Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2017 Apr;56(4):344–351.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.007

Are Social and Communication Difficulties a Risk Factor for the Development of Social Anxiety?

Hannah Pickard a,, Fruhling Rijsdijk a, Francesca Happé a, William Mandy b
PMCID: PMC5376399  PMID: 28335879

Abstract

Objective

Social anxiety (SA) is a common condition associated with social and communication (SC) difficulties in typically developing young people, as well as those with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Whether SC difficulties place children at risk for developing SA is unclear. Using a longitudinal design, the present study aimed to disentangle the relationship between SA symptoms and SC difficulties using a population-based sample of 9,491 children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC).

Method

Parent-reported data on SC difficulties and SA symptoms were collected at ages 7, 10, and 13 years. A cross-lagged panel model was used to investigate the longitudinal stability and directional relationship between latent SC difficulties and SA constructs over time.

Results

More SC difficulties were associated with greater SA symptoms at all ages. Earlier SC difficulties predicted a small but significant amount of variance in later SA symptoms. The reverse relationship from SA to SC difficulties was not observed. The relationship from SC difficulties to SA was strongest from age 7 to 10 years. No sex differences were observed.

Conclusion

The evidence suggests that SC difficulties may be an important risk factor for the development of SA. These findings suggest the potential usefulness of incorporating social skills training alongside effective interventions to prevent or alleviate symptoms of SA in childhood.

Key words: ALSPAC, social anxiety, social and communication difficulties, autism spectrum disorders, longitudinal


Social anxiety (SA) disorder is characterized by an intense fear of social situations, which is often accompanied by the fear of being scrutinized by others.1 SA is often experienced during several social situations, including interacting with others, eating in public, or giving speeches. Anxiety-related fears are commonly driven by negative self-perceptions and a fear of being ridiculed by others, which can lead to increased social withdrawal and avoidance.2 SA is the third most common psychiatric disorder, with epidemiological research showing prevalence rates between 3% to 4% in childhood and 9% in adolescence.3, 4 The onset of SA is usually between late childhood and adolescence5; however, SA disorder can manifest in children at 7 or 8 years of age.6

SA is a dimensional trait that is continuously distributed throughout the general population. Subthreshold symptoms of SA are associated with adverse outcomes and an increased risk of developing SA disorder and additional comorbid disorders.7 Given the burden that subthreshold SA can have on an individual’s well-being, it is important that research investigates potential risk factors underpinning dimensionally measured SA traits in the general population.

SA and Social Communication Difficulties in Childhood

Etiological models of SA in childhood have implicated the role of several development risk factors, including behavioral inhibition, parent–child interactions, and peer relationships.8 In addition, social and communication (SC) difficulties, including problems in social behaviors, social cognition, and reciprocal social communication, are common among children with SA and have also been proposed as a risk factor. SC ability is a continuously distributed trait that extends throughout the general population,9 with those who experience severe difficulties often receiving a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by SC difficulties and restricted interests and repetitive behaviors. SA co-occurs highly in children with ASD (4.5−9.5 years) and high subthreshold ASD traits (10−15 years),10, 11 suggesting that those with greater SC difficulties may have a heightened risk of developing SA disorder. However, the developmental relationship between SC difficulties and SA is unclear. The present study aims to address whether an individual’s position on the continuum of SC traits influences their risk of later developing SA.

Cross-sectional research has supported the association between SC difficulties and SA. For example, typically developing children with SA disorder exhibited lower self and peer ratings of social competence during both laboratory and school-based social interaction tasks, compared to peers without anxiety.4, 12 Furthermore, using parent-report questionnaires, research has found that SC difficulties are higher among children with SA disorder compared to those with other anxiety disorders,13 suggesting a specific relationship between SC difficulties and SA. Although SC difficulties may not be universal in SA,14 the evidence suggests that for a significant subgroup in the general population, SC difficulties may underlie the development of SA.15

Intervention studies in children have informed our understanding of the relationship between SC difficulties and SA. Research has shown that children (age 8−12 years) with SA disorder who completed a Social Effectiveness Therapy (SET) program to enhance social skills and peer relationships showed increases in social skills and decreases in SA at posttreatment and after 6 months, compared to the control participants in a nonspecific intervention.16 It is evident that a relationship between SA and SC difficulties exists and that social skills training effectively reduces SA; however, we do not fully understand whether SC difficulties contribute to the development of SA. This research is important for identifying early warning signs on the developmental trajectory of SA.

In the ASD literature, cross-sectional studies have shown that social skill deficits and greater physiological arousal contributed toward elevated SA symptoms in adolescents with ASD.17 Contradictory research in children with ASD revealed that higher levels of SA predicted lower responsible and assertive social skills.18 Inconclusive findings regarding the directional relationship between SA and SC difficulties have led researchers to postulate a bidirectional relationship in ASD. It is suggested that SC difficulties may hinder social experiences, contributing to increased SA and social withdrawal, which subsequently impedes an individual’s SC ability.19 However, this relationship remains to be explored.

Research using population-based samples has supported the relationship between SA symptoms and SC difficulties. Population-based research allows the use of large samples to examine associations across the trait distribution. These findings can inform research in clinical populations. Using a population-based twin sample of children with ASD and their affected and unaffected cotwins, Hallett et al.11 found that increased SC difficulties and higher IQ were associated with greater parent-reported SA, supporting clinical findings. To date, no longitudinal work using a population-based sample has specifically assessed whether SC difficulties are a risk factor for SA, or whether SA reduces an individual’s SC ability.

We aim to examine the directional relationship between parent-reported SA symptoms and SC difficulties in a population-based sample of children at ages 7, 10, and 13 years. Sex differences will also be explored. Furthermore, the relationship between SA and SC difficulties will be examined while controlling for generalized anxiety, to test whether SC difficulties are related to SA-specific symptoms, compared to generalised anxiety. Based on previous research, we predict a directional and specific relationship between SC difficulties and SA, with early SC difficulties contributing to later SA symptoms.

Method

Sample

All participants were from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort, a population-based sample of children born in Bristol between 1991 and 1992. A total of 14,541 children were recruited into the original cohort, with 14,062 live births and 13,988 alive at 12 months.20 (The study website contains details of all of the data available through a fully searchable data dictionary at http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/.) Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and local research ethics committees. A total of 9,597 children had available data to test the study hypotheses at ages 7 (n = 8,148), 10 (n = 7,723), and 13 (n = 7,008) years. Following ALSPACs exclusion criteria for prorated scores, only children with 50% or more complete data on all measures of interest at all ages were included in the present study. Based on these exclusion criteria, 248 children (3%), 204 children (2.6%), and 226 children (3.2%) were excluded at ages 7, 10, and 13 years, respectively. Merging the three samples at ages 7 (n = 7,900), 10 (n = 7,519), and 13 (n = 6,782) years, the final sample included 9,491 children (4,654 female) with data at one, two, or three time points. This final sample was used in all further analyses (Table 1). Compared to the original ALSPAC cohort not included in the current analyses, young people in our final sample were more likely to have a mother who was a homeowner (odds ratio [OR] = 2.94, 95% CI = 2.72, 3.19) and had completed higher education (OR = 2.33, 95% CI = 2.13, 2.55).

Table 1.

Demographic Information for the Sample at 7, 10, and 13 Years of Age

Demographics Age 7 (n = 7,900) Age 10 (n = 7,519) Age 13 (n = 6,782)
Female % 49 50 50
Parental HE% (age 18+) 42 43 44
Owned/mortgaged home % 82 83 84
Ethnicity, white % 96 96 96
Full scale IQ, mean (SD) (Range: 45−151)a 105.30 (16.32) 105.24 (16.39) 105.70 (16.31)
Verbal IQ 108.14 (16.68) 108.08 (16.69) 108.51 (16.63)
Performance IQ 100.57 (16.94) 100.59 (17.03) 100.98 (16.93)

Note: IQ age 7 (n = 5,829), 10 (n = 5,761), and 13 (n = 5,307) years. HE = higher education; SD = standard deviation.

a

Full range of scores at all ages.

Measures

Socioeconomic Status

Socioeconomic status (SES) was captured using parental maternal education. Previous research in ALSPAC has reported that maternal education is a valid indicator of SES.21 At 32 weeks of gestation, mothers reported their current highest level of educational achievement from six possible responses: “none,” “CSE” (basic UK qualification), “vocational,” “O-level” (a prerequisite to further education), “A-levels,” and “degree or above.” Higher scores are indicative of better maternal education and thus higher SES.

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children−Third Edition

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children−Third Edition (WISC-III)22 is a measure of child IQ. In the present study, the abbreviated version of the WISC, including random items from 10 subtests, was administered during the clinical data collection wave at age 8 years. A total of 6,726 children (70.9%) from the final sample had a complete IQ measure at age 8 years.

Social and Communication Disorders Checklist

The Social and Communication Disorders Checklist (SCDC)23, 24 is a parent-reported questionnaire that measures social and communication (SC) difficulties related to ASD. The questionnaire consists of 12 items with a response scale ranging from 0 to 2 (“not true,” “quite or sometimes true,” “very often true”), which was designed to capture a child’s social behavior and functioning over the previous 6 months. A total score ranges from 0 to 24, with higher scores indicating greater SC difficulties. The SCDC shows high internal consistency (0.93), as well as good specificity (0.91) and sensitivity (0.88) when discriminating between individuals with and without ASD.23 Furthermore, research conducted in the ALSPAC cohort supports both the construct validity and reliability of the SCDC at measuring SC traits in the general population.24 In the ALSPAC sample, research has shown that the SCDC measures SC trait variability in the general population that overlaps with ASD in terms of genetic effects,25 supporting the SCDC’s validity as a measure of ASD-specific SC difficulties. The SCDC had excellent internal reliability (α = 0.81−0.89).

Development and Wellbeing Assessment

The Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA)26 questionnaire was administered as a parent-report questionnaire to capture child and adolescent psychopathology that corresponds with the International Classification of Diseases–10th Revision (ICD-10) and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—4th Edition (DSM-IV) criteria. The DAWBA has been tested and validated in large population samples.26 In the present research, SA symptoms were measured using the social fears (SF) subscale, and generalized anxiety was measured using the general anxiety (GA) subscale. The DAWBA-SF has six items in which parents report whether their child had experienced any specific SA symptoms over the last month: “no,” “a little,” “a lot,” and “hasn’t done this in last month.” Any parent responses of “hasn’t done this in last month” were excluded, as this response is not present in the original online DAWBA and is ambiguous in its answer to the six SF items. An SF total score (range 0−12) can be created by summing the responses over the six SA items, which was used in the present study. Higher scores on the DAWBA-SF indicate more severe SA symptoms. The DAWBA-SF showed good internal reliability (α = 0.79−0.81). The DAWBA-GA subscale consists of seven items in which parents report the frequency of their child worrying over the past 6 months: “no,” “sometimes,” and “often.” A GA total score (range 0−14) is computed by summing responses on all items, with higher scores indicating more generalized anxiety symptoms. The DAWBA-GA showed acceptable internal reliability (α = 0.53−0.72).

Data Analyses

Analyses were conducted in R, using the Lavaan package for structural equation modeling (SEM).27 The present study used a three-wave (time), two-level cross-lagged panel model to estimate relationships between SC difficulties and SA symptoms. The cross-lagged panel model incorporates the inherent time nature of longitudinal data and is frequently used to assess causal relationships in nonexperimental studies using panel data.28, 29

Confirmatory Factor Analyses

Three confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to assess the construct validity of the DAWBA-SF and SCDC at all ages. A two-factor structure was specified with a single factor for each scale: SC difficulties (SCDC) with 12 indicators, and SA (DAWBA-SF) with 6 indicators. Measures recommended for large datasets were used.30 Absolute fit measures included the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). For the SRMR, a value less than 0.08 indicates a good model fit, and for RMSEA, a value below 0.08 indicates an acceptable model fit, with values less than 0.05 indicating good model fit.31, 32 The comparative fit index (CFI) was also used, with values above 0.90 and 0.95 indicating acceptable to good model fit, respectively.32

Cross-Lagged Panel Model

The predicted relationships between SC difficulties and SA symptoms at ages 7, 10, and 13 years are depicted in Figure 1. The simultaneously solved paths are reported as partial regression coefficients: autoregressive paths estimate the stability of one trait over time; covariance paths estimate the correlation between two traits at each time point; cross-lagged paths estimate the predictive relationship of one variable on another at a later time point, independent of the stability and covariance paths.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cross-lagged panel model of social and communication difficulties (SC Diff) and social anxiety (SA) latent factors. Note: A = autoregressive paths, b = cross-lagged paths; c = covariance paths.

The relationships were tested between latent factors to capture more robust constructs free of measurement error. Each latent factor was specified within the model, with the 6 items from the DAWBA-SF loading on to the SA construct and the 12 items from the SCDC loading on to the SC difficulties construct (excluded from Figure 1 for simplicity). In the full model, latent factors were free to covary within time points. Latent factor item residuals were specified to covary between time points.

Model fit was tested with the Satorra–Bentler Scaled χ2 statistic,33 to compare χ2 when data are nonnormal. To test the study’s first hypothesis, model fit across 8 nested models were examined to assess the following: longitudinal stability of each latent variable; the relationship between the latent variables within time; and the stability of cross-lagged paths and difference in the cross-lagged paths at each time point (7→10, 10→13). Model fit was determined by the difference in fit statistics of the full model and a nested model in which equality constraints are applied to path estimates (e.g., a1 = a2 or a3 = a4 to assess stability of the autoregressive paths). To assess sex differences, likelihood ratio testing was conducted between a full model in which all paths were freely estimated across sex and one in which either all cross-lagged paths (b12, b21, b23, b32) or autoregressive paths (a1, a2, a3, a4) were equated across sex. A Bonferroni correction was applied to assess the significance of all path coefficients (p < .003). χ2 for model-fit differences were considered to be statistically significant at p < .006.

Specificity

To explore the specificity of the relationship between SC difficulties and SA, scores on the DAWBA-GA subscale (where available) were regressed out of the SA latent variable traits at ages 7, 10, and 13 years to create a more specific SA-related latent construct.

Results

All questionnaire data were cleaned using ALSPAC guidelines for data preparation. Tests of selective attrition for SC difficulties and SA symptoms were conducted, and acceptable results were observed (see Supplements 1 and 2, available online). Mean scores on the SCDC and DAWBA-SF are reported in Table 2. The full distribution of scores on the SCDC and DAWBA-SF scales are available online (see Table S1, available online).

Table 2.

Parent-Reported Child Characteristics on Questionnaire Data

Questionnaires Age 7 y (n = 7,900)
Age 10 y (n = 7,519)
Age 13 y (n = 6,782)
Mean (SD) [CI] Mean (SD) [CI] Mean (SD) [CI]
SCDC 2.8 (3.66) 2.37 (3.58) 2.52 (3.60)
(Range: 0−24)a [2.72, 2.88] [2.29, 2.45] [2.44, 2.61]
DAWBA-SF 0.88 (1.6) 0.98 (1.7) 1.26 (1.91)
(Range: 0−12)a [0.85, 0.92] [0.94, 1.02] [1.21, 1.30]

Note: DAWBA-SF = Development and Wellbeing Assessment–Social Fears; SCDC = Social Communication Disorders Checklist; SD = standard deviation.

a

Full range of scores at all ages.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Three two-factor models were specified to test the construct validity of the SCDC scale and DAWBA-SF subscale at ages 7, 10, and 13 years. Two out of three of the fit indices for the two-factor models at 7 years (RMSEA = 0.067 [0.065, 0.068], SRMR = 0.06, CFI = 0.83), 10 years (RMSEA = 0.067 [0.066, 0.069], SRMR = 0.06, CFI = 0.84), and 13 years (RMSEA = 0.073 [0.072, 0.074], SRMR = 0.06, CFI = 0.82) were indicative of a good/acceptable model fit. The results imply that the SCDC and DAWBA-SF are two distinct and separate constructs measuring SC difficulties and SA symptoms.

Latent Variable Correlations

A saturated model with no cross-lagged or stability paths was fitted to examine the correlations among all latent factors. Significant associations among all latent factors were observed (Table 3).

Table 3.

Correlation Coefficients Among All Latent Factors in the Saturated Model

R [CI]
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. SA7 1
2. SC Diff7 0.20∗∗∗ [0.17, 0.24] 1
3. SA10 0.54∗∗∗ [0.51, 0.58] 0.21∗∗∗ [0.18, 0.25] 1
4. SC Diff10 0.16∗∗∗ [0.12, 0.19] 0.74∗∗∗ [0.72, 0.77] 0.23∗∗∗ [0.19, 0.26] 1
5. SA13 0.40∗∗∗ [0.36, 0.44] 0.17∗∗∗ [0.14, 0.21] 0.52∗∗∗ [0.49, 0.56] 0.20∗∗∗ [0.16, 0.23] 1
6. SC Diff13 0.14∗∗∗ [0.11, 0.18] 0.61∗∗∗ [0.58, 0.65] 0.19∗∗∗ [0.16, 0.23] 0.71∗∗∗ [0.68, 0.74] 0.22∗∗∗ [0.19, 0.26] 1

Note: Subscript numbers show the age at assessment. SA = social anxiety symptoms; SC Diff = social and communicative difficulties.

∗∗∗

p < .001.

Cross-Lagged Panel Model Path Estimates

In the cross-lagged panel model, effects of IQ and SES were regressed out of the SA latent variable at ages 7 years (IQ: β = −0.18, SES: β = −0.02) 10 years (IQ: β = −0.11, SES: β = 0.01), and 13 years (IQ: β = −0.04, SES: β = 0.00). For the SC difficulties latent variable, the effects were at ages 7 years (IQ: β = −0.17, SES: β = 0.00), 10 years (IQ: β = −0.11, SES: β = −0.00), and 13 years (IQ: β = −0.04, SES: β = −0.00).

Covariance Paths

The covariance path estimates between SC difficulties and SA were significant at all ages (Figure 2). The covariance path weights steadily decreased over time; however, no significant decrease in model fit was observed when the covariance paths at ages 7 and 10 years (Δχ2[df] = 4.92[1], p = .03) and ages 10 and 13 years (Δχ2[df] = 0.12[1], p = .73) were constrained to be equal (see Table S2, available online).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cross-lagged panel model showing the relationship between social and communication difficulties (SC Diff) and social anxiety (SA) latent factors at 7, 10, and 13 years of age. Note: Standardized β coefficients with standard errors. All analyses controlled for IQ and socioeconomic status (SES). Significant paths are shown in bold. ∗p < .001.

Stability Paths

The autoregressive paths for SC difficulties (a1 and a2) and SA (a3 and a4) were significantly stable over time. However, the longitudinal stability of both SC difficulties and SA significantly decreased over time: SC difficulties (Δχ2[df] = 53.87[1], p = 2.15e-13) and SA symptoms (Δχ2[df] = 12.16[1], p = 4.89e-04).

Cross-Lagged Paths

The cross-lagged paths from SC difficulties to SA (b12 and b23) were both significant, but not significantly different in size (Δχ2[df] = 4.52[1], p = .03). The reverse cross-lagged paths from SA to SC difficulties (b21 and b32) were not significant. Subsequent analyses explored the difference in cross-lagged path weights at ages 7→10 years and 10→13 years. A significant difference in the cross-lagged paths from age 7→10 years was observed (Δχ2[df] = 13.04[1], p = 3.06e-04), with the path from SC difficulties to SA having a significantly greater contribution compared to the reverse cross-lagged path. No significant difference was seen for the cross-lagged paths from age 10→13 years (Δχ2[df] = 1.06[1], p = .30).

Sex Differences

No significant decrease in model fit was observed for a nested model constraining all cross-lagged paths to be equal across male and female participants (Δχ2[df] = 1.59[4], p =.81), compared to a full model, indicating no sex differences in the predictive relationship between SA and SC difficulties constructs at all ages. Analyses investigating sex differences in the longitudinal stability showed a significant difference in the autoregressive pathways for SC difficulties (Δχ2[df] = 22.68[2], p = 1.19e-05), with females showing less stability in SC difficulties compared to males. No sex differences were observed for the SA autoregressive paths (Δχ2[df] = 4.61[2], p = .10).

Specificity Analyses

Specificity analyses tested the relationship between SC difficulties and SA, while controlling for generalized anxiety (see Table S3, available online). The analyses revealed a pattern of results identical to that of the full cross-lagged panel model, showing both significant autoregressive paths and significant cross-lagged paths from SC difficulties to SA at ages 7→10 and 10→13 years. The reverse cross-lagged paths from SA to SC difficulties were not significant.

Discussion

We used a longitudinal design to investigate the relationship between SC difficulties and SA symptoms in a population-based cohort of children at ages 7, 10, and 13 years. We predicted that SC difficulties would contribute specifically to the development of SA symptoms in later childhood. We found that, first, more parent-reported SC difficulties were associated with heightened SA symptoms across all ages. Second, the data supported the construct validity of the SCDC and DAWBA-SF, suggesting that SA and SC difficulties are distinct domains across childhood. Third, extending previous research and supporting our predictions, we found a directional and asymmetrical relationship between SC difficulties and SA symptoms; earlier SC difficulties contributed toward the development of later SA symptoms, but not vice versa. In terms of this directional relationship, sex differences were not observed. Finally, SC difficulties predicted later SA symptoms while controlling for generalized anxiety, emphasizing that SC difficulties are a specific risk factor for SA. The interpretation of these results, clinical implications, limitations, and conclusions are discussed below.

In typically developing children, associations between clinical SA symptoms and poorer social skills have been reported.4, 12 Our results both support and extend previous findings by illustrating the stability of these relationships throughout childhood. In accordance with research reporting more SC difficulties and greater SA symptoms in individuals with ASD,11, 13 we found similar associations in a population-based sample of children. The magnitude of these associations, although only modest compared to results in clinical samples,11 mimic the findings from previous traitwise research examining parent-reported SC difficulties and SA symptoms.34 Our results may be indicative of the low levels of SA and SC difficulty scores in the present sample.

Previous intervention studies have supported the efficacy of social skills therapies for improving SC ability and having downstream benefits on SA.16 Building on this work, our study demonstrates that these SC difficulties not only co-occur with SA, but also appear to play a role in the development of SA across childhood. In addition, our novel longitudinal findings in a population-based sample suggest that SC difficulties are a risk factor for the development of SA across the trait distribution. These findings emphasize a potential marker for the development of SA that could be targeted with early prevention approaches.

Furthermore, our results are consistent with etiological theories proposing that SC difficulties may provoke negative reactions from others, which, through repeated experience, may result in increased SA.15 This is one possible mechanism through which SC difficulties may predispose to greater SA symptoms in childhood; however, there may be several alternative mechanisms, for example peer victimization, bullying, or social insight,35 that may contribute to the development of SA in those who exhibit severe SC difficulties. For example, in adolescents with ASD, self-reported peer victimization and bullying are associated with increased internalizing problems.36 It is possible that SC difficulties predispose to these additional risk factors or that they develop independent of social ability. Further research exploring the mediating mechanisms on the developmental pathway from SC difficulties to SA in childhood is warranted.

Interestingly, SC difficulties in earlier childhood made a greater contribution to SA symptoms, compared to the alternative cross-lagged path from age 7 to 10 years, suggesting that earlier SC problems heighten a child’s risk of developing SA. In support of this, our model results show that the strongest association between SC difficulties and SA was present at age 7 years. The greater impact of SC difficulties, particularly in earlier childhood, may explain the high prevalence rates of SA disorder in children with ASD,10 where SC difficulties arise earlier in development. Given these findings and that SA is common among children as young as 7 and 8 years of age,4 it is important for further research to investigate whether SC difficulties predict SA in younger children.

In late childhood, no difference in the strength of the cross-lagged paths was observed. The decrease in the difference of predictive magnitude at ages 10 to 13 years may reflect the influence of additional risk factors. These risk factors may be exacerbated by SC difficulties (e.g., bullying or peer neglect) or independent of these abilities (e.g., traumatic events), but contribute toward the development of SA. Alternatively, age-specific socioemotional and physical changes in late childhood/adolescence, including puberty and increased social pressures, may contribute to SA development. These developmental changes bring with it increased feelings of anxiety, stress, and social pressures, with more opportunities to misjudge social situations and experience social failure, a process that could be exacerbated by an individual’s social disability and insight into their ability.37 In fact, high-functioning adolescents with ASD who may have greater insight into their social ability exhibit greater SA symptoms.38 Coupled with SC difficulties, greater social insight may play an important role in the onset of SA in late childhood. Alternatively, a lack of social insight may be a protective factor against the development of SA, as these individuals may simply not care about what others think about them. Research exploring this process requires further investigation.

Our results revealed no sex differences in the pattern of directional relationships, despite epidemiological research reporting high prevalence rates of SA in females.39 Interestingly, although girls who experience severe SC difficulties come to clinical attention less often than boys,40 our results suggest that girls with SC difficulties are just as likely as boys to suffer the negative consequences of these impairments, in terms of SA. Further research examining sex differences in the risk factors for SA is warranted, as important sex differences may emerge.

The specificity analysis revealed that SC difficulties are a specific risk factor for childhood SA symptoms, over and above generalized anxiety. Supporting evidence shows that children with SA disorder exhibit greater parent-reported SC difficulties, compared to children with other anxiety and mood disorders.13 The combined evidence highlights the specificity of the relationship between SC difficulties and SA symptoms, compared to other forms of anxiety.

The present study was strengthened by its large sample size and the use of consistent measures throughout childhood. However, limitations should be considered. First, all analyses relied on parent-report measures, and future research would benefit from a multi-informant approach. Second, the DAWBA-SF scale fails to capture several physiological, cognitive, and behavioral symptoms and relies on six items, suggesting a lack of richness compared to other clinical measures. In addition, the DAWBA has a tendency to overdiagnose emotional disorders,41 so it is possible that SA levels are elevated in this sample. Novel and comprehensive measures of IQ, SA, and SC difficulties would be beneficial in future research. Finally, the cross-lagged model design is subject to many limitations,42 and the modest correlations between SC difficulties and SA suggest that additional factors may influence this relationship over time, an interesting avenue for future research.

In conclusion, our findings demonstrate that SC difficulties in early childhood contribute toward the development of later SA symptoms, but not vice versa. In light of these novel findings, clinical implications are considered. Previous research using social skill interventions based on cognitive-behavioral therapy approaches have been effective at both increasing social skills and decreasing symptoms of anxiety in adolescents with ASD.43 Building on this work, our results support the use of social skill programs alongside gold standard interventions in childhood, which offers the opportunity to develop SC skills while simultaneously improving symptoms of SA. Furthermore, given the focus of social cognition in our measure of SC difficulties, our findings imply that improving social cognition may be a key target for SA interventions. Using dual treatment programs that target social skills and potentially social cognition in childhood could be an effective route to alleviate symptoms of SA.

Footnotes

This article can be used to obtain continuing medical education (CME) at www.jaacap.com.

The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). This publication is the work of the authors, who will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. This research was specifically funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (PAD7125) and Autistica (PCFTAAR).

Information in this study was presented as a poster at the Neurodevelopmental Disorders Seminar Series, London, June 23, 2016.

Prof. Happé and Dr. Mandy are joint senior authors.

Ms. Pickard served as the statistical expert for this research.

The authors are extremely grateful to all of the families who took part in this study; the midwives for their help in recruiting them; and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.

Ms. Pickard would like to thank Joni Coleman and Laurie Hannigan for their help and guidance with learning R.

Disclosure: Dr. Rijsdijk, Prof. Happé, Dr. Mandy, and Ms. Pickard report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Supplement 1

Preliminary Analyses

In accordance with Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children documentation for handling data, all variables were recoded, and total scores were created using the following prorating system. Individuals with more than 50% missing data on the Development and Wellbeing Assessment—Social Fears subscale (DAWBA-SF; >4 items) and Social Communication Disorders Checklist (SCDC; >7 items) at 7, 10, and 13 years of age were excluded from the specific time point and were not included in further analyses. Data checks were performed to assess outliers, and none were identified. Normality checks using skewness and kurtosis values revealed that several total score variables and items were not normally distributed; nonparametric tests were used as necessary. In all structural equation modeling analyses, a robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator was used to account for the data’s deviations from normality. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to account for incomplete data missing at random (MAR). The final analytic sample consisted of data from 9,491 children at 7, 10, and 13 years of age.

Supplement 2

Selective Attrition

Mann−Whitney U tests were carried out to investigate the selective attrition of social anxiety (SA) symptoms and social and communication (SC) difficulties. Group score differences on SA symptoms and SC difficulties were compared between those at age 7 years who had available and unavailable data at age 13 years. No significant difference in the SA symptoms at age 7 was found for those who had available (Median [Md] = 0, SD = 1.56) and unavailable (Md = 0, SD = 1.71) SA symptom data at age 13 (W = 6521400, p = .50, d = 0.02). A small but significant difference in SC difficulties at age 7 for those who had available (Md = 2, SD = 3.50) and unavailable (Md = 2, SD = 4.21) data at age 13 (W = 6032100, p = 6.88e-04, d = 0.13) was observed, with more SC difficulties present in those at age 7 who had unavailable data at age 13.

Table S1.

Number of Children Who Fall Within the Distribution Bands of Scores on the Social Communication Disorders Checklist (SCDC) and Development and Wellbeing Assessment Social Fears Subscale (DAWBA-SF) at 7, 10, and 13 Years

Questionnaires Age 7 y
(n = 7,900)
Age 10 y
(n = 7,519)
Age 13 y
(n = 6,782)
n (%) n (%) n (%)
SCDC (Range: 0−24)a
 0−3 5,608 (71.0) 5,757 (76.6) 4,993 (73.6)
 4−7 1,534 (19.4) 1,125 (15.0) 1,171 (17.3)
 8−12 525 (6.6) 419 (5.6) 441 (6.5)
 13−18 158 (2.0) 152 (2.0) 124 (1.8)
 18−24 75 (0.9) 66 (0.9) 53 (0.8)
DAWBA-SF (Range: 0−12)a
 0−3 7,282 (92.2) 6,854 (91.2) 5,945 (87.7)
 4−7 558 (7.1) 590 (7.8) 742 (10.9)
 8−12 60 (0.8) 75 (1.0) 95 (1.4)

Note:

a

Full range of scores at all ages.

Table S2.

Cross-Lagged Panel Model Path Estimates Between Social and Communication (SC) Difficulties and Social Anxiety (SA) Latent Constructs and Fit Statistics for Constrained Model Paths Estimates

Model Paths B (SE) 95% CI β (SE) p Model Comparisons χ2 (df) AIC Δχ2 (df) p
Full Model 26604.13 (1408) 376308.86
Covariance Paths Covariance Paths
 c1 0.18 (0.02) 0.00e+00 c1 = c2 26613.72 (1409) 1017001.48 4.92 (1) .03
 c2 0.11 (0.02) 7.27e-07 c2 = c3 26604.36 (1409) 1016992.12 0.12 (1) .73
 c3 0.10 (0.02) 4.64e-07
Autoregressive Paths Autoregressive Paths
 a1 1.11 (0.04) [1.03, 1.20] 0.74 (0.01) <2e-16 a1 = a2 26793.46 (1409) 376496.20 53.87 (1) 2.15e-13
 a2 0.67 (0.03) [0.61, 0.73] 0.70 (0.02) <2e-16 a3 = a4 26629.72 (1409) 376332.45 12.16 (1) 4.89e-04
 a3 0.61 (0.03) [0.56, 0.67] 0.51 (0.02) <2e-16
 a4 0.48 (0.02) [0.44, 0.53] 0.49 (0.02) <2e-16
Cross-Lagged Paths Cross-Lagged Paths
 b12 0.11 (0.02) [0.07, 0.15] 0.09 (0.02) 1.00e-07 b12 = b23 26612.25 (1409) 376314.98 4.52 (1) .03
 b21 0.00 (0.02) [−0.03, 0.04] 0.00 (0.01) 8.29e-01 b21 = b32 26605.40 (1409) 376308.13 0.86 (1) .35
 b23 0.05 (0.01) [0.02, 0.08] 0.07 (0.02) 2.31e-04 b12 = b21 26624.15 (1409) 1017011.91 13.04 (1) 3.06e-04
 b32 0.03 (0.02) [−0.00, 0.06] 0.03 (0.01) 7.40e-02 b23 = b32 26605.66 (1409) 1016993.42 1.06 (1) .30

Note: Standardized β coefficients and unstandardized B coefficients are reported with standard errors (SE). 95% CIs are reported for standardized B coefficients. Scaled χ2 difference test statistic (Δχ2) using the Satorra–Bentler method. Maximum likelihood χ2 statistics are used. Exact path p values are reported. Bold p values are significant. All pathways are denoted by subscripts (see Figure 1). AIC = Akaike Information Criterion.

Table S3.

Specificity Cross-Lagged Panel Model Path Estimates Between Social and Communication Difficulties and Social Anxiety Latent Constructs With Generalized Anxiety Scores Regressed Out

Model Paths B (SE) 95% CI β (SE) p
Covariance Paths
 c1 0.13 (0.02) 1.39e-13
 c2 0.10 (0.02) 9.81e-06
 c3 0.09 (0.02) 1.26e-05
Autoregressive Paths
 a1 1.11 (0.04) [1.02, 1.2] 0.74 (0.01) <2e-16
 a2 0.67 (0.03) [0.61, 0.73] 0.70 (0.02) <2e-16
 a3 0.57 (0.03) [0.52, 0.63] 0.49 (0.02) <2e-16
 a4 0.47 (0.02) [0.43, 0.51] 0.48 (0.02) <2e-16
Cross-Lagged Paths
 b12 0.09 (0.02) [0.05, 0.13] 0.07 (0.02) 1.82e-05
 b21 0.01 (0.02) [−0.02, 0.05] 0.01 (0.01) 4.72e-01
 b23 0.04 (0.01) [0.02, 0.07] 0.06 (0.02) 1.72e-03
 b32 0.03 (0.02) [0.00, 0.06] 0.03 (0.01) 4.44e-02

Note: Standardized β coefficients and unstandardized B coefficients are reported with standard errors (SE). 95% CIs are reported for unstandardized B coefficients. Bold p values are significant. All pathways are denoted by subscripts (see Figure 1).

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association . American Psychiatric Association; Arlington, VA: 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Beidel D.C., Turner S.M., Young B.J., Ammerman R.T., Sallee F.R., Crosby L. Psychopathology of adolescent social phobia. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2007;29:46–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Burstein M., He J.P., Kattan G., Albano A.M., Avenevoli S., Merikangas K.R. Social phobia and subtypes in the National Comorbidity Survey−Adolescent Supplement: prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:870–880. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Beidel D.C., Turner S.M., Morris T.L. Psychopathology of childhood social phobia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38:643–650. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199906000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grant B.F., Hasin D.S., Blanco C. The epidemiology of social anxiety disorder in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66:1351–1361. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Costello E.J., Egger H.L., Angold A. The developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and comorbidity. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2005;14:631–648. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fehm L., Beesdo K., Jacobi F., Fiedler A. Social anxiety disorder above and below the diagnostic threshold: prevalence, comorbidity and impairment in the general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43:257–265. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0299-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Spence S.H., Rapee R.M. The etiology of social anxiety disorder: an evidence-based model. Behav Res Ther. 2016;86:50–67. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ronald A., Happé F., Price T.S., Baron-Cohen S., Plomin R. Phenotypic and genetic overlap between autistic traits at the extremes of the general population. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45:1206–1214. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000230165.54117.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Salazar F., Baird G., Chandler S. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in preschool and elementary school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:2283–2294. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2361-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hallett V., Ronald A., Colvert E. Exploring anxiety symptoms in a large-scale twin study of children with autism spectrum disorders, their co-twins and controls. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:1176–1185. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Spence S.H., Donovan C., Brechman-Toussaint M. Social skills, social outcomes, and cognitive features of childhood social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108:211–221. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.2.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Halls G., Cooper P.J., Creswell C. Social communication deficits: specific associations with social anxiety disorder. J Affect Disord. 2014;172:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cartwright-Hatton S., Tschernitz N., Gomersall H. Social anxiety in children: social skills deficit, or cognitive distortion? Behav Res Ther. 2005;43:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rapee R.M., Spence S.H. The etiology of social phobia: empirical evidence and an initial model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:737–767. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Beidel D.C., Turner S.M., Morris T.L. Behavioral treatment of childhood social phobia. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:1072–1080. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bellini S. The development of social anxiety in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2006;21:138–145. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chang Y., Quan J., Wood J. Effects of anxiety disorder severity on social functioning in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2012;24:235–245. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.White S.W., Schry A.R., Kreiser N.L. Social worries and difficulties: autism and/or social anxiety disorder? In: Davis T.E. III, White S.W., Ollendick T.H., editors. Handbook of Autism and Anxiety. Autism and Child Psychopathology Series. Springer; Cham, Switzerland: 2014. pp. 121–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Boyd A., Golding J., Macleod J. Cohort profile: the “children of the 90s”—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Aguinis H., Beaty J.C., Boik R.J., Pierce C.A. Effect size and power in assessing moderating effects of categorical variables using multiple regression: a 30-year review. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90:94–107. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wechsler D., Golombok J., Rust S. Psychological Corporation; Sidcup, UK: 1992. Manual for the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III) [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Skuse D.H., Mandy W.P., Scourfield J. Measuring autistic traits: heritability, reliability and validity of the Social and Communication Disorders Checklist. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:568–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Skuse D.H., Mandy W., Steer C. Social communication competence and functional adaptation in a general population of children: preliminary evidence for sex-by-verbal IQ differential risk. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:128–137. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819176b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.St Pourcain B., Skuse D.H., Mandy W.P. Variability in the common genetic architecture of social-communication spectrum phenotypes during childhood and adolescence. Mol Autism. 2014;5:18. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Goodman R., Ford T., Richards H., Gatward R., Meltzer H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:645–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:1–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jöreskog K. Statistical estimation of structural models in longitudinal developmental investigations. In: Nesselroade J.R., Baltes P.B., editors. Longitudinal Research in the Study of Behavior and Development. Illustrated. Academic Press; New York: 1979. pp. 129–169. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Berrington A., Smith P.W.F., Sturgis P. ESRC National Centre for Research Methods; Southampton, UK: 2006. An Overview of Methods for the Analysis of Panel Data. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hooper D., Coughlan J., Mullen M. Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electron J Bus Res Methods. 2008;6:53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.MacCallum R.C., Browne M.W., Sugawara H.M. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods. 1996;1:130–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hu L., Bentler P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equat Model. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Satorra A. Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In: Heijmans R.D.H., Pollock D.S.G., Satorra A., editors. Innovations in Multivariate Statistical Analysis. Springer US; Boston, MA: 2000. pp. 233–247. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hallett V., Ronald A., Rijsdijk F., Happé F. Disentangling the associations between autistic-like and internalizing traits: a community based twin study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40:815–827. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9596-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cappadocia M.C., Weiss J.A., Pepler D. Bullying experiences among children and youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:266–277. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1241-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Adams R.E., Fredstrom B.K., Duncan A.W., Holleb L.J., Bishop S.L. Using self- and parent-reports to test the association between peer victimization and internalizing symptoms in verbally fluent adolescents with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:861–872. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1938-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Scarpa A., White S.W., Attwood T. Guilford; New York: 2016. CBT for Children and Adolescents with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kuusikko S., Pollock-Wurman R., Jussila K. Social anxiety in high-functioning children and adolescents with autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1697–1709. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0555-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Beidel D.C., Turner S.M. 2nd ed. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2007. Shy Children, Phobic Adults. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dworzynski K., Ronald A., Bolton P., Happé F. How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:788–797. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Aebi M., Kuhn C., Metzke C.W., Stringaris A., Goodman R., Steinhausen H.-C. The use of the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA) in clinical practice: a randomized trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:559–567. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0293-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hamaker E.L., Kuiper R.M., Grasman R.P. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol Methods. 2015;20:102–116. doi: 10.1037/a0038889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.White S.W., Ollendick T., Albano A.M. Randomized controlled trial: multimodal anxiety and social skill intervention for adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:382–394. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1577-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES