10 min. After a 5 min or 24 h delay, the animal was tested for 1 min with new replicates of one preexposed object (Fig. 1B, middle row). One replicate was placed at the same spatial position as in the sample phase, whereas the other replicate was at a different spatial position (with respect to the visual cues on the walls of the arena). Animals normally spend more time exploring the replicate at the different location than the replicate at the same location, indicating encoding and retrieval of object–place associations (Dix and Aggleton, 1999). All other aspects of the object-in-place task, including the counterbalancing procedures, were identical to the object recognition task.
Spatial task 2.
In the object displacement task, two identical replicates of an object were placed at two corners of the arena (e.g., top left and bottom left) and the mouse was allowed to explore freely for 10 min. After the required delay, the animal was tested for 1 min with two new replicates of the same object. One replicate was at the same location as in the sample phase, whereas the other replicate was displaced to a new location that was not occupied by any object in the sample phase. Like the object-in-place task, it is anticipated that animals would spend more time exploring the replicate at the new location (Dix and Aggleton, 1999). The counterbalancing procedures were identical to the object recognition task.
Odor recognition
The odor recognition task was identical to the object recognition task except that the sample–test delay was always 5 min and shot glasses with essential oils rather than objects were presented in sample and test phases (Fig. 1B, bottom row).
Automated tracking of object and olfactory exploration
All sample and test phases were recorded via a web camera positioned above the center of the arena. Automated tracking of exploratory activity was conducted with ANY-maze software (version 4.5; Stoelting; RRID:SCR_014289). The tracking protocol used was similar to that in previous studies (Tam et al., 2014, 2015, 2016). For each video file, the 20 cm × 20 cm floor of the arena was outlined in ANY-maze, and 2 7 cm × 7 cm notional zones were placed at the corners of the arena where the objects were located. The position of the mouse's head within the arena was determined on a second-by-second basis and the amount of time its head was inside each of the two notional zones was recorded for every minute of the sample and test phases.
Data treatment
Memory performance on each recognition trial was expressed as a ratio score (N − F)/(N + F), where N and F represent the amount of time spent exploring novel versus familiar stimuli in the object and odor recognition tasks or objects at novel versus familiar locations in the object-in-place and object displacement tasks. The higher the ratio score is above zero (which indicates no stimulus discrimination), the better their recognition memory performance. Scores from the same task were averaged for each animal, resulting in one mean score for ZT/CT 6 and one mean score for ZT/CT 18 for each recognition task.
Core body temperature assessment
To investigate whether mice would maintain rhythmicity and nocturnality under the abnormal lighting condition, a naive cohort of mice were implanted with telemetry devices and, after recovery, their core body temperature was recorded continuously across multiple diurnal and circadian cycles for 16 d under rLL, as shown in Figure 3. We measured body temperature rather than wheel-running or rest-activity rhythms because constant light suppresses general activity, making it difficult to assess underlying circadian rhythms (Jud et al., 2005).
Surgery and telemetry equipment
During surgery, the mouse was anesthetized with isoflurane (IsoFlo; Abbott Laboratories,; 3% induction and 0.7–1.5% maintenance), and a telemetry transmitter (PhysioTel F20-EET; Data Sciences International) with a volume of 1.9 cm3 and a weight of 3.9 g was implanted into the peritoneum. Analgesics including buprenorphine (Vetergesic; Sogeval; 0.01 mg kg−1) and meloxicam (Metacam; Boehringer Ingelheim; 0.5 mg kg−1) were administered subcutaneously before surgery and 0.5 ml of saline was administered subcutaneously immediately after surgery to prevent dehydration; a second dose of meloxicam (0.5 mg kg−1) was given the next day. During recovery under LD and, for the duration of the experiment, telemetry-implanted animals were housed individually in transparent plastic cages (length × width × height = 48 cm × 26.5 cm × 21 cm) with ad libitum access to food and water. The cages were placed inside an LTC identical to that in the behavioral experiment except that the interior was shielded with aluminum foil to reduce interference. A telemetry receiver (PhysioTel RPC-1; Data Sciences International) was placed under each cage. When the implanted transmitters were switched on, signals received were transmitted to a hub (Data Exchange Matrix; Data Sciences International) and subsequently (via a local area network) to data acquisition software installed on a computer.
Data acquisition and processing
Implanted transmitters were switched on 6 weeks after surgery. Data acquisition began 5 min before ZT 0 (the start of the light phase under LD). As in the behavioral experiment, the lighting condition was alternated between 2 d of LD and 2 d of LL (Fig. 3). Over a period of 16 d, core body temperature was recorded continuously with a temporal resolution of 10 s using the Dataquest ART system (Data Sciences International). For each 2 d period under LD or LL, the time series in 10 s bins was smoothed using a 10 min moving average and converted to Z scores: (temperature in each bin − mean temperature)/standard deviation. The normalized time series was then fitted with a sinusoidal function, Asin(ωt + ϕ), using package nls2 in R (version 3.1.1; R Core Team, 2015), which enables us to determine the amplitude (A), period length τ (2π/ω), and phase (ϕ) of the mouse's core body temperature rhythm.
Per1, Per2, and Fos gene expression
Brain tissue collection
A naive cohort of mice was housed either under LD or rLL conditions identical to that in the behavioral experiment (Fig. 1A). Brain tissue samples from the SCN, dorsal hippocampus, and olfactory bulb were collected at ZT 6 and 18 under LD and at CT 6 and 18 after four cycles of LD-LL alternation. At the required time of day, each mouse was removed from its home cage and killed by cervical dislocation under 100 lux of illumination, similar to that in the behavioral experiment. The eyes were removed immediately. The brain was removed from the skull, dipped in PBS to remove hair and blood, and then placed onto an ice-cold slicer matrix with 1 mm intervals (Zivic Instruments) with the ventral surface of the brain facing upward. Based on the Franklin and Paxinos (1997) mouse brain atlas, two skin graft blades (Swann-Morton) were placed at bregma −0.10 mm and −1.10 mm and SCN samples (located above the optic chiasm) were collected from this coronal section using a specimen punch with an internal diameter of 1 mm (Uni-Core; GE Healthcare). Two additional blades were placed at bregma −2.10 mm and −3.10 mm and hippocampal samples (including dorsal dentate gyrus, CA3, and CA1 subregions) were collected from these coronal sections. Finally, the entire olfactory bulb was collected from the brain. Samples were immediately frozen on dry ice and subsequently stored at −80°C before RNA extraction.
RNA extraction and real-time quantitative PCR (qPCR)
For each sample, total RNA was extracted and purified (RNeasy Mini Kit; QIAGEN) and RNA concentration was determined using the NanoDrop 1000 spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific). Total RNA was converted into cDNA (qScript Synthesis Kit; Quanta Biosciences) and qPCR was performed in a thermocycler (StepOne Plus; Applied Biosystems) using SYBR Green I (QuantiFast SYBR Green PCR Kit; QIAGEN). The primer sequences 5′-3′ for Per1 were AGTTCCTGACCAAGCCTCGTTAG (forward) and CCTGCCCTCTGCTTGTCATC (reverse); the sequences for Per2 were GGGGTGAGATTCGTCATTGAACTTG (forward) and AGGACATTGGCACACTGGAAAGAG (reverse); and the sequences for Fos were ATCGGCAGAAGGGGAAAGTAG (forward) and GCAACGCAGACTTCTCATCTTCAAG (reverse). The sequences for the housekeeping gene β-actin were ACCAACTGGGACGATATGGAGAAGA (forward) and CGCACGATTTCCCTCTCAGC (reverse); the sequences for the housekeeping gene β2M were GCCTTCACCCCAGAGAAAGG (forward) and GCGGTTGGGATTTACATGTTG (reverse); and the sequences for the housekeeping gene Gapdh were TGCACCACCAACTGCTTAG (forward) and GATGCAGGGATGATGTTC (reverse). Melting curve analyses were performed to verify the specificity and identity of PCR products. Any sample with more than one peak in the first-derivative plot of the melting curve (which indicates primer-dimers or contaminating DNA) was excluded from subsequent analyses.
Quantification of gene expression
Per1, Per2, and Fos mRNA levels were quantified using the comparative threshold cycle (CT) method described by Schmittgen and Livak (2008). The 2−CT values of Per1, Per2, and Fos in each sample were expressed as a ratio relative to the geometric mean of the 2−CT values of the housekeeping genes in the same sample to obtain 2−ΔCT. The 2−ΔCT values at ZT 6, CT 6, and CT 18 were then normalized to the mean 2−ΔCT value of all samples from the same brain region at ZT 18 to obtain 2−ΔΔCT, which indicates the fold change in gene expression relative to midnight. Therefore, a 2−ΔΔCT value >1 indicates that gene expression is elevated relative to ZT 18 under normal conditions, whereas a 2−ΔΔCT value <1 indicates that gene expression is attenuated relative to ZT 18.
Plasma corticosterone measurement
For a subgroup of mice from the gene expression experiment, we also collected blood samples and measured plasma corticosterone (CORT) levels at ZT 6 and 18 under LD and at CT 6 and 18 after four cycles of LD–LL alternation. The method of blood collection and CORT measurement are described in Pilorz et al. (2016). Briefly, trunk blood was obtained from the site of decapitation and collected into microcentrifuge tubes containing the anticoagulant ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (EDTA; Sigma-Aldrich), with 5 μl of EDTA for every 250 μl of blood collected. Blood samples were kept on ice and centrifuged within 15 min after collection; the plasma obtained was stored at −20°C. Before CORT measurement, plasma samples were diluted 10-fold and CORT levels were determined with the AssayMax Corticosterone ELISA Kit (Assaypro) according to the manufacturer's instruction manual. Optical absorbance was read at 450 and 570 nm on a microplate reader (FLUOstar Omega; BMG Labtech) and the reading at the latter wavelength was subtracted from the former to correct for any optical imperfection. The standard curve relating optical absorbance to CORT concentration (in nanograms per milliliter) was derived by fitting a four-parameter logistic function.
Statistical analyses
For recognition data collected under LD and rLL, two-way mixed ANOVAs were conducted on ratio scores and on total object exploration duration in each task, with lighting condition (LD or rLL) as a between-subjects factor and time of day (ZT/CT 6 or 18) as a within-subjects factor. For recognition data collected under control LL, a separate task (object, object-in-place, or odor) × time of day (CT 6 or 18) mixed ANOVA was conducted on ratio scores. Multiple one-sample t tests (two-tailed) were performed to compare mean recognition scores against the value of zero, which indicates no stimulus discrimination. For core body temperature data collected under rLL, block (days 1–4, 5–8, 9–12, or 13–16) × lighting condition (48 h LD or LL) within-subjects ANOVAs were conducted on amplitude (A), period length τ (2π/ω), and phase (ϕ), which were determined by nonlinear regression described above. For gene expression and plasma CORT data, lighting condition (LD or rLL) × time of day (ZT/CT 6 or 18) between-subjects ANOVAs were conducted on 2−ΔΔCT and CORT concentration values, respectively. In all analyses, we adopted α = 0.05 for statistical significance (unless otherwise specified). Statistical analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics (version 22; RRID:SCR_002865).
Results
Object memory performance is optimal at midday
We first investigated whether performance in the object recognition task varied at ZT 6 versus 18 under LD and if such a time-of-day effect would persist at CT 6 versus 18 under rLL. Object memory performance was better at ZT 6 under LD and this time-of-day effect persisted under rLL (Fig. 1B, top row). A lighting condition [LD (n = 32) or rLL (n = 8)] × time of day (ZT/CT 6 or 18) ANOVA conducted on object recognition scores revealed that performance was better at ZT/CT 6 than at ZT/CT 18, as indicated by the significant main effect of time of day (F(1,38) = 4.560, p < 0.05). However, the time-of-day effect on object performance did not differ between LD and rLL (main effect of lighting condition: p = 0.647; lighting condition × time of day interaction: p = 0.718). Under LD, the mean recognition scores at ZT 6 and 18 were both significantly different from the value of zero (both p < 0.0001), suggesting that mice were able to discriminate between novel and familiar objects at both times of day despite performance being better at ZT 6 than at ZT 18. Under rLL, the mean recognition score at CT 6 was significantly different from zero (p < 0.025), but the mean score at CT 18 was not different from chance (p = 0.196), further confirming that object recognition performance was optimal at subjective midday. Notably, the poorer performance at ZT/CT 18 was not a consequence of animals' reduced interest in object exploration per se. On the contrary, the total amount of time spent in object exploration in the sample phases was significantly higher at ZT/CT 18 (main effect of time of day: F(1,38) = 7.985, p < 0.01; Table 2), although no day/night difference in overall exploratory activity was found in the test phases (p = 0.353; Table 3).
Table 2.
Lighting condition | Time of day | Task |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Objecta |
Object-in-placea |
Object displacementa |
Odora |
||||||
Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | ||
Condition 1: LD | ZT 06 | 91.235 | 7.364 | 78.236 | 6.767 | 140.750 | 8.844 | 56.035 | 5.907 |
ZT 18 | 131.727 | 8.989 | 101.719 | 7.388 | 169.448 | 11.348 | 74.938 | 6.627 | |
Condition 2: rLL | CT 06 | 105.192 | 14.433 | 74.016 | 5.813 | 116.284 | 8.614 | 62.031 | 5.939 |
CT 18 | 124.230 | 9.935 | 79.204 | 5.073 | 124.478 | 8.989 | 76.099 | 6.092 | |
Control LL | CT 06 | 123.087 | 13.557 | 95.660 | 9.303 | — | — | 56.405 | 8.120 |
CT 18 | 132.898 | 11.587 | 101.521 | 7.614 | — | — | 79.565 | 10.156 |
aSignificant main effects of time of day (p < 0.05).
Table 3.
Lighting condition | Time of day | Task |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Object |
Object-in-place |
Object displacement |
Odor |
||||||
Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | ||
Condition 1: LD | ZT 06 | 30.984 | 2.051 | 21.756 | 3.077 | 31.556 | 2.554 | 18.219 | 2.202 |
ZT 18 | 31.631 | 2.627 | 20.338 | 2.610 | 27.819 | 2.495 | 19.931 | 3.167 | |
Condition 2: rLL | CT 06 | 16.988 | 2.271 | 18.513 | 1.639 | 24.908 | 1.578 | 14.217 | 1.827 |
CT 18 | 10.825 | 2.191 | 15.163 | 1.953 | 27.413 | 1.782 | 19.583 | 2.340 | |
Control LL | CT 06 | 24.644 | 3.364 | 29.913 | 4.954 | — | — | 6.775 | 1.502 |
CT 18 | 31.431 | 2.812 | 33.525 | 5.013 | — | — | 9.738 | 3.086 |
Visuospatial memory performance is optimal at midday
Spatial task 1: object-in-place task
Similar to the object recognition task, performance was better at ZT 6 in the object-in-place task under LD and this time-of-day effect persisted under rLL (Fig. 1B, middle row). A lighting condition [LD (n = 16)) or rLL (n = 16)] × time of day ANOVA conducted on spatial recognition scores revealed a main effect of time of day (F(1,30) = 4.261, p < 0.05), but there was no main effect of lighting condition (p = 0.749) or lighting condition × time of day interaction (p = 0.335). At ZT 6 under LD and CT 6 under rLL, mean recognition scores were significantly different from zero (both p < 0.005), but mean scores at ZT 18 and CT 18 were not (p = 0.109 and 0.400, respectively), indicating that mice did not discriminate between different spatial positions at the latter time of day. Again, the level of object exploratory activity in the sample phases was higher at ZT/CT 18 (main effect of time of day: F(1,30) = 6.323, p < 0.025; Table 2), but no day/night difference was found in the test phases (p = 0.244; Table 3).
Spatial task 2: object displacement task
In accordance with the findings from the object-in-place task, performance in the object displacement task was optimal at ZT/CT 6 (Table 4). As before, a lighting condition [LD (n = 16)) or rLL (n = 24)] × time of day ANOVA revealed a main effect of time of day (F(1,38) = 4.395, p < 0.05), which did not differ between LD and rLL (lighting condition × time of day interaction: p = 0.866). Object exploratory activity in the sample phases was again higher at ZT/CT 18 (main effect of time of day: F(1,38) = 5.820, p < 0.025; Table 2), but not in the test phases (p = 0.288; Table 3).
Table 4.
Lighting condition | Time of day | Ratio scorea |
|
---|---|---|---|
Mean | SEM | ||
Condition 1: LD | ZT 06 | 0.208 | 0.065 |
ZT 18 | 0.061 | 0.109 | |
Condition 2: rLL | CT 06 | 0.414 | 0.063 |
CT 18 | 0.241 | 0.087 |
Recognition memory performance was expressed as a ratio score, (N − F)/(N + F), where N and F represent objects at novel versus familiar spatial positions. The higher the score is above zero, the better is the spatial recognition performance.
aSignificant main effect of time of day (p < 0.025).
Short-term versus long-term memory performance
Evidence from invertebrate studies suggests that time-of-day effects on performance could vary depending on the delay interval between training and test (Fernandez et al., 2003; Lyons et al., 2005). To investigate whether this is the case for mice, we compared short-term (5 min sample–test delay) versus long-term memory performance (24 h sample–test delay) directly under LD. Similar time-of-day effects were found regardless of whether the test phase of each recognition trial was given 5 min or 24 h after the sample phase (Table 5). A three-way ANOVA was conducted, with sample–test delay [5 min (n = 16) or 24 h (n = 8)] as a between-subjects factor and task (object, object-in-place, or object displacement) and time of day (ZT 6 or 18) as within-subjects factors. There was a main effect of time of day as anticipated, with better performance at ZT 6 (F(1,22) = 6.095, p < 0.025); however, it did not interact with delay (delay × time of day interaction: p = 0.814; delay × task × time of day interaction: p = 0.397). Long-term memory performance appeared to be poorer than short-term memory performance (Table 5), although the main effect of delay was not statistically significant (p = 0.101). Consistent with these results, the mean recognition score (pooled across tasks) at ZT 6 after a 24 h delay was significantly different from zero (p = 0.05), but the mean score at ZT 18 was not (p = 0.551), further confirming that day/night differences in object and visuospatial recognition performance persisted when the sample–test delay was increased to 24 h.
Table 5.
Sample–test delay | Time of day | Task |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Objecta |
Object-in-placea |
Object displacementa |
|||||
Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | Mean | SEM | ||
5 min | ZT 06 | 0.451 | 0.076 | 0.317 | 0.093 | 0.208 | 0.065 |
ZT 18 | 0.258 | 0.083 | 0.193 | 0.113 | 0.061 | 0.109 | |
24 h | ZT 06 | 0.248 | 0.213 | 0.182 | 0.105 | 0.252 | 0.156 |
ZT 18 | 0.229 | 0.104 | −0.142 | 0.102 | 0.032 | 0.117 |
Recognition performance was expressed as a ratio score, (N − F)/(N + F), where N and F represent the amount of time spent exploring novel versus familiar objects or objects at novel versus familiar spatial positions. The higher the score is above zero, the better the recognition performance.
aSignificant main effect of time of day (p < 0.05).
Olfactory memory performance varies depending on lighting conditions
Strikingly, in contrast to object and visuospatial performance, the time-of-day effect in the odor recognition task varied between lighting conditions: Olfactory memory performance was optimal at ZT 6 under LD, but was optimal at CT 18 under rLL (Fig. 1B, bottom row). An ANOVA conducted on odor recognition scores, with lighting condition [LD (n = 8) or rLL (n = 24)] as a between-subjects factor and time of day as a within-subjects factor, showed that there was a significant interaction (F(1,30) = 11.738, p < 0.0025). This interactive effect was due to the fact that, under LD, odor recognition performance was optimal at ZT 6 (simple main effect of time of day: F(1,7) = 7.162, p < 0.05), similar to object and visuospatial performance. In contrast, under rLL, the time-of-day effect on performance was inverted. Olfactory performance was optimal at CT 18 (simple main effect of time of day: F(1,23) = 10.604, p < 0.005). Furthermore, olfactory performance at CT 18 under rLL was significantly enhanced relative to ZT 18 under LD (simple main effect of lighting condition: F(1,30) = 12.938, p < 0.005). Consistent with these results, the mean odor recognition score at ZT 6 under LD was significantly different from zero (p < 0.025), but mean at ZT 18 was not (p = 0.719); however, under rLL, the mean score at CT 18 was different from zero (p < 0.0001), but the mean at CT 6 was not (p = 0.223). Interestingly, this phase shift in olfactory performance was not accompanied by any shift in exploratory activity. Under both LD and rLL, odor exploratory activity in the sample phases was higher at ZT/CT 18, as indicated by the significant main effect of time of day (F(1,30) = 9.585, p < 0.005; Table 2), which did not interact with lighting condition (p = 0.653). No day/night difference in exploratory activity was found in the test phases (p = 0.171; Table 3).
Forty-eight-hour LL does not alter the circadian phase of olfactory memory performance
To investigate whether the phase shift in olfactory performance was due to repeated alternation between LD and LL or prolonged exposure to light, a cohort of naive mice were given only a single 48 h period of light as a control condition (Fig. 2A). In contrast to the results observed under rLL, in the control condition, performance in the object task (n = 16), the object-in-place task (n = 8), and the odor task (n = 16) was consistently better at CT 6 (main effect of time of day: F(1,37) = 4.935, p < 0.05), similar to that under LD. Crucially, this day/night difference in performance did not vary across the three tasks (task × time of day interaction: p = 0.370; Fig. 2B). This indicates that a one-off, 48 h exposure to light was not sufficient to induce a complete reversal of olfactory performance at CT 6 and 18. However, there was a main effect of task (F(2,37) = 3.471, p < 0.05) due to the fact that performance in the odor task was generally poorer than performance in the object task in these animals (Bonferroni post hoc comparison, p < 0.05; Fig. 2B).
Circadian rhythms are maintained under rLL
To confirm that mice maintained nocturnality as well as rhythmicity under rLL, core body temperature was recorded continuously across multiple diurnal and circadian cycles in five telemetry-implanted mice. An example of the core body temperature rhythm is shown in Figure 3A; mean amplitudes (A), period lengths τ (2π/ω), and phase changes (Δϕ) across days are displayed in Figure 3, B–D, respectively. Visual inspection of Figure 3A suggests that mice maintained rhythmicity and nocturnality under rLL, with a trough and a peak during the (subjective) day and night, respectively. Furthermore, the internal period (the duration from one peak to the next) was lengthened under LL (Aschoff, 1960). These observations were confirmed by ANOVAs, which showed that mean amplitudes did not vary significantly across days (Fig. 3B). However, as predicted by Aschoff's rule, the mean τ was longer under LL (main effect of lighting condition: F(1,4) = 25.039, p < 0.01; Fig. 3C); in addition, it was significantly longer than 24 h (p < 0.005). This increase in τ under LL led to a significant phase delay across days (main effect of block: F(3,12) = 14.466, p < 0.0005; block × lighting condition interaction: F(3,12) = 97.294, p < 0.0005; Fig. 3D). Nevertheless, these slight changes in circadian rhythms (i.e., period lengthening and phase shifting of <1.5 h) do not explain the complete reversal of the time-of-day effect on olfactory performance under rLL.
Gene expression in the SCN, dorsal hippocampus, and olfactory bulb is differentially affected by rLL
To understand how the abnormal lighting condition would affect molecular rhythms in different brain regions, we measured mRNA expression of two canonical clock genes, Per1 and Per2, which regulate behavioral rhythms (Bae et al., 2001; Reppert and Weaver, 2002). In addition, we looked at mRNA expression of the immediate-early gene Fos, a marker of neuronal activity. Mean Per1, Per2, and Fos mRNA levels in different regions under LD versus rLL (after four cycles of LD–LL alternation) are displayed in Figure 4.
SCN
We found that Per1 mRNA levels in the SCN were higher at ZT 6 than at ZT 18 under LD and higher at CT 6 than at CT 18 under rLL (main effect of time of day: F(1,20) = 5.023, p < 0.05; Fig. 4A). Moreover, Per1 mRNA expression was generally lower under rLL relative to LD (main effect of lighting condition: F(1,20) = 10.997, p < 0.005), consistent with previous findings that prolonged periods of LL dampen circadian rhythms in the SCN (Beaulé et al., 2003; Sudo et al., 2003; Granados-Fuentes et al., 2004; Coomans et al., 2013). However, the lighting condition × time of day interaction was not significant (p = 0.469), suggesting that rLL dampened, but did not completely abolish, the rhythmic expression of Per1 in the SCN. Similarly, Fos mRNA expression was higher at ZT 6 than at ZT 18 under LD and this day/night difference was attenuated under rLL (Fig. 4C), although these effects were only marginally significant (main effect of time of day: F(1,20) = 4.065, p = 0.057; lighting condition × time of day interaction: F(1,20) = 4.088, p = 0.057).
Dorsal hippocampus
In contrast to the weakening of molecular rhythms in the SCN, rLL did not dampen Per1, Per2, or Fos mRNA levels in the dorsal hippocampus, a region that (together with adjacent and interconnected cortical areas) contributes to object and visuospatial recognition memory processes (Aggleton et al., 2012; Cohen et al., 2013). Hippocampal gene expression was elevated at ZT 6 and CT 6, as indicated by significant main effects of time of day (Per1: F(1,47) = 6.004, p < 0.025; Per2: F(1,49) = 6.115, p < 0.025; Fos: F(1,19) = 9.356, p < 0.01; Fig. 4D–F, respectively). However, there was no main effect of lighting condition (Per1, p = 0.269; Per2, p = 0.358; Fos, p = 0.545) or lighting condition × time of day interaction (Per1, p = 0.724; Per2, p = 0.869; Fos, p = 0.940), indicating that hippocampal gene expression was unaffected under rLL. These molecular data mirror the behavioral observations that object and visuospatial recognition performance were unaffected under rLL.
Olfactory bulb
Strikingly, Per1 mRNA expression patterns in the olfactory bulb were phase shifted under rLL, as indicated by a significant lighting condition × time of day interaction (F(1,48) = 4.733, p < 0.05). This is due to the fact that there was a significant decrease in Per1 mRNA levels at CT 18 under rLL relative to ZT 18 under LD (simple main effect of lighting condition: F(1,27) = 8.349, p < 0.01; Fig. 4G). A similar phase shift was found for Fos mRNA expression under rLL (lighting condition × time of day interaction: F(1,18) = 4.419, p = 0.05; Fig. 4I). These changes in Per1 and Fos mRNA expression correspond to the behavioral observation that odor recognition performance was phase shifted under rLL.
Plasma corticosterone levels are reduced following rLL
Prolonged exposure to LL is known to reduce the level of arousal in mice, typically assessed by measuring plasma CORT (Fonken et al., 2010; Coomans et al., 2013). To investigate whether rLL would reduce the level of CORT as in previous studies, in a subgroup of mice from the gene expression experiment, we collected blood samples and measured plasma CORT levels at ZT 6 and 18 under LD and at CT 6 and 18 after 4 cycles of LD–LL alternation. Consistent with previous studies, the mean CORT concentration under rLL (4.047 ± 0.637 ng/ml) was significantly lower than the mean under LD (7.274 ± 1.551 ng/ml) (main effect of lighting condition: F(1,16) = 7.910, p < 0.025). CORT concentrations were slightly higher at (subjective) night, as expected in nocturnal rodents (ZT 6 vs ZT 18: 6.442 ± 2.628 ng/ml and 8.105 ± 2.628 ng/ml, respectively; CT 6 vs CT 18: 3.745 ± 0.898 ng/ml and 4.350 ± 0.970 ng/ml, respectively). However, these day/night differences were not statistically significant (main effect of time of day: p = 0.127; lighting condition × time of day interaction: p = 0.590).
Discussion
Here, we demonstrate that there are diurnal rhythms in recognition memory for objects, places, and odors, with performance consistently better at ZT 6 regardless of the delay interval between sample and test phases. Strikingly, these rhythms in performance become desynchronized under rLL. Object and visuospatial performance remains better at CT 6, but olfactory performance becomes better at CT 18. This desynchrony in behavioral performance is mirrored by differential changes in Per1, Per2, and Fos mRNA expression in the SCN, dorsal hippocampus, and olfactory bulb. Temporal desynchrony among these regions does not result in arrhythmicity because core body temperature and exploratory activity rhythms persist under rLL. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration that abnormal lighting conditions can give rise to internal desynchrony at the level of circadian oscillators in different brain regions and at the level of behavioral performance in object, visuospatial, and odor recognition tasks without inducing arrhythmicity or disrupting memory performance (Ruby et al., 2008, 2013; Fernandez et al., 2014).
Our finding that memory performance is generally better at ZT/CT 6 is consistent with previous studies, which reported optimal performance in the light phase in aversively motivated and appetitively motivated tasks (Chaudhury and Colwell, 2002; Rawashdeh et al., 2014; but see Valentinuzzi et al., 2001; Gritton et al., 2009, 2012, for different results). So why is recognition performance generally better at ZT/CT 6? A potential factor that could have contributed to our behavioral results is arousal—either the absolute level or differential change in arousal—induced by handling or removal from the home cage, resulting in differential levels of performance at ZT/CT 6 and 18. Nevertheless, because the form of the arousal–performance relationship often follows an inverted-U function (Yerkes and Dodson, 1908; Broadhurst, 1957; Diamond et al., 2007), it is unclear whether the day/night difference in performance was caused by a facilitative effect at ZT/CT 6 due to a moderate change in arousal from a previously inactive state, a disruptive effect at ZT/CT 18 due to a further elevation in the absolute level of arousal from a previously mildly active state, or both. This issue can only be resolved in future studies in which physiological measures of arousal (such as heart rate) are assessed before and during behavioral testing at ZT/CT 6 and 18.
Another factor that could have contributed to our behavioral results is the amount of sleep, which is regulated by circadian cycles as well as preceding sleep–wake history (Borbély et al., 2016). Importantly, sleep–wake history between sample and test phases is known to be a crucial determinant of performance, especially when the sample–test delay is relatively long (1.5–24 h). For example, forced wakefulness manipulations immediately after object preexposure, such as handling (Palchykova et al., 2006; Inostroza et al., 2013) and stimulation of arousal-related pathways (Rolls et al., 2011), are known to disrupt long-term recognition performance, whereas sleep after object preexposure facilitates visuospatial performance (Binder et al., 2012). These findings are consistent with the general idea that posttraining sleep may be important for reactivation of memory traces (Buzsáki, 1998; Ribeiro and Nicolelis, 2004; Rasch and Born, 2013). Nevertheless, although the role of sleep in memory consolidation is potentially relevant to our results on trials with a 24 h sample–test delay, it is unlikely to be relevant to our results on trials with a 5 min delay because sleep-dependent memory consolidation would not be necessary for successful short-term stimulus recognition (Squire et al., 2015).
Alternatively, the day/night difference in short-term memory performance may be partly related to sleep-driven synaptic homeostasis (Cirelli and Tononi, 2000; Tononi and Cirelli, 2003), leading to differences in synaptic weights in the relevant brain circuitries involved in recognition memory processes. One possible factor is the diurnal rhythm in GluA1-containing AMPA receptors in the postsynaptic density, which reflects an animal's preceding sleep–wake history (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). Notably, GluA1-mediated synaptic plasticity may, at least in part, underlie the short-term recognition memory processes that we have investigated in the present study because GluA1 deletion selectively disrupts short-term object, spatial, and olfactory memory performance while sparing long-term memory (Sanderson et al., 2011; Sanderson and Bannerman, 2012; Ang, 2016). However, the exact relationship between diurnal variation in synaptic GluA1 levels driven by preceding sleep–wake history and subsequent memory performance is not known. Future studies in which sleep electroencephalogram and synaptic GluA1 levels are assessed can help to confirm the effects of preceding sleep–wake history on GluA1-dependent recognition memory performance. Nevertheless, synaptic homeostasis per se is not sufficient to account for the fact that rLL desynchronizes daily rhythms in olfactory versus object and visuospatial performance.
The decoupling of behavioral performance suggests that synaptic changes required for these recognition processes are regulated by distinct circadian oscillators (Herzog, 2007; Frank, 2016). More specifically, under LD, Per1, Per2, and Fos are all rhythmically expressed in the dorsal hippocampus, with higher expression at ZT 6, highly similar to the phasing of gene expression in the SCN as in previous studies (Gilhooley et al., 2011; Reale et al., 2013; Loh et al., 2015; Ma et al., 2016; but see Wang et al., 2009; Jilg et al., 2010; Harbour et al., 2014, for different results). These genes are also rhythmically expressed in the olfactory bulb, but are in antiphase to the gene expression profile in the SCN (Abe et al., 2002; Abraham et al., 2005; Granados-Fuentes et al., 2006). Crucially, under rLL, Per1 and Fos rhythms in the SCN are dampened, although not completely abolished, consistent with previous findings that SCN rhythms are compromised by prolonged periods of LL (Beaulé et al., 2003; Sudo et al., 2003; Granados-Fuentes et al., 2004; Coomans et al., 2013). In contrast, under rLL, hippocampal gene expression is unaffected, whereas Per1 and Fos expression in the olfactory bulb is phase shifted.
These data are consistent with previous findings that the olfactory bulb expresses autonomous and entrainable circadian rhythms, which persist independently of the SCN (Abe et al., 2002; Granados-Fuentes et al., 2004; Abraham et al., 2005; Granados-Fuentes et al., 2006; Guilding and Piggins, 2007). Circadian variation in olfactory discrimination performance persists in SCN-lesioned mice that exhibit arrhythmicity in wheel-running activity; however, the circadian phase of optimal olfactory discrimination is shifted relative to control animals (Granados-Fuentes et al., 2011). Our data complement and extend these observations by showing that abnormal lighting conditions can suppress the influence of the SCN, giving rise to internal desynchrony at the level of circadian oscillators in different brain regions and at the level of behavioral performance in different sensory domains.
We cannot rule out the possibility that changes in the circadian phase of olfactory processing under rLL could be the result of altered feeding behavior because prolonged LL can induce a phase shift in feeding behavior (Fonken et al., 2010), potentially leading to phase shifts in Per1 and Per2 rhythms in food-entrainable oscillators among different extra-SCN regions (Wakamatsu et al., 2001) and changes in behavioral performance (Loh et al., 2015). However, in these studies, altered feeding was also accompanied by arrhythmicity (Fonken et al., 2010; Loh et al., 2015) and phase shifts in hippocampal Per1 and Per2 rhythms (Wakamatsu et al., 2001). Notably, neither of these effects was observed in our study, suggesting that it is unlikely that feeding behavior was altered by rLL.
Our results can be understood in terms of the hierarchical organization of the circadian system, with the SCN playing a key role in maintaining entrainment of peripheral oscillators (Dibner et al., 2010). Under LD, peripheral clocks in the olfactory bulb and hippocampus are coupled to the SCN or its downstream signals such as circulating glucocorticoids (which may synchronize peripheral oscillators via mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors; Balsalobre et al., 2000). This results in optimal object, visuospatial, and olfactory recognition performance at ZT 6 (Fig. 5A). However, under rLL, molecular rhythms in the SCN and its downstream signals (e.g., CORT) are dampened, weakening the coupling between the SCN and extra-SCN regions. The olfactory bulb becomes autonomous from the SCN, leading to a shift in the circadian phase of olfactory processing (Fig. 5B). In contrast, the oscillator in the hippocampus, and presumably oscillators in adjacent and interconnected cortical areas that also contribute to object and visuospatial processing, may depend upon signals from the SCN to drive performance. It should be noted that molecular oscillations in the hippocampus may also be autonomous under certain conditions, although previous studies were inconclusive (Abe et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2009; Ma et al., 2016).
What are the adaptive advantages of multiple dissociable oscillators? In nature, where resources and predators are encountered at varying times of day, anticipating the temporal regularities of these different events is essential to survival. Multiple clocks that adopt different phase relationships with one another may enable processes occurring in different regions to be optimized to specific phases of the 24 h cycle (Antle and Silver, 2009). Moreover, such a dispersed network of circadian oscillators may provide greater flexibility when faced with conflicting environmental signals.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (Grant 098461/Z/12/Z to R.G.F., S.N.P., and M.W.H.) and the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (Grant BB/I021086/1 to S.N.P. and R.G.F.). D.M.B. was supported by Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship 087736.
References
- Abe M, Herzog ED, Yamazaki S, Straume M, Tei H, Sakaki Y, Menaker M, block GD (2002) Circadian rhythms in isolated brain regions. J Neurosci 22:350–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abraham U, Prior JL, Granados-Fuentes D, Piwnica-Worms DR, Herzog ED (2005) Independent circadian oscillations of Period1 in specific brain areas in vivo and in vitro. J Neurosci 25:8620–8626. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2225-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Brown MW, Albasser MM (2012) Contrasting brain activity patterns for item recognition memory and associative recognition memory: Insights from immediate-early gene functional imaging. Neuropsychologia 50:3141–3155. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albrecht U, Sun ZS, Eichele G, Lee CC (1997) A differential response of two putative mammalian circadian regulators, mper1 and mper2, to light. Cell 91:1055–1064. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80495-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ang G. (2016) Investigating sleep and circadian rhythm disruptions in models of schizophrenia. Doctoral thesis, University of Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Antle MC, Silver R (2009) Neural basis of timing and anticipatory behaviors. Eur J Neurosci 30:1643–1649. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06959.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aschoff J. (1960) Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 25:11–28. 10.1101/SQB.1960.025.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bae K, Jin X, Maywood ES, Hastings MH, Reppert SM, Weaver DR (2001) Differential functions of mPer1, mPer2, and mPer3 in the SCN circadian clock. Neuron 30:525–536. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00302-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balsalobre A, Brown SA, Marcacci L, Tronche F, Kellendonk C, Reichardt HM, Schütz G, Schibler U (2000) Resetting of circadian time in peripheral tissues by glucocorticoid signaling. Science 289:2344–2347. 10.1126/science.289.5488.2344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaulé C, Houle LM, Amir S (2003) Expression profiles of PER2 immunoreactivity within the shell and core regions of the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus: Lack of effect of photic entrainment and disruption by constant light. J Mol Neurosci 21:133–147. 10.1385/JMN:21:2:133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevins RA, Besheer J (2006) object recognition in rats and mice: A one-trial non-matching-to-sample learning task to study 'recognition memory'. Nat Protoc 1:1306–1311. 10.1038/nprot.2006.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binder S, Baier PC, Mölle M, Inostroza M, Born J, Marshall L (2012) Sleep enhances memory consolidation in the hippocampus-dependent object–place recognition task in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 97:213–219. 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz-Justice A, Deboer T (2016) The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res 25:131–143. 10.1111/jsr.12371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent NJ, Gaskin S, Squire LR, Clark RE (2010) object recognition memory and the rodent hippocampus. Learn Mem 17:5–11. 10.1101/lm.1650110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadhurst PL. (1957) Emotionality and the Yerkes-Dodson law. J Exp Psychol 54:345–352. 10.1037/h0049114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown MW, Warburton EC, Aggleton JP (2010) Recognition memory: material, processes, and substrates. Hippocampus 20:1228–1244. 10.1002/hipo.20858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G. (1998) Memory consolidation during sleep: A neurophysiological perspective. J Sleep Res 7:17–23. 10.1046/j.1365-2869.7.s1.3.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaudhury D, Colwell CS (2002) Circadian modulation of learning and memory in fear-conditioned mice. Behav Brain Res 133:95–108. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00471-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cirelli C, Tononi G (2000) Gene expression in the brain across the sleep-waking cycle. Brain Res 885:303–321. 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03008-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR (2000) Impaired recognition memory in rats after damage to the hippocampus. J Neurosci 20:8853–8860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen SJ, Munchow AH, Rios LM, Zhang G, Asgeirsdóttir HN, Stackman RW Jr (2013) The rodent hippocampus is essential for nonspatial object memory. Curr Biol 23:1685–1690. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coomans CP, van den Berg SA, Houben T, van Klinken JB, van den Berg R, Pronk AC, Havekes LM, Romijn JA, van Dijk KW, Biermasz NR, Meijer JH (2013) Detrimental effects of constant light exposure and high-fat diet on circadian energy metabolism and insulin sensitivity. FASEB J 27:1721–1732. 10.1096/fj.12-210898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond DM, Campbell AM, Park CR, Halonen J, Zoladz PR (2007) The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: A synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Neural Plast 2007:60803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dibner C, Schibler U, Albrecht U (2010) The mammalian circadian timing system: Organization and coordination of central and peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol 72:517–549. 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dix SL, Aggleton JP (1999) Extending the spontaneous preference test of recognition: evidence of object-location and object-context recognition. Behav Brain Res 99:191–200. 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00079-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennaceur A, Delacour J (1988) A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav Brain Res 31:47–59. 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez F, Lu D, Ha P, Costacurta P, Chavez R, Heller HC, Ruby NF (2014) Dysrhythmia in the suprachiasmatic nucleus inhibits memory processing. Science 346:854–857. 10.1126/science.1259652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez RI, Lyons LC, Levenson J, Khabour O, Eskin A (2003) Circadian modulation of long-term sensitization in Aplysia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:14415–14420. 10.1073/pnas.2336172100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher SP, Godinho SI, Pothecary CA, Hankins MW, Foster RG, Peirson SN (2012) Rapid assessment of sleep–wake behavior in mice. J Biol Rhythms 27:48–58. 10.1177/0748730411431550 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonken LK, Workman JL, Walton JC, Weil ZM, Morris JS, Haim A, Nelson RJ (2010) Light at night increases body mass by shifting the time of food intake. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:18664–18669. 10.1073/pnas.1008734107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MG. (2016) Circadian regulation of synaptic plasticity. Biology (Basel) 5: pii: E31. 10.3390/biology5030031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G (1997) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic. [Google Scholar]
- Gilhooley MJ, Pinnock SB, Herbert J (2011) Rhythmic expression of per1 in the dentate gyrus is suppressed by corticosterone: implications for neurogenesis. Neurosci Lett 489:177–181. 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granados-Fuentes D, Prolo LM, Abraham U, Herzog ED (2004) The suprachiasmatic nucleus entrains, but does not sustain, circadian rhythmicity in the olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 24:615–619. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4002-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granados-Fuentes D, Tseng A, Herzog ED (2006) A circadian clock in the olfactory bulb controls olfactory responsivity. J Neurosci 26:12219–12225. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3445-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granados-Fuentes D, Ben-Josef G, Perry G, Wilson DA, Sullivan-Wilson A, Herzog ED (2011) Daily rhythms in olfactory discrimination depend on clock genes but not the suprachiasmatic nucleus. J Biol Rhythms 26:552–560. 10.1177/0748730411420247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gritton HJ, Sutton BC, Martinez V, Sarter M, Lee TM (2009) Interactions between cognition and circadian rhythms: Attentional demands modify circadian entrainment. Behav Neurosci 123:937–948. 10.1037/a0017128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gritton HJ, Kantorowski A, Sarter M, Lee TM (2012) Bidirectional interactions between circadian entrainment and cognitive performance. Learn Mem 19:126–141. 10.1101/lm.023499.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guilding C, Piggins HD (2007) Challenging the omnipotence of the suprachiasmatic timekeeper: Are circadian oscillators present throughout the mammalian brain? Eur J Neurosci 25:3195–3216. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05581.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harbour VL, Weigl Y, Robinson B, Amir S (2014) Phase differences in expression of circadian clock genes in the central nucleus of the amygdala, dentate gyrus, and suprachiasmatic nucleus in the rat. PLoS One 9:e103309. 10.1371/journal.pone.0103309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog ED. (2007) Neurons and network in daily rhythms. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:790–802. 10.1038/nrn2215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog ED, Tosini G (2001) The mammalian circadian clock shop. Semin Cell Dev Biol 12:295–303. 10.1006/scdb.2001.0257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inostroza M, Binder S, Born J (2013) Sleep-dependency of episodic-like memory consolidation in rats. Behav Brain Res 237:15–22. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jilg A, Lesny S, Peruzki N, Schwegler H, Selbach O, Dehghani F, Stehle JH (2010) Temporal dynamics of mouse hippocampal clock gene expression support memory processing. Hippocampus 20:377–388. 10.1002/hipo.20637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jud C, Schmutz I, Hampp G, Oster H, Albrecht U (2005) A guideline for analyzing circadian wheel-running behavior in rodents under different lighting conditions. Biol Proced Online 7:101–116. 10.1251/bpo109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kyriacou CP, Hastings MH (2010) Circadian clocks: Genes, sleep, and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 14:259–267. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loh DH, Jami SA, Flores RE, Truong D, Ghiani CA, O'Dell TJ, Colwell CS (2015) Misaligned feeding impairs memories. eLife 4: pii: e09460. 10.7554/eLife.09460 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons LC, Rawashdeh O, Katzoff A, Susswein AJ, Eskin A (2005) Circadian modulation of complex learning in diurnal and nocturnal Aplysia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:12589–12594. 10.1073/pnas.0503847102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma Z, Jiang W, Zhang EE (2016) Orexin signaling regulates both the hippocampal clock and the circadian oscillation of Alzheimer's disease-risk genes. Sci Rep 6:36035. 10.1038/srep36035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore RY, Eichler VB (1972) Loss of a circadian adrenal corticosterone rhythm following suprachiasmatic lesions in the rat. Brain Res 42:201–206. 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90054-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palchykova S, Winsky-Sommerer R, Meerlo P, Dürr R, Tobler I (2006) Sleep deprivation impairs object recognition in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 85:263–271. 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilorz V, Tam SK, Hughes S, Pothecary CA, Jagannath A, Hankins MW, Bannerman DM, Lightman SL, Vyazovskiy VV, Nolan PM, Foster RG, Peirson SN (2016) Melanopsin regulates both sleep-promoting and arousal-promoting responses to light. PLoS Biol 14:e1002482. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002482 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2015) A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Rasch B, Born J (2013) About sleep's role in memory. Physiol Rev 93:681–766. 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawashdeh O, Jilg A, Jedlicka P, Slawska J, Thomas L, Saade A, Schwarzacher SW, Stehle JH (2014) PERIOD1 coordinates hippocampal rhythms and memory processing with daytime. Hippocampus 24:712–723. 10.1002/hipo.22262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reale ME, Webb IC, Wang X, Baltazar RM, Coolen LM, Lehman MN (2013) The transcription factor Runx2 is under circadian control in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and functions in the control of rhythmic behavior. PLoS One 8:e54317. 10.1371/journal.pone.0054317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reppert SM, Weaver DR (2002) Coordination of circadian timing in mammals. Nature 418:935–941. 10.1038/nature00965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro S, Nicolelis MA (2004) Reverberation, storage, and postsynaptic propagation of memories during sleep. Learn Mem 11:686–696. 10.1101/lm.75604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls A, Colas D, Adamantidis A, Carter M, Lanre-Amos T, Heller HC, de Lecea L (2011) Optogenetic disruption of sleep continuity impairs memory consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:13305–13310. 10.1073/pnas.1015633108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruby NF, Hwang CE, Wessells C, Fernandez F, Zhang P, Sapolsky R, Heller HC (2008) Hippocampal-dependent learning requires a functional circadian system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:15593–15598. 10.1073/pnas.0808259105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruby NF, Fernandez F, Garrett A, Klima J, Zhang P, Sapolsky R, Heller HC (2013) Spatial memory and long-term object recognition are impaired by circadian arrhythmia and restored by the GABAA antagonist pentylenetetrazole. PLoS One 8:e72433. 10.1371/journal.pone.0072433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson DJ, Bannerman DM (2012) The role of habituation in hippocampus-dependent spatial working memory tasks: Evidence from GluA1 AMPA receptor subunit knockout mice. Hippocampus 22:981–994. 10.1002/hipo.20896 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson DJ, Hindley E, Smeaton E, Denny N, Taylor A, Barkus C, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Bannerman DM (2011) Deletion of the GluA1 AMPA receptor subunit impairs recency-dependent object recognition memory. Learn Mem 18:181–190. 10.1101/lm.2083411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmittgen TD, Livak KJ (2008) Analyzing real-time PCR data by the comparative CT method. Nat Protoc 3:1101–1108. 10.1038/nprot.2008.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR, Genzel L, Wixted JT, Morris RG (2015) Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 7:a021766. 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephan FK, Zucker I (1972) Circadian rhythms in drinking behavior and locomotor activity of rats are eliminated by hypothalamic lesions. Proc Natl Acad U S A 69:1583–1586. 10.1073/pnas.69.6.1583 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sudo M, Sasahara K, Moriya T, Akiyama M, Hamada T, Shibata S (2003) Constant light housing attenuates circadian rhythms of mPer2 mRNA and mPER2 protein expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of mice. Neuroscience 121:493–499. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00457-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam SKE, Robinson J, Jennings DJ, Bonardi C (2014) Dissociations in the effect of delay on object recognition: Evidence for an associative model of recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn 40:106–115. 10.1037/xan0000003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam SKE, Bonardi C, Robinson J (2015) Relative recency influences object-in-context memory. Behav Brain Res 281:250–257. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam SKE, Hasan S, Hughes S, Hankins MW, Foster RG, Bannerman DM, Peirson SN (2016) Modulation of recognition memory performance by light requires both melanopsin and classical photoreceptors. Proc Biol Sci 283: pii: 20162275. 10.1098/rspb.2016.2275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson S, Lupi D, Hankins MW, Peirson SN, Foster RG (2008) The effects of rod and cone loss on the photic regulation of locomotor activity and heart rate. Eur J Neurosci 28:724–729. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06388.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Cirelli C (2003) Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: A hypothesis. Brain Res Bull 62:143–150. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentinuzzi VS, Kolker DE, Vitaterna MH, Ferrari EAM, Takahashi JS, Turek FW (2001) Effect of circadian phase on context and cued fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice. Anim Learn Behav 29:133–142. 10.3758/BF03192822 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Pfister-Genskow M, Faraguna U, Tononi G (2008) Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat Neurosci 11:200–208. 10.1038/nn2035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakamatsu H, Yoshinobu Y, Aida R, Moriya T, Akiyama M, Shibata S (2001) Restricted-feeding-induced anticipatory activity rhythm is associated with a phase-shift of the expression of mPer1 and mPer2 mRNA in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus but not in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of mice. Eur J Neurosci 13:1190–1196. 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01483.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang LM, Dragich JM, Kudo T, Odom IH, Welsh DK, O'Dell TJ, Colwell CS (2009) Expression of the circadian clock gene Period2 in the hippocampus: Possible implications for synaptic plasticity and learned behaviour. ASN Neuro 1: pii: e00012. 10.1042/AN20090020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yerkes RM, Dodson JD (1908) The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18:459–482. 10.1002/cne.920180503 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yoo SH, Yamazaki S, Lowrey PL, Shimomura K, Ko CH, Buhr ED, Siepka SM, Hong HK, Oh WJ, Yoo OJ, Menaker M, Takahashi JS (2004) PERIOD2::LUCIFERASE real-time reporting of circadian dynamics reveals persistent circadian oscillations in mouse peripheral tissues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:5339–5346. 10.1073/pnas.0308709101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]