Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Apr 4;7:45926. doi: 10.1038/srep45926

Effect of Vitamin D Replacement on Cognition in Multiple Sclerosis Patients

Hala Darwish 1,2, Ribal Haddad 2, Sahar Osman 2,*, Stephanie Ghassan 2,*, Bassem Yamout 2,3, Hani Tamim 4, Samia Khoury 2,3,a
PMCID: PMC5379671  PMID: 28374837

Abstract

Multiple Sclerosis is associated with deficient serum 25 hydroxyvitamin D (25 (OH)D) level and cognitive impairment. The aim of this study is to evaluate cognitive performance in MS patients with deficient 25 (OH)D (<25 ng/ml) compared to patients with sufficient levels (>35 ng/ml), then to evaluate the change in cognitive performance after 3 months of vitamin D3 oral replacement. Eighty-eight MS patients with relapsing remitting and clinically isolated type of MS, older than 18 years treated with interferon beta were enrolled. Cognitive testing was performed at baseline and at 3 months using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Stroop, Symbol Digit Modalities (SDMT) and Brief Visuospatial Memory Test (BVMT-R). Serum 25 (OH)D was measured at baseline and at the end of the study. Vitamin D3 replacement improved the MS patients’ cognitive performance after 3 months on the MoCA and BVMT-Delayed Recall (DR). Sufficient serum 25 (OH)D level predicted better cognitive performance on the BVMT-DR at baseline (β: 1.74, p: <0.008) and 3 months (β: 1.93, p: <0.01) after adjusting for all measured confounding variables. Vitamin D3 replacement could improve cognitive performance in MS patients and make a significant difference in the patient’s quality of life.


Multiple Sclerosis (MS) is a chronic inflammatory disease of the central nervous system that is linked to environmental factors such as smoking and vitamin D level. Low serum 25 hydroxyvitamin D (25 (OH) D) is associated with increased risk of MS1,2 and increased disease activity in patients with MS3,4. Vitamin D supplementation protects from disease in MS animal models5,6,7. Furthermore, Vitamin D levels appear to be lower in MS patients than controls8,9.

Cognitive impairment is a common symptom of MS that impacts quality of life, and affects up to 65% of patients10,11,12. The impairment affects a wide range of cognitive functions including verbal and non-verbal memory, and information processing speed12. Studies in humans provide support for a role of the vitamin D in cognitive functioning, and recent studies have linked low serum 25 (OH)D levels to cognitive dysfunction in adults13,14,15. Vitamin D receptors are expressed by neurons and glial cells16,17 supporting a role for vitamin D beyond bone homeostasis. Based on prevalence of hypovitaminosis D in our clinical population9, we hypothesized that low serum 25 (OH)D has a negative effect on cognitive performance. This study aim was to measure cognitive performance in patients with serum 25 (OH)D deficiency (<25 ng/ml) compared to patients with sufficient levels (>35 ng/ml), then evaluate the change in cognitive performance in 25 (OH)D deficient subjects after supplementation with oral vitamin D3 (Cholecalciferol, Euro-D 10000, Montreal, Quebec, Canada).

Results

A total of 111 participants were recruited, of whom 23 had 25 (OH)D levels between 25 and 35 ng/ml (inclusive) and were thus excluded from the study. Of the remaining 88 participants, 40 were males (45.4%) and the average age of all the participants was 36.3 ± 12.2 years. At baseline, there were 47 (53.4%) patients with sufficient and 41 (46.6%) with deficient 25 (OH)D. Of those, 84 participants underwent cognitive tests (39 in deficient group and 45 in sufficient group). After 3 months, 22 patients were lost to follow up; 66 had their serum 25 (OH)D level tested; 61 of them completed the study and underwent cognitive tests (31started in deficient group and 30 in sufficient group) (Fig. 1 and Table 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow Diagram of the subjects enrollment from recruitment through study completion.

Table 1. Sample characteristics per group.

    Sufficient 25 (OH)D N (%) Deficient 25 (OH)D N (%) P value
  N = 47 N = 41
Age Mean (±sd) 37.2 (±12.3) 35.3 (±12.3) 0.48
BMI Mean (±sd) 26.1 (±4.23) 24.8 (±2.9) 0. 11
Gender Male 20 (42.6%) 20 (48.8%) 0.56
Female 27 (57.4%) 21 (51.2%)
Living situation Alone 2 (5.4%) 3 (8.6%) 0.60
With others 35 (94.6%) 32 (91.4%)
Disease duration (years) Mean (±sd) 5.7 (±6.5) 4.8 (±6.4) 0.53
EDSS Mean (±sd) 1.1 (±0.9) 1.6 (±1.1) 0.04
Anxiety score Mean (±sd) 1.7 (±0.6) 2.1 (±0.7) 0.008
Depression score Mean (±sd) 1.9 (±0.7) 2.1 (±1.0) 0.18
Total Hopkins score Mean (±sd) 1.8 (±0.6) 2.1 (±0.8) 0.07
Calcium Visit 1 Mean (±sd) 9.7 (±0.4) 9.6 (±0.4) 0.66
Calcium Visit 2 Mean (±sd) 12.4 (±15.9) 9.7 (±0.4) 0.34
Education level Elementary 2 (5.3%) 3 (8.6%) 0.31
High school 8 (21.1%) 3 (8.6%)
College 28 (73.7%) 29 (82.9%)
Employment Employed for wages 21 (53.8%) 19 (54.3%) 0.28
Self-employed 6 (15.4%) 4 (11.4%)
Out of work for more than 1 year 3 (7.7%) 0 (0.0%)
Out of work for less than 1 year 0 (0.0%) 3 (8.6%)
Homemaker 5 (12.8%) 4 (11.4%)
Student 4 (10.3%) 4 (11.4%)
Retired 0 (0.0%) 1 (2.9%)
Lifestyle Physical activity No 14 (35.0%) 22 (61.1%) 0.02
Yes 26 (65.0%) 14 (38.9%)
Disability No 27 (93.1%) 23 (85.2%) 0.41
Yes 2 (6.9%) 4 (14.8%)
Smoking cigarettes No 24 (61.5%) 29 (80.6%) 0.07
Yes 15 (38.5%) 7 (19.4%)
smoking waterpipe No 25 (69.4%) 19 (65.5%) 0.74
Yes 11 (30.6%) 10 (34.5%)
smoking cigars No 32 (94.1%) 28 (96.6%) 1.00
Yes 2 (5.9%) 1 (3.45)
alcohol No 23 (60.5%) 27 (79.4%) 0.08
Yes 15 (39.5%) 7 (20.65)
Leisure and activities Read No 8 (50.0%) 10 (50.0%) 1.00
Yes 8 (50.0%) 10 (50.0%)
Watch TV No 7 (31.8%) 7 (35.0%) 0.83
Yes 15 (68.2%) 13 (65.0%)
Video games No 13 (76.5%) 13 (68.4%) 0.72
Yes 4 (23.5%) 6 (31.6%)
Board games No 14 (93.3%) 15 (88.2%) 1.00
Yes 1 (6.7%) 2 (11.8%)
Social media No 9 (47.4%) 10 (50.0%) 0.87
Yes 10 (52.6%) 10 (50.05)
Internet No 10 (55.6%) 8 (42.1%) 0.41
Yes 8 (44.4%) 11 (57.9%)
Volunteer No 30 (81.1%) 24 (68.6%) 0.22
Yes 7 (18.9%) 11 (31.4%)
Religious Religious 18 (50.0%) 19 (65.5%) 0.26
Spiritual 5 (13.9%) 1 (3.4%)
Neither 13 (36.1%) 9 (31.0%)
Score of leisure activities Meansd) 0.1520.189) 0.2430.271) 0.075
Sleep Hours of sleep/night 1–3 hours 0 (0.0%) 1 (2.9%) 0.57
4–7 hours 24 (63.2%) 22 (62.9%)
8+ hours 14 (36.8%) 12 (34.3%)
Days of sleep insufficiency 1–3 days 9 (25.7%) 8 (32.0%) 0.86
4–7 days 7 (20.0%) 6 (24.0%)
8–12 days 11 (31.4%) 6 (24.0%)
13+ days 7 (20.0%) 5 (20.0%)
None 1 (2.9%) 0 (0.0%)
Memory Memory loss/ confusion last year No 21 (72.4%) 14 (60.9%) 0.38
Yes 8 (27.6%) 9 (39.1%)
Yes 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%)

Serum 25 (OH)D

Mean 25 (OH)D serum level was 15.8 ± 6.5 ng/ml in the deficient group and 59.6 ± 24.5 ng/ml in 25 (OH)D sufficient group (P < 0.0001). After supplementation with high dose vitamin D3 for three months, 25 (OH)D level increased to 49.0 ± 14.6 ng/ml (p < 0.001 compared to baseline) in the deficient group, although it remained lower than the sufficient group that received usual care 64.2 ± 18.7 ng/ml (p < 0.001).

Despite weekly phone calls to remind participants to complete their diaries, only 30 of 88 (34%) participants completed it. There was no statistically significant difference between groups in terms of sun exposure (2.22 ± 2.08 vs. 2.16 ± 1.57 hours/week, p = 0.79) number of body parts exposed to the sun (1.29 ± 1.27 vs. 1.42 ± 0.84, p = 0.46), estimated weekly vitamin D intake from rich food sources; such as, fatty fish and egg yolks (653.17 ± 509.89 vs. 736.41 ± 582.09 IU/week, p = 0.676) and body mass index (26.1 ± 4.23 vs. 24.8 ± 2.9, p = 0.11).

Sample characteristics per group

There were no significant differences between the two groups in age, gender, disease duration, and other demographic factors (Table 1). However, patients with deficient 25 (OH)D level had higher EDSS scores (1.6 ± 1.1 vs. 1.1 ± 0.9, p = 0.04) and reported significantly less engagement in physical activity than those with sufficient 25 (OH)D (38.9% vs. 65%, p = 0.02), and were involved in more leisure activities such as reading, watching TV and playing board games than those with sufficient 25 (OH)D; but the difference did not reach statistical significance (0.243 ± 0.271 vs. 0.152 ± 0.189, p = 0.07). The anxiety score was significantly lower in subjects with sufficient 25 (OH)D than the 25 (OH)D deficient group (1.7 ± 0.6 vs. 2.1 ± 0.7, p = 0.008). Total depression and anxiety score tended to be higher in the deficient 25 (OH)D compared to sufficient group; however, this was not statistically significant (2.1 ± 0.7 vs. 1.8 ± 0.6, p = 0.07).

Cognitive performance after vitamin D3 replacement per group

The cognitive performance of the group with deficient 25 (OH)D group improved significantly on the, BVMT delayed recall (p = 0.02) and the MoCA (p = 0.006) after 3 months of vitamin D3 replacement; but not on the SDMT. A composite score of the BVMT learning trials (T1–T3) was computed; both groups showed improvement on the total BVMT trials that was statistically significant (p = 0.003 for the sufficient; and p = 0.004 for the deficient). Moreover, both groups showed improvement on the Stroop test; however, it was not statistically significant (p = 0.56) (Table 2). The SDMT scores adjusted for age and years of education of the majority of participants regardless of their 25 (OH)D level was within normal limits [100% of the deficient 25 (OH)D and 94.3% of the sufficient group] at baseline. Similarly, the majority of the sample showed no interference effect on Stroop test indicating intact attention [69.2% of the deficient 25 (OH)D and 60% of the sufficient group].

Table 2. Paired t-tests by cognitive test per group.

  Sufficient 25 (OH)D group N=29
Deficient 25 (OH)D group N=31
Mean (±sd) 95% (CI) P value Mean (±sd) 95% (CI) P value
STROOP (Baseline) 3.66 (±7.86) (−7.21, 2.4) 0.06 5.6 (9.2) (−16.55, 2.6) 0.56
STROOP (3 months) 7.1 (±8.95) 6.6 (11.5)
SDMT (Baseline) 59.4 (±13.2) (−4.51, 2.43) 0.53 53.0 (16.9) (−12.3, 12.8) 0.11
SDMT (3 months) 58.4 (±14.6) 50.2 (12.3)
BVMT Trial 1 (Baseline) 5.3 (±3.2) (−2.30, −2.4) 0.02 4.7 (3.0) (−1.77, −1.6) 0.02
BVMT Trial 1 (3 months) 6.6 (±2.6) 5.7 (3.2)
BVMT Trial 2 (Baseline) 7.8 (±3.4) (−2.18, −0.5) 0.003 7.0 (3.8) (−1.91, 0.10) 0.08
BVMT Trial 2 (3 months) 9.2 (±2.3) 7.9 (3.8)
BVMT Trial 3 (Baseline) 9.0 (±3.4) (−1.5, 0.05) 0.07 7.7 (4.1) (−2.17,3.4) 0.009
BVMT Trial 3 (3 months) 9.7 (±2.3) 8.9 (3.5)
BVMT-Trials Composite Score (Baseline) 22.10 (± 9.28) (−5.52, −1.23) 0.003 19.41 (±10.12) (−5.17, −1.08) 0.004
BVMT-Trials Composite Score (3 months) 25.48 (±6.6) 22.54 (± 9.75)
BVMT Delayed Recall (Baseline) 9.4 (±3.0) (−1.07, 0.10) 0.11 7.5 (3.8) (−1.84, 0.16) 0.02
BVMT Delayed Recall (3 months) 9.9 (±2.6) 8.5 (3.8)
MOCA (Baseline) 26.1 (±2.9) (−1.47, 0.98) 0.08 24.5 (3.8) (−2.7, −5.10) 0.006
MOCA (3 months) 26.8 (±2.8) 26.1 (3.8)

MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory test. CI = Confidence Interval.

Multivariate analysis

To further explore the vitamin D3 replacement effect on cognitive performance, after adjusting for other confounding variables such as age and years of education, a multivariate analysis was performed for all outcome variables at baseline and at three months for the: Stroop, MoCA, SDMT and BVMT-R (T1, T2, T3, Total score and DR). Dependent variables included in the model were: baseline 25 (OH)D status (sufficient vs. deficient), at 3 months 25 (OH)D status (sufficient vs. deficient), disease duration, EDSS, age, education (college or high school), physical activity, smoking, alcohol, score of leisure activities, anxiety score, depression score, and total Hopkins score. The results showed that baseline 25 (OH)D status was a significant predictor of performance on the BVMT delayed recall both at baseline and at three months after controlling for all other variables. Those with deficient 25 (OH)D level at baseline were found to have 1.74 (95% CI [−3.01, 0.47], p = 0.008) and 1.93 (95% CI [−3.42, −0.44], p = 0.01) points less on BVMT-DR compared to those with sufficient level, at baseline and at 3 months respectively. It was also positively correlated with immediate recall at three months on the BVMT-T2 (10 seconds immediate recall) (95% CI [−3.18, −0.2], p = 0.03), but not significant for the total BVMT trials 1–3 score at baseline, and at 3 months (Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3. Multivariate analysis for the predictors of the 7 cognitive performance tests at baseline (stepwise method).
Predictors STROOP
R2 Unstandardized Beta 95% CI P value
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.20 2.4 −1.39: 6.19 0.21
Education: Elementary   REF REF REF
Education: College   12.1 4.83: 19.32 0.001
Education high school   9.58 1.09: 18.08 0.03
Age   0.18 0.33:0.03 0.02
SDMT
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.47 −4.03 −9.08: 1.02 0.12
Age   0.63 0.83:0.43 <0.0001
Anxiety score   5.58 9.60:1.56 0.007
Alcohol   6.16 0.72: 11.60 0.03
BVMT T1
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.30 −0.6 −1.68: 0.57 0.33
Age   0.11 0.16:0.06 <0.0001
Education: college   2.02 0.68: 3.36 0.004
BVMT T2
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.34 −0.74 −2.01: 0.53 0.25
Age   0.16 0.21:0.11 <0.0001
Score leisure activities   2.75 0.06: 5.43 0.05
BVMT T3
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.23 −0.73 −2.15: 0.69 0.31
Age   0.14 0.20:0.09 <0.0001
BVMT-Total Score
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.248 −2.88 −7.23: 1.462 .189
EDSS   −3.23 −5.37:−1.09 0.004
BVMT DR
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.31 1.74 3.01:0.47 0.008
Age   0.14 0.20:0.09 <0.0001
MOCA
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.16 0.86 2.27: 0.56 0.23
Age   0.08 0.14:0.02 0.006
Anxiety score   1.17 2.27:0.07 0.04

Variables entered in all models are: 25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N), Disease duration, EDSS, Age, Education: college, Education: high school, Physical activity, smoking, alcohol, score leisure activities, Anxiety score at baseline, Depression score at baseline, Total Hopkins score at baseline. Abbreviations: MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory test.

Table 4. Multivariate analysis for the predictors of the 7 cognitive performance tests at 3 months (stepwise method).
Predictors STROOP
R2 Unstandardized Beta 95% CI P value
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.26 2.06 −3.05: 7.17 0.42
Anxiety score 7.44 11.46:3.42 <0.0001
Age 0.24 0.45:0.02 <0.0001
SDMT
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.39 −5.79 −12.52: 0.94 0.09
Education: college 14.54 6.18: 22.90 0.001
Age 0.35 0.65:0.05 0.02
Alcohol 7.92 0.51: 15.32 0.04
BVMT T1
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.21 0.01 −1.43: 1.45 0.99
Alcohol 1.83 0.26: 3.41 0.02
EDSS 0.99 1.85:0.14 0.02
BVMT T2
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.27 1.69 3.18:0.20 0.03
Score leisure activities 4.90 1.89: 7.92 0.002
Age 0.10 0.17:0.04 0.002
BVMT T3
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.23 −0.73 −2.15: 0.69 0.31
Age 0.14 0.20:0.09 <0.0001
BVMT-Total Score
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.48 −0.17 −4.57: 4.22 .936
EDSS −2.55 −5.01:−0.093 0.042
Education: College 4.377 0.658: 8.09  
Score leisure activities 9.36 1.09: 17.637 0.022
Alcohol 4.78 0.63: 9.49 0.047
MOCA
25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N) 0.31 −0.71 −2.22: 0.81 0.36
Education: College 3.90 2.05: 5.76  < 0.0001
EDSS 0.91 1.79:0.03 0.04

Variables entered in all models are: 25 (OH)D (Sufficient Y/N), Disease duration, EDSS, Age, Education: college, Education: high school, Physical activity, smoking, alcohol, score leisure activities, Anxiety score at baseline, Depression score at baseline, Total Hopkins score at baseline. Abbreviations: MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory test.

As expected, age was inversely correlated (at baseline and after 3 months) with the Stroop, SDMT, BVMT-T3 and BVMT-DR. Age also correlated inversely at baseline with MoCA and BVMT-T1 (Tables 3 and 4). The anxiety score was inversely correlated with Stroop at the second, SDMT and MoCA at baseline (Tables 3 and 4). EDSS was negatively correlated with the total BVMT score at baseline and follow up, and with the MoCA, and BVMT-T1 at the follow up visit. High school and college education were positively correlated at baseline with Stroop, and college education with the total BVMT score at baseline and at 3 months. College education was also positively correlated with the BVMT-T1, and correlated positively after three months with SDMT and MoCA (Tables 3 and 4). Leisure score correlated positively with BVMT-T2 at both visits; whereas, it correlated positively with the BVMT-total Score and DR at the second visit. Alcohol intake positively correlated with the BVMT-total score at 3 months and with the SDMT at both visits; and BVMT-T1 at the second visit (Tables 3 and 4).

Discussion

There is a known association between 25 (OH)D levels and cognitive performance15,18,19 in older adults, but the relationship between 25 (OH)D and cognition in MS patients has not been explored. In this report, we show that a relatively short course of oral supplementation with vitamin D3 in MS patients deficient in serum 25 (OH)D, improved memory as measured by the BVMT Delayed Recall and MoCA (general memory test that includes long term as well as visuo-spatial memory). Moreover, a level of 25 (OH)D >35 ng/ml was a predictor of a better cognitive performance on the BVMT-DR after adjusting for all measured confounding variables.

The Stroop and SDMT are sensitive to processing speed and attention and can be impaired in MS; studies showed significant SDMT changes over 5–10 years and mostly in progressive MS subtypes20,21,22. Not surprisingly given their short disease duration, the participants in this study had normal performance of the STROOP and SDMT. As processing speed is predictive of performance on visuospatial learning and memory tests23, this may explain our findings of improvement on the BVMT-T1-3 total score in both groups at 3-months (practice effect). In this study we uncovered an interesting uncoupling between performance on immediate and delayed recall in the subjects with 25 (OH)D deficiency. Given that delayed recall is a more demanding process this could indicate that the effect of 25 (OH)D is more important for long-term, demanding, memory. Interestingly, hippocampal neurons (necessary for delayed retrieval processes) express vitamin D receptors17.

EDSS and low physical activity were significantly higher in the low 25 (OH)D group. With a greater disease severity as measured by EDSS, which places a great emphasis on mobility and walking, participants are less likely to exit their homes and get sun exposure. Moreover, low 25 (OH)D levels correlate with greater disability and relapse rate in multiple sclerosis7. Further, subjects in our study who engaged in non–physical leisure activities were more likely to have lower 25 (OH)D as well; possibly as a consequence of a higher EDSS score and decreased physical activity, leading them to stay inside and engage in board games, reading, or watching television, among others.

Similarly, anxiety scores were significantly higher in the low 25 (OH)D group compared to the sufficient 25 (OH)D group. This is consistent with previous studies in rats and humans that demonstrated that 25 (OH)D plays an integral role in anxiety centers in the brain24,25.

The main limitation of this study is the short time frame. Three months was chosen to assess the earliest possible effect of vitamin D3 replacement on cognitive performance in MS; however, three months may not have been sufficient for a true improvement in cognition to be observed between groups. Several previous studies have shown delayed improvement in cognition with stroke26. A study with donezepil demonstrated improvement in cognition after 24 weeks27,28 while most studies of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis patients found improvement at 6 months29. Moreover, further studies to assess the effect of vitamin D3 replacement on subjects with higher EDSS scores and longer disease duration are also needed. It would be important to test whether long-term vitamin D3 supplementation along with other cognitive rehabilitation interventions can reverse cognitive deficits.

Another possible limitation of this study is the poor response rate to lifestyle diary, not allowing true control for other sources of vitamin D as possible confounders, such as sun exposure and dietary vitamin D. However, there was no difference between groups in terms of sun exposure, vitamin D intake and body mass index among the 30 patients who completed their diaries.

In summary, our study suggests a positive effect of vitamin D3 supplementation on cognitive function in MS. Thus, exploring the longitudinal effect (at least 1 year) of vitamin D3 replacement on cognitive performance in a larger sample of MS subjects with 25 (OH)D deficiency is being planned; while placing more emphasis on additional memory components; such as, verbal memory. Improving cognition early in the course of the disease with a simple intervention such as vitamin D3 supplementation could make a significant difference in the patient’s quality of life.

Methods

Participants and Study design

This prospective study was conducted at the MS center of the American University of Beirut Medical Center over a span of two years (2012–2014). A sample size of 88 was calculated for the primary outcome of immune marker changes, the results of this study will be reported separately. A Post-hoc analysis on the BVMT-DR results on the available subjects showed a power of 0.74.

Eighty-Eight participants MS patients were diagnosed according to McDonald 2010 criteria30. Males and females aged 18 or older with Relapsing Remitting MS or Clinically Isolated Syndrome, clinically stable, in other words no exacerbations (defined as an episode of neurologic dysfunction lasting at least 24 hours) within four weeks of enrollment, on interferon beta with no gadolinium enhancing lesions on MRI and did not receive any corticosteroid therapy within four weeks prior to recruitment were enrolled. MRI of brain and spinal cord with and without gadolinium were obtained as part of their usual care. All patients using drugs associated with hypercalcemia or treated with disease modifying therapies other than IFN-b within six weeks prior to enrolment, pregnant and with history of primary hyper PTH, hypercalcemia, renal dysfunction, cardiac disease, malignancy, granulomatous disease, dementia, traumatic brain injury, diagnosis of epilepsy or history of seizure, psychiatric disease, substance abuse/dependence, alcohol abuse/dependence were excluded from the study. Patients with 25 (OH)D serum level between 25 and 35 ng/ml were also excluded.

Subjects were recruited and assigned to sufficient (25 (OH)D >35 ng/ml) and deficient (<25 ng/ml) 25 (OH)D groups. Subjects with deficient 25 (OH)D levels were given a high dose vitamin D3 supplementation (10,000 IU daily for 3 months). Subjects with sufficient (25 (OH)D >35 ng/ml) continued their usual treatment that may include vitamin D3 supplementation at various doses. Cognitive tests were performed at baseline and at 3 months.

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

This study was registered in 19/09/2013 at clinical Trials.gov with the identification number NCT01952483. The study protocol is available in the supplementary information. The American University of Beirut Institutional Review Board approved this study, protocol number IM.SK1.04. All methods were performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of the American University of Beirut Institutional Research Board and all participants signed an informed consent.

Screening and other assessment tools

Hopkins Symptoms Checklist (HSCL-25) was administered at baseline to screen all participants for any depressive or anxiety symptoms, which may be a confounding factor on cognitive function. This is a 25-item tool that has been translated into Arabic and is widely used due to its brevity, reliability, and validity18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31.

Brief Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS)

Excerpts from BRFSS32 were filled out to collect demographic data such as age, years of education, and lifestyle factors known to affect cognitive function such as smoking, exercise, alcohol intake and recreational habits. All medications (including anti-depressants or anxiolytics) were recorded at each visit.

Food Diaries

To control for additional vitamin D intake from food sources, all subjects were handed a diary to document their daily ingestion of food known to be rich in vitamin D such as salmon, egg yolk and to document their body parts exposure to sun as well. A research assistant contacted each participant weekly to collect information on their food intake, ensure compliance with their supplementation and that they are filling their diaries which were collected at the end of the three months period (Fig. 1).

Cognitive assessment

A cognitive test battery measuring different fields was administered. The test took around 45 minutes to complete (Table 5).

Table 5. Cognitive fields measured by the cognitive tests battery.
Cognitive field MoCA Stroop SDMT BVMT-R
Verbal Memory X      
Learning X     X
Working Memory X   X  
Interference        
Mental Flexibility   X    
Attention: Selective and Divided X X X  
Information Processing Speed   X X  
Visual Tracking X   X X
VisuoSpatial X     X
Short Term Memory X     X
Long Term Memory X     X
Recognition X     X
Language X      
Abstraction X      
Orientation X      

MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory test.

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA-10 minutes) is a screening tool for cognitive impairment in adults. The MoCA-Arabic version has demonstrated 92.3% sensitivity and 85.7% specificity for detecting mild cognitive impairment in older adults in Egypt33. We have used this test in a sample of healthy adults and older adults in Lebanon and the test showed high internal consistency (α = 0.83)15.

The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT-5 minutes) is a known and widely used speed of processing test. Oral SDMT was administered; it does not require translation and has been used in Lebanon before15.

Stroop test (Arabic-5 minutes) is a highly established test of attention and was administered based on an earlier description34. Subject is given a list of colors in black ink to read (Stroop 1), then a list of colors in their corresponding ink color (Stroop 2), and then finally a list of colors with incongruent ink colors (Stroop 3), the participant is expected to read the word without being affected by interfering mismatching color. The number of words read in a minute (minus the errors) was the dependent measure and interference was calculated using the following equation: Interference = Stroop 3 − [(Stroop 1 + Stroop 2)/2].

Brief Visuospatial Memory test (BVMT-R- 25 minutes) is a visuospatial memory test that is comprised of three memory trials (10 seconds each) followed by delayed recall after 25 minutes and a recognition trial. It has been widely used as a quick measure of memory at bedside and in MS patients35.

Serum 25 (OH)D testing

Serum 25 (OH)D levels were measured in the Endocrine Core laboratory at AUBMC using Roche Diagnostics total assay. This assay has a between day precision of CV = 9.4% and 1.9% at mean concentrations of 43.3 and 105 nmol/L using controls as provided by Roche Diagnostics as well as a mean percent bias of less than 5% from all laboratory trimmed mean when compared to IDS RIA or IDS iSYS assays36.

Statistical analysis

Predictor variables

25 (OH)D status (sufficient Y/N), –25 (OH)D, age, years of education, physical activity, sun exposure, dietary vitamin D intake, smoking, leisure activity and alcohol consumption –were analyzed using descriptive measures. Independent sample and paired t-tests (before and after and between groups) and Pearson’s r correlations were performed between predictor and outcome variables, MoCA, Stroop, BVMT-Trials 1 to 3 and total score, BVMT-DR and SDMT. A stepwise linear regression analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between predictor and outcome variables. Only complete cases were used in the analysis.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Darwish, H. et al. Effect of Vitamin D Replacement on Cognition in Multiple Sclerosis Patients. Sci. Rep. 7, 45926; doi: 10.1038/srep45926 (2017).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Information
srep45926-s1.pdf (36.7KB, pdf)

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Lebanese National Council for Scientific Research and the American University of Beirut Medical Practice Plan. We thank Ms. Natali Farran from the American University of Beirut for assistance with the manuscript preparation.

Footnotes

We disclose that S.K. and H.D. have received research grants from Novartis. H.D. furthermore received honoraria for lectures from Novartis, travel awards from Novartis, Merck and Genzyme, and Research grants from MENACTRIMS. B.Y. received honoraria for lectures from Bayer, Genzyme, Merck-Serono, Novartis, Genpharm, Biogen, research grants from Bayer, Merck-Serono, Novartis, Biogen, Pfizer and he serves on the Advisory board with Honoraria from Bayer, Novartis, Merck-Serono, Biogen, Genzyme, Genpharm.

Author Contributions H.D. contributed to conceptualization, design, data analysis and interpretation, drafting and revising of the manuscript for intellectual content. R.H. contributed to data entry and drafting of the manuscript. S.O. and S.G. contributed to data collection. B.Y. contributed to subject recruitment and revising of the manuscript. H.T. contributed to the data cleaning and statistical analysis. S.K. contributed to conceptualization, design, data interpretation and revising of the manuscript for intellectual content.

References

  1. Ascherio A. & Munger K. L. Environmental risk factors for multiple sclerosis. Part II: Noninfectious factors. Ann Neurol 61, 504–513 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ascherio A. & Marrie R. A. Vitamin D in MS: a vitamin for 4 seasons. Neurology 79, 208–210 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ascherio A. et al. Vitamin D as an early predictor of multiple sclerosis activity and progression. JAMA Neurol 71, 306–314 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Fitzgerald K. C. et al. Association of Vitamin D Levels With Multiple Sclerosis Activity and Progression in Patients Receiving Interferon Beta-1b. JAMA neurology 72, 1458–1465 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Joshi S. et al. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D(3) ameliorates Th17 autoimmunity via transcriptional modulation of interleukin-17A. Mol Cell Biol 31, 3653–3669 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Smolders J., Moen S. M., Damoiseaux J., Huitinga I. & Holmoy T. Vitamin D in the healthy and inflamed central nervous system: access and function. J Neurol Sci 311, 37–43 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Smolders J., Muris A. H. & Damoiseaux J. Immunomodulation by vitamin D in multiple sclerosis: More than IL-17. J Neuroimmunol 292, 79–80 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Correale J., Ysrraelit M. C. & Gaitan M. I. Immunomodulatory effects of Vitamin D in multiple sclerosis. Brain: a journal of neurology 132, 1146–1160 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Mouhieddine T. H. et al. Risk factors for multiple sclerosis and associations with anti-EBV antibody titers. Clin Immunol 158, 59–66 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Amato M. P. et al. Treatment of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: position paper. J Neurol 260, 1452–1468 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Amato M. P. et al. Cognitive reserve and cortical atrophy in multiple sclerosis: a longitudinal study. Neurology 80, 1728–1733 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Amato M. P., Zipoli V. & Portaccio E. Cognitive changes in multiple sclerosis. Expert Rev Neurother 8, 1585–1596 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Llewellyn D. J., Lang I. A., Langa K. M. & Melzer D. Vitamin D and cognitive impairment in the elderly US population. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 66, 59–65 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Llewellyn D. J., Langa K. M. & Lang I. A. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D concentration and cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 22, 188–195 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Darwish H. et al. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D predicts cognitive performance in adults. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 11, 2217–2223 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eyles D. W., Liu P. Y., Josh P. & Cui X. Intracellular distribution of the vitamin D receptor in the brain: comparison with classic target tissues and redistribution with development. Neuroscience 268, 1–9 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eyles D. W., Smith S., Kinobe R., Hewison M. & McGrath J. J. Distribution of the vitamin D receptor and 1 alpha-hydroxylase in human brain. J Chem Neuroanat 29, 21–30 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Balion C. et al. Vitamin D, cognition, and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 79, 1397–1405 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Annweiler C. et al. ‘Vitamin D and cognition in older adults’: updated international recommendations. J Intern Med 277, 45–57 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Schwid S. R., Goodman A. D., Weinstein A., McDermott M. P. & Johnson K. P. Cognitive function in relapsing multiple sclerosis: minimal changes in a 10-year clinical trial. J Neurol Sci 255, 57–63 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bergendal G., Fredrikson S. & Almkvist O. Selective decline in information processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Neurol 57, 193–202 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Piras M. R. et al. Longitudinal study of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: neuropsychological, neuroradiological, and neurophysiological findings. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 74, 878–885 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Tam J. W. & Schmitter-Edgecombe M. The Role of Processing Speed in the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 27, 962–972 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kalueff A. V., Lou Y. R., Laaksi I. & Tuohimaa P. Increased anxiety in mice lacking vitamin D receptor gene. Neuroreport 15, 1271–1274 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Armstrong D. J. et al. Vitamin D deficiency is associated with anxiety and depression in fibromyalgia. Clin Rheumatol 26, 551–554 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ballard C., Rowan E., Stephens S., Kalaria R. & Kenny R. A. Prospective follow-up study between 3 and 15 months after stroke: improvements and decline in cognitive function among dementia-free stroke survivors >75 years of age. Stroke 34, 2440–2444 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Krupp L. B. et al. Donepezil improved memory in multiple sclerosis in a randomized clinical trial. Neurology 63, 1579–1585 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Krupp L. B. et al. Multicenter randomized clinical trial of donepezil for memory impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 76, 1500–1507 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. O’Brien A. R., Chiaravalloti N., Goverover Y. & Deluca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: a review of the literature. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 89, 761–769 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Polman C. H. et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Annals of neurology 69, 292–302 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mahfoud Z. et al. The Arabic validation of Hopkins Symptoms Checklist-25 against MINI in a disadvantaged suburb of Beirut, Lebanon. The International Journal of Educational and Psychological Assessment (TIJEPA) 13, 17–33 (2013). [Google Scholar]
  32. CDC. Behavioral risk factor surveillance system survey questionnaire. Atlanta, Georgia: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2009).
  33. Rahman T. T. & El Gaafary M. M. Montreal Cognitive Assessment Arabic version: reliability and validity prevalence of mild cognitive impairment among elderly attending geriatric clubs in Cairo. Geriatr Gerontol Int 9, 54–61 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Al-Ghatani A. M., Obonsawin M. C., Binshaig B. A. & Al-Moutaery K. R. Saudi normative data for the Wisconsin Card Sorting test, Stroop test, Test of Non-verbal Intelligence-3, Picture Completion and Vocabulary (subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised). Neurosciences (Riyadh) 16, 29–41 (2011). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Niccolai C. et al. A comparison of the brief international cognitive assessment for multiple sclerosis and the brief repeatable battery in multiple sclerosis patients. BMC Neurol 15, 204 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Abdel-Wareth L. et al. Total vitamin D assay comparison of the Roche Diagnostics “Vitamin D total” electrochemiluminescence protein binding assay with the Chromsystems HPLC method in a population with both D2 and D3 forms of vitamin D. Nutrients 5, 971–980 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information
srep45926-s1.pdf (36.7KB, pdf)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES